ebook img

Postcopulatory guarding behaviour in a territorial damselfly, Pseudagrion p. pilidorsum (Brauer), for submerged ovipositing females (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae) PDF

2005·1.5 MB·English
by  MatsubaraK
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Postcopulatory guarding behaviour in a territorial damselfly, Pseudagrion p. pilidorsum (Brauer), for submerged ovipositing females (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae)

Odonatologica34(4):387-396 December I,2005 Postcopulatory guardingbehaviour ina territorial damselfly, Pseudagrionp. pilidorsum(Brauer), for submergedovipositing females (Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae) K. Matsubara¹* andNM.Hironaka² 1DepartmentofAppliedBiologicalSciences,Faculty ofAgriculture,SagaUniversity, Honjo1,Saga,840-8502,Japan 2 DepartmentofBiology,Faculty ofMedicine,HamamatsuUniversitySchool ofMedicine, Handayama1-20-1,Hamamatsu,Shizuoka,431-3192,Japan ReceivedSeptember29, 2004/RevisedandAcceptedApril22, 2005 Thepostcopulatorymateguardingbehavioursbyterritorial andnon-territorial 33 forsubmergedovipositing $9wereinvestigatedinthefield.Aftercopulations,9 9in tandem begantoovipositatthewatersurfaceandthereaftertheyusuallysubmerged completelyunderwater.The 9 oftenrepeatedthesubmergenceandemergenceatsev- eraloviposition sites.Whenthe 9submergedcompletely,the 3 released herwithout submergenceandrested abovethewatersurface duringoviposition(non-submerged guarding), orthe 3 submergedcompletely and remained intandem,whether only atfirstorforthe durationoftheoviposition(submergedguarding).Territorial 33 alwaysperformednon-submergedguardingwhen the 9ovipositedinsidetheir ter- ritories. Thenon-submergedguardinginside theterritorymightallow theterritorial 3bothtoguardtheovipositing 9 andtomaintain histerritory.On theother hand, when the 9 ovipositedinside another 3’s territories, territorial and non-territorial 33 exhibited bothnon-submergedguardingand submergedguarding.Thus, P.p. pilidorsum66 mayadopteither submergedguardingornon-submergedguarding inresponse tochangeintheprobabilityofatakeoveroftheemerged 9byrival 33 insideanother 3’s territory. INTRODUCTION Inmany insects, the maleprolongs its relationship with its mate beyond the timeit takesto transfera sufficientnumberof gametesto fertilizeallofthefe- *Correspondingauthor:Tel&Fax: +81-952-28-8792;- e-mail:[email protected] 388 K. Matsubara &M.Hironaka male’seggs(ALCOCK, 1994).Mostprolongedassociationswithmates havede- veloped to assure paternity underthe conditionthatthelastmaleto inseminate a femaleis morelikely than previous malesto fertilizetheeggs. Postcopulatory mate guarding, seen inmany species of dragonflies and damselflies, is one of themostfamiliarexamplesofhowamalepreventsothermalesfromcopulating with his partner(WAAGE, 1984;ALCOCK, 1994;CORBET, 1999). In these species, postcopulatory mate guarding is aparticularly effectivemeans, because spermremovalordisplacement gives thelast malea fertilization advantage (see WAAGE, 1984). Dragonflies and damselflies exhibit two manners of postcopulatory mate guarding: contact guarding (tandemoviposition), inwhichthemalestays physi- cally attachedto thefemale(e.g. B1CK&BICK, 1963),andnon-contact guard- ing. inwhichthemaleremainsnearthefemaleandchasesawayothermales(e.g. WAAGE, 1973). Although contact guarding offersbettersecurity against take- over ofhismate(SAKAGAMletah, 1974;WAAGE, 1984),themalemaybeun- abletomatewithotherfemalesas longas heisattachedtotheovipositing female (ALCOCK, 1979;UEDA,1979;WAAGE, 1984).On theotherhand,non-contact guardingallowsthemaleto acquirenewmateswhileguardingtheovipositing fe- maleandto simultaneously defendhisterritory(ALCOCK, 1979;UEDA, 1979; SHERMAN,1983;WAAGE, 1984),buttherisk thatthefemalewillre-mate may begreater(UEDA, 1979;ALCOCK, 1982;CONVEY, 1989). Submerged oviposition byfemales is foundonly inthe Zygoptera (WAAGE, 1984).Whenafemalecommencessubmerged oviposition, herpartnerguards her by hovering orperching abovethewatersurface (e.g. Hetaerina vulnerata, AL- COCK, 1982; Enallagma hageni, FINCKE, 1982).On the otherhand, m some genera(e.g. Paracercion, ARAI, 1977;Erythromma, WINSLAND, 1983;Lestes, ANDO,1969;Pseudagrion FURTADO, 1972,MESKIN, 1986, 1989;andXan- , thocnemis,CRUMPTON, 1975),theguardingmalesubmergescompletely andin tandemwiththefemale.However,theadaptive significance ofsubmerged guard- ing by males hasnot beenfullyanalyzed. InP.p.pilidorsum, themale formsaterritorynearoviposition sites, andthefe- malefrequently performs submerged oviposition(AZUMA, 1987;ISHIDA etah, 1988).Inordertoelucidatetheadaptive significance ofsubmerged guarding, we investigated postcopulatory guarding behavioursby territorialandnon-territorial malesforsubmerged ovipositing females Wediscuss themating tacticsofterrito- rialandnon-territorialmalesassociated withpostcopulatory mate guarding. METHODS STUDYSITE, —The studywascarriedoutattheupperpartofastreamontheSenbaru campus oftheUniversityoftheRyukyusinNishihara,thesouthernpart ofOkinawaIsland,Japan(26°14'N, 127°45'E).Atthe studysite,theriverwasabout3rawide and 0,5m deep,andaboveitsbanks stood agrove ofleucaena Leucaenaleucocephalaand anevergreenforest.Damselflies rarelyappearedat PostcopulatoryguardinginPseudagrionpilidorsum 389 thestudysiteintheearlymorningorevening,when thewatersurface wasshaded. Thereproductive behaviourwas observedata 15m lengthoftheriver inthe studysite. Intheobservation area,there weretwosandysitesontheriverbank,one3mlongand theother 5m.Thesesiteswerecoveredwith grasses,andsomeofthestemsandrootswereunderwater.Female damselfliesusedtheseunderwater stemsand rootsasovipositionsubstrates, OBSERVATIONOFREPRODUCTIVEBEHAVIOUR. - Althoughdamselfliesappearedsuc- cessivelyatthesitefromearlyApriltolateNovember,theobservationwasconductedintwoperiods, JulyandSeptember1996,whenmanydamselfliesappeared.Intwodaysbeforeeachobservationpe- riod,wecollected asmanymaleandfemaledamselflies aspossibleatthe studysite andsurrounding area, andindividuallymarked them with paintmarkers (MitsubishiPaint MarkerPX-21®)onthe thorax andabdomen.Furthermore,ifanyunmarked individuals werefoundduringobservationpe- riod,theyweremarkedandreleased. The behaviourofdamselflies didnotappeartobe affectedby markingthemonthe thoraxandabdomen. Wecarriedoutcontinuous observation principallyfrom 0900to 1500h,exceptonrainyorovercast days(observationtime: 114hin July,69 hinSeptember). Weexamined thetimeandlocation inwhichreproductivebehaviours byindividual damselflies were observed.The time andduration ofthese behaviours weremeasured with theuseofastopwatch. Intotal,149malesand46females(July: 77malesand31 females;September:72 malesand15fe- males)wereindividuallydistinguishedbymarking. TERRITORIALBEHAVIOUR Territorieswere usuallyestablishedaroundoviposition sitesthatcontainedtwo orthreelocationsofoviposition substrates, elongated stemsandroots ofgrasses underwater, andeach territoryalways containedatleastone perch onstems or leavesofgrassesprotruding 10-15cmabove thewater surface.Territorialmales defendedagainst intruding conspecific males by the chase display and/or the elaborateconfrontation-chasedisplay(FURTADO, 1972;MESKIN,1986,1989, 1993).Territorial malesalmost always succeededin defending theirterritories; intrudersousted defending malesfromtheirownterritoryonly 3times(1.4%) in 220observedinteractions.Territorysize, as definedby thedistanceatwhichthe ownerreactedto anapproaching male,was quite variable,ranging from 1-4m. Territorialmaleswere residentat thesameoviposition sitefrom 1-8 days(mean ± SE =2.33 ± 0.43, n=21). Thesocial statusofmalessometimeschanged from territorialto non-territorialorvice versa. MATINGBEHAVIOUR Neitherterritorialnor non-territorialmalescourtedfemales.Whena territo- rialmalediscoveredafemaleapproaching anoviposition siteinsidehisterritory, heimmediately grasped thefemalewithhisanalappendages. Thenon-territorial male, ontheotherhand, usually stayed aroundanothermale’sterritory untila femaleapproached anoviposition site, whereupon it quickly seizedthe female. Aftera territorialor non-territorialmale had seizeda female, the couple flew immediately intandemto a nearby plant and perched onit.Themale simulta- neously raisedandcurved hisabdomento bringthe primary gonoporein con- 390 K.Matsubara&M.Hironaka tact withthe secondary genitalia, presumably translocating sperm. Copulation ensuedimmediately. Themean(±SE) copulation durationwas 17.42± 1.75min (range =4.08-50.30,N =29). OVIPOSITION BEHAVIOUR BY FEMALESAND POSTCOPULATORY BEHAVIOURS BYMALES Inthe 85(91.4%) of93tandempairs thatappeared atthe observationsite, the femaleperformed submerged oviposition. In theother 8 tandempairs (8.6%), the femaleoviposited onthewatersurfaceandthenflewaway fromtheobserva- tionsite.Thus,whentandempairsappeared attheobservationsite, mostfemales performed submerged oviposition atleastonce. Figure 1 is a flow chart of the successive postcopulatory behaviours. After copulation, the maleand femalein tandemflewto anoviposition site nearthe water’s edge. Thefemalelandedonthe siteandbent herabdomen, andherovi- positor contacted the oviposition substrates.The maleresponded by adopting an“upright tandemposture”in whichhisbody stoodvertically onthetipofhis abdomen(his analappendages remaining interlockedwiththe female’sthorax), andflapped andvibratedhis wings. Oviposition began at thewater surfaceand usuallywas completed underwater.Insurfaceoviposition, onlythefemale’sovi- positor was immersed, whilethe male maintainedthe upright tandemposture. Whenthe femalesubmerged by movingbackwards along thesite in tandem(n = 107), the maleoften released the femalewithoutsubmerging with her(non- submerged guarding, n =74), or the maleremainedin tandemandsubmerged eitherforashortmoment atfirst(n= 10)orforthedurationofthefemale’ssub- mergence(n =23) (submerged guarding, n =33). Malesthatsubmerged only at firstreturnedto thesurfacetorest abovetheoviposition site.Anyindividualwas abletoperform bothsubmerged andnonsubmerged postcopulatory guardingfor submerged ovipositing hispartner. Afterthemalereleasedthe submerged ovipositing femaleandperched onveg- etationabovethefemale, heusuallyrecaptured thefemaleoncesheemerged, and thetandempair began searching foranotheroviposition site.Whenthemalere- mainedsubmerged withthe femalethroughout oviposition, theirsearch foran- othersiteresumedpromptly uponemergence. Thefemaleoftensubmerged and emerged atseveraloviposition sites(theaveragenumberofovipositions (n,mean ± SE.): n= 17,2.2±0.4, range = 1-7). Themean(±SE) durationofsubmerged oviposition by femalesandthemean (±SE) durationofsubmerged guarding bymaleswhen themaleadapted either thesubmerged guardingor thenon-submerged guardingare showninTableI. Afemalewithanonsubmerged malehadasimilaroviposition timeas thatof afemalewithanat-firstsubmerged maleor withafullysubmerged male(signifi- cantpost hoc Mann-Whitney Utest(Padjusted to0.0167 toreducethechance PostcopulatoryguardinginPseudagrionpilidorsum 391 of committing a type Ierror), vs. a femalewith anat-first submerged male, z = -1.400, P = 0.1615; vs. a femalewitha fullysubmerged male, z = -1.847, P = 0.0647). Thedurationofsubmerged oviposition by a femalewas shorterwhen the maleremainedsubmerged withherthroughout oviposition than when the male was submerged only at first (significant post hoc Mann-Whitney U test (P adjusted to0.0167 toreducethechance ofcommitting a typeI error),z = - 2.663, P =0.0077). Thedurationofsubmerged oviposition byfemaleswas sig- nificantly longer thanthatof submerged guarding by males(Wilcoxon signed ranks, afemalewithanat-firstsubmerged male,n=7, z =-2.366, P =0.0180; a femalewith afullysubmerged male, n= 17, z = -3.621, P - 0.0003). Thedura- tionofsubmergence by males didnotdifferbetweenmalesthatsubmerged only Fig. 1.The flowchart ofthesuccessivepostcopulatorybehaviours inP.p.pilidorsum. — [Aquadri- lateralshows aparticularevent.Figures inparenthesisindicatesamplesize] 392 K. Matsubara& M.Hironaka atfirstandthosethatremainedsubmerged throughout (Mann-Whitney Utest, z = -0.896,P = 0.3704). Tandempairswereofteninterferedbyalonemales. Theinterferencefrequently resultedin anoticeablechange offlightor oviposition behavioursby thepair. TERRITORIALITYAND POSTCOPULATORY MATE GUARDING BEHAVIOURS Aftercopulating withaterritorialmale, thefemaleoviposited underwaterin- sidehisterritoryor inanothermale’sterritory. Figure 2 shows the proportions of submerged guarding and non-submerged guarding by territorialmaleswhenfemalesoviposited insidetheirterritoriesor anothermale’s territory,andthoseproportions by non-territorialmaleswhenfe- malesoviposited insideanothermale’sterritory. When thefemaleperformedsubmerged oviposition insideherpartner’s terri- tory, the territorialmalealwaysreleasedthefemaleandrestedonthewater sur- faceduringoviposition. On theotherhand,whenthefemaleoviposited underwa- terinsideanothermale’s territory,themaleowning theterritoryexhibitedeither submerged guarding ornon-submerged guarding; theproportion ofsubmerged guarding was higher than thatof non-submerged guarding (Fisher’s exacttest, P =0.0021, Fig. 2a). Of 12 territorialmales performing non-submerged guarding forhis partner inside their territories, only one maleacquired an additional femalethat had emerged from underwater, by eliminating the non-territorialmaleperforming non-submerged guardingforhispartner. Non-territorialmalesalso performed both submerged and nonsubmerged postcopulatory guardingwhen thefemaleoviposited underwaterinsideanother male’s territory, andthe proportion ofsubmerged guardingwas similarto that of nonsubmerged guarding (chi-square goodness-of-fit test, x2= 3.13, df = 1, P>0.05, Fig. 2b). TableI The mean(± SE)duration(min)ofsubmergedovipositionbyfemalesandthemean(+SE)duration (min)ofsubmergedguardingbymales whenthemaleadaptedeitherthesubmergedguardingorthe non-submergedguarding[Figuresinparenthesisindicatesamplesize] Duration ofsubmerged Duration ofsubmerged ovipositionbyfemales guardingbymales Non-submergedguarding 21.2313.24(42) Submergedguarding Afemalewith anatfirstsubmergedmale 33.2518.25(7) 8.4212.08 (9) Afemalewith afullysubmergedmale 8.5811.35(18) 6.1411.09(20) PostcopulatoryguardinginPseudagrionpilidorsum 393 DISCUSSION FemalesP.p.pilidorsumbeganoviposition onthesurfacebutusually completed it underwater. Submerged oviposition hasbeenfoundin P.perfuscatum, P. mi- crocephalum(FURTADO, 1972),P. hagenitropicanum (MESKIN, 1986),P. cit- ricola, P.i. inconspicuum andP.salisburyense (MESKIN, 1989). Thus, submerged oviposition mightbe ubiquitous inthegenusPseudagrion. Whenthefemalesubmerged deeply, themaleeitherreleasedthefemalewithout submerging orthemalesubmerged completelyandremainedintandem,whether onlyatfirstorforthedurationoftheoviposition. Similarpostcopulatory behav- ioursbymaleshavebeenfoundinP.perfuscatum andP. microcephalum. FURTA- DO(1972) reported anexample inwhichamaleofP. microcephalum submerged withanovipositing femaleintandembutthenseparatedaftertwominutes,float- ing to thesurfacetorest onthenearest perch. FURTADO (1972) reported two otherexamples inmalesofP.perfuscatum: inone, themale submerged withthe ovipositing femalefor 15minutes,andin theother, the malehoveredabove the ovipositing femalewithoutsubmerged guarding. InthegenusPseudagrion, there- fore,thepostcopulatory behavioursbymalesforsubmerged ovipositing females maybeflexible, depending onhowlong thefemalesare submerged. Submerged oviposition sometimes lasts more than an hourand may incur considerableenergycostsand predation risks for the female(WAAGE, 1984). Furthermore, FINCKE (1988) and MILLER (1990) consid- ered that submerged oviposit- ing females suffer costssuch as waterloggingandimpaired vigor upon surfacing. Therefore, it is likely that submerged guarding has the same effects on males, and the cost may be far larg- er than that of non-submerged guarding. Flowever, submerged guardingmaybemoreadvanta- geousto themalethannon-sub- mergedguarding,sincethereisa higherrisk thatothermaleswill takeover theemergentfemaleif the guarding male did not sub- Fig. 2,Theproportionsofsubmergedguardingandnon- -submergedguardingbyterritorial males when females merge. ovipositedinsidetheirterritoriesoranothermale’sterri- Whena femaleoviposited in- tory(a), and thoseproportionsby non-territorial males side her partner’s territory, the when females oviposited inside anothermale’s territory maleowing thatterritoryalways (b). — [Figuresincolumnsindicatesamplesize] 394 K. Matsubara&M.Hironaka releasedthefemalewithoutsubmergence andrestedabovethewater surfacedur- ing oviposition (non-submerged guarding, Fig. 2a). Two hypotheses have been proposed toexplain theadaptive significance ofnon-contact guardingbytheter- ritorialmale insidehisterritory (ALCOCK, 1979,UEDA, 1979,SHERMAN, 1983). ALCOCK(1979) andUEDA(1979) suggested thatnon-contact guarding is reproductively advantageous because it allowsamaleboth to guard the ovi- positing femaleand to acquire newmates(multiple mating hypothesis). On the otherhand,SHERMAN(1983)andWAAGE(1984) suggested thatnon-contact guarding isreproductively important becauseitallowsamalebothto guard the ovipositing femaleandto maintainhisterritory (territorymaintenancehypoth- esis).Of 12territorialmalesperforming non-submerged guarding forhispartner insidetheirterritories,only one malecould acquire anadditionalfemale.From thisresult, we could not necessarily discard themultiple mating hypothesis, be- causeofshortageofthedata.SHERMAN(1983) posited thatamaleintandem wouldhavedifficultyfightingoff intrudersandthattandemmateguardingcould reduceamale’sability tomaintainhisterritory.Inthisstudy, although theterrito- rialmalein tandemdidnot eliminateintruders fromhis territory,the territorial malewhenalone prevailed inmostmale-maleinteractions. Therefore,non-sub- merged guardingallowsterritorialmalesbothto guardtheovipositingfemaleand tomaintainhisterritory,supporting theterritorymaintenancehypothesis. Whenafemaleoviposited underwaterinsideanothermale’sterritory,bothsub- merged guardingandnon-submerged guardingbytheterritorialmalewerefound, andtheproportionofsubmerged guardingwas higherthanthatofnon-submerged guarding (Fig. 2a). Non-territorialmalesalsoexhibitedbothtypes of guarding whenthefemaleoviposited underwaterinsideanothermale’sterritory(Fig. 2b). UEDA (1979)showedthatSympetrumparvulummalescouldadoptbothcontact guardingandnon-contact guarding, changing fromoneto theotherinresponse to changes intheprobability ofatakeover. InP.p. pilidorsum, malesalso adopt eithersubmerged or non-submerged guardingforthe samereason. Theterritorialmaleoftenlefthisterritorytoaccompany amate to anovipo- sitionsiteelsewhere.Sinceamaledidnot always detectthepresenceofafemale ovipositing in his territory, theprobability thathe wouldtakeover a femaleovi- positingwithanintruding malemightbelow.Therefore, amalemightnotalways engageinsubmerged guardingevenwhenthey are inanothermale’sterritory. CONVEY (1989) reported thatSympetrumsanguineum malesdecidedwhether ornot to switch fromcontact to non-contact guardingbasedontheirassessment ofmaleharassmentearlyinanoviposition bout.Suchharassmentby othermales may affectthe male’schoicebetween submerged andnon-submerged guarding in P.p.pilidorsum, but this remainstobeelucidated. Whenamalesubmerged intandemforthedurationofoviposition, thefemale completed heroviposition fasterthanwhenthemaleremainedsubmerged only at first. The maleremainedunderwaterthe same amount of timewhether his Postcopulatory guardingin Pseudagrionpilidorsum 395 submergence was only atfirst or fortheduration(Tab. I).Therefore, malesmay ceasesubmerging whenthefemale’ssubmergence isprolonged, partiallybecause themalecannot stay underwateras long as thefemale. Submerged guarding mayhaveevolvedincloseassociationwiththemannerof oviposition, inwhichthefemalefrequently repeatssubmergence andemergence, laying eggs atseveral sites. Ifafemaleoviposits almostallof hermature eggs at asingle site, she maythenbetakenover by anothermaleatno cost to herpart- ner, sinceshewillnot produce mature eggs again forseveral days.On theother hand, if afemaleoviposits eggs at several sites byrepeated submerged oviposi- tioning, such as Hetaerinavulneratafemalesdo(ALCOCK, 1982),herpartner’s submerged guarding maysubstantially benefithimbypreventing anothermale from taking her. Therefore, submerged guarding by P. p. pilidorsum males may haveevolved by thefemales’frequent, repeated submergence to lay eggs at dif- ferent sites untilalmostallofher eggs are gone. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT WethankProf.N.SUZUKI, DepartmentofAppliedBiologicalSciences,FacultyofAgriculture, SagaUniversity, forvaluable suggestionsandcriticallyreadingthemanuscript. REFERENCES ALCOCK, J., 1979,MultiplematinginCalopteryxmaculata(Odonata:Calopterygidae)andthead- vantageofnon-contactguardingbymales. J.not.Hist. 13:439-446. ALCOCK,X, 1982.Post-copulatorymateguardingbymales ofthedamselflyHetaerina vulnerata Selys(Odonata:Calopterygidae).Anim. Behav. 30:99-107. ALCOCK,J., 1994.Postinsemination associations between males andfemales in insects:the mate- -guardinghypothesis.Amu.Rev. Ent. 39: 1-21. ANDO,H,, 1969. SubmergedovipositionofLestessponsa. Tombo12:27-28. ARAI,Y., 1977. Behaviour ofmatureindividuals ofCercion sieboldii Selys (Agrionidae).Tombo 20: 13-16. AZUMA,S., 1987.Fieldguide-booktotheinsectsofOkinawa,Vol. 4.OkinawaShuppan,Okinawa. BICK,G.H.&J.C.BICK,1963. Behavior andpopulationstructureofthedamselfly,Enallagmaciv- ile(Hagen)(Odonata:Coenagriidae).SIVest.Nat. 8:57-84. CONVEY,P., 1989. Post-copulatoryguardingstrategiesinthenon-territorial dragonflySympetrum sanguineum(Muller)(Odonata:Libellulidae).Anim. Behav. 37: 56-63. CORBET, P.S., 1999. Dragonflies:behavior and ecologyofOdonata. Cornell Univ. Press, Ithaca/ NY. CRUMPTON,W.J., 1975.Adultbehaviour ofXanthocnemis zealandicaMcLachlan andAustrolestes colensonis WhiteatselectedSouth Island(N.Zealand)habitats(Zygoptera:Coenagrionidae, Lestidae). Odonatologica4; 149-168. FINCKE, O.M., 1982. Lifetimematingsuccessinanatural populationofthedamselflyEnallagma hageni(Walsh)(Odonata:Coenagrionidae).Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 10:293-302. FINCKE, O.M., 1988.Sourcesofvariation inlifetimereproductivesuccessinanonterritorial dam- selfly (Odonata:Coenagrionidae).In:T.H. Clutton-Brock,[Ed.],Reproductive success, pp. 24-43. Univ. ChicagoPress, Chicago. 396 K. Matsubara&M.Hironaka FURTADO,XL,1972. ThereproductivebehaviourofIschnura senegalensis(Rambur),Pseudagrion microcephalum(Rambur)andP.perfuscatumLieftinck (Odonata,Coenagrionidae).Malay. J.Sci.(A) 1:57-69. ISHIDA,S„K.ISHIDA,K.KOJIMA&M.SUGIMURA, 1988. Illustratedguideforidentification oftheJapaneseOdonata.Tokai Univ. Press,Tokyo. MESKIN,I., 1986.Territorialbehaviour inPseudagrionhagenitropicanumPinhey(Zygoptera:Coen- agrionidae).Odonalologica15: 157-167. MESKIN,I., 1989.AspectsofterritorialbehaviourinthreespeciesofPseudagrionSelys(Zygoptera: Coenagrionidae).Odonalologica18:253-261. MESKIN,I., 1993. Territorial behaviour in Pseudagrionkersteni (Gerstaeker)(Zygoptera: Coen- agrionidae).Odonalologica22:63-70. MILLER,P.L., 1990.Therescueserviceprovidedbymale Enallagmacyathigerum(Charpentier)for females afteroviposition. J. Br.DragonflySoc. 6: 8-14. SAKAGAMI,S.F.,H. UBUKATA,M.IGA&M.TODA, 1974. Observations onthebehavior of someOdonataintheBoninIslands,withconsiderations ontheevolution ofreproductivebe- haviorinLibellulidae. J.Fac Sci.,Hokkaido Univ. (VI) 19:722-757. SHERMAN,K.J., 1983. Theadaptivesignificanceofpostcopulatorymateguardinginadragonfly, Pachydiplaxlongipennis.Anim. Behav. 31: 1107-1115. UEDA,T, 1979.Plasticity ofthereproductivebehaviour inadragonflySympetrumparvulumBar- teneff,with reference tothesocial relationshipofmales anddensityofterritories. Res. Pop. Ecol. 21: 135-152. WAAGE, J.K., 1984. Spermcompetition and the evolution of odonate matingsystems. In. R.L. Smith,[Ed.], Sperm competitionand the evolution ofanimal matingsystems, pp. 251-290. Acad. Press,New York. WAAGE,J.K.,1973.Reproductivebehaviouranditsrelation toterritorialityinCalopteryxmaculata (Beauvois)(Odonata:Calopterygidae).Behaviour 47: 240-256. WINSLAND,D., 1983. Some observations ofErythromma najas(Hansemann). J. Br. Dragonfly Soc. 1:6.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.