ebook img

Physiological Psychology PDF

637 Pages·1980·22.47 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Physiological Psychology

p>tin yS,Wπ ϊ*l O1 g l~ Ca^ l 7 THOMAS S. BBRROO WN ACADEMIC PRESS Λ Λ New York DePaul University San Francisco PATRICIA M. WALLACE °^sidiar of A y psychology Clarion State College HwoJrtBÎace Jovanovich, Publishers Cover illustration on photograph by D. L. Cramer Copyright ©1980, by Academic Press, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Ill Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24/28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX ISBN: 0-12-136660-X Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 79-52344 PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA There is an air of enthusiasm in a field like physiological preface psychology that is very typical of young sciences. The excitement even infects the general public as more and more TV specials and magazine articles appear which deal with topics in this field. We hope we will convey some of this enthusiasm to the students who read this book, some of whom will continue with their training and go on to explore the fascinating relationship between physiology and behavior. Our aim in writing this book was to present an introduction to a field that is rapidly growing and changing. This area of study combines the talents of psychologists, biologists, chem- ists, physicists, engineers, nutritionists, and many others inter- ested in the relationship between biology and behavior. Though a young field, it is accumulating significant information very rapidly, partly because of the development of new techniques and new approaches, and partly because of the growing aware- ness that we cannot understand human behavior without "look- ing inside" the body. This book is intended for an undergraduate course usually called physiological psychology, but which may also go under such names as biological psychology, psychobiol- ogy, brain and behavior, or physiology and behavior. The book's organization reflects the most recent trends in the field of physiological psychology. We begin with basic infor- mation about the nervous system and the neuron, and then discuss the topics about which physiological psychologists know the most: the sensory systems. The next section examines the physiological bases of some of the more "primitive" behaviors, such as hunger and thirst, reproduction, sleep, and emotion. The last section explores the topics of development, and of learning and memory, from the standpoint of plasticity in the nervous system. This approach acknowledges a major change in the way scientists are viewing the nervous system. An approach that we believe will lead to a better understanding of the way the nervous system is able to change in response to internal and external events. The final chapter deals with the higher processes such as thinking, decision making, attention, and language; it describes ground-breaking studies that are actually exploring the physiological bases of human consciousness. In a field as interdisciplinary as this, there will be many students who do not have extensive backgrounds in psychology, biology, or the other sciences that have contributed to knowl- edge in the area. We have written the book with this in mind. And no strong science prerequisites are necessary in order to understand the material. Even so, we recognize that physiolog- ical psychology is not an easy subject, and we have given much thought and effort to designing a book that will provide students with as much help as possible. One simple technique we use is tightly organized chapters. Every chapter is preceded by an outline, and within the chapter are running summaries that review each major section. This technique will not only improve the student's understanding of the material, it will allow instructors to assign parts of chapters without causing confusion. Key terms are typed in bold face, and are listed at the end of each chapter, along with a list of suggested readings. All key terms are defined in an extensive glossary at the end of the book. Another technique we employ to help the student is the use of many human experiments and applications. The effects of drugs on feeding behavior in a rat are certainly fascinating to physiological psychologists, but students usually want to know how these findings might apply to humans. Within each chapter we emphasize the logical progression of experiments which lead to specific conclusions in every subject area, and we spend extra time examining the methods used to arrive at conclusions. It is particularly important for students to understand "how you get there from here," and to be able to evaluate the methods scientists use to test hypotheses. No method, whether it is a lesion, a pharmacological manipulation, an electrical recording, or any other, is free from complications in interpretation. Yet valid conclusions can be drawn based on the weight of the evidence from several kinds of studies. When a question is attacked from several angles, the experimental results often begin to fit a pattern, and the pieces of the puzzle gradually fall into place. TTie knowledge base of physiological psychology has benefited from this multifaceted approach, and we emphasize it throughout the book, explaining the advantages and limitations of each technique. There is no separate chapter on techniques—this material is interwoven into each of the subject areas. We have found that students are far more likely to appreciate methodological issues when they can see how they affect the interpretations of specific experimental results. It may be acceptable to understate the significance and rele- vance of experimental findings among colleagues, but students appreciate an explanation of why a specific experiment was performed, and how the results fit into and support (or refute) a general hypothetical framework. In each chapter, we empha- size the relevance of experimental results and try to show why each set of experiments answers some questions and raises still more. We also frequently point out the relevance of findings in xii preface one chapter to issues presented in another. The division of a book into chapters might lead a student to believe that there are discrete subsystems of behavior, each of which has its own set of physiological underpinnings. We tried to counteract this by cross-referencing between chapters, without sacrificing the in- dependence of each chapter. A unique feature of this book is the inclusion of a brief atlas of the human brain. Our students have frequently been exas- perated by the numerous anatomical terms, and it seemed quite logical to provide them with a detailed map to help them find their way in this novel terrain. A comprehensive study guide accompanies and parallels the book, which contains more features to help the student. The guide recapitulates the important points in each chapter, and includes multiple choice, essay, and matching questions for self- examination. There are anatomical diagrams to provide practice in identifying and labeling those structures mentioned in the text. The study guide also contains useful projects which can be conducted by the student, or by the instructor as class demon- strations. Another feature of the study guide is the section on "Ideas for Research Topics," with suggested sources. It has been our experience that students often have difficulty finding a topic for a research paper, and this section was included to help alleviate that problem. No book is the work of its authors alone, and this one is no exception. Our reviewers, some of whom stayed with us through the writing of the entire manuscript, provided valuable advice, criticism, and suggestions. We would like to thank Paul D. Coleman, University of Rochester Medical Center; Michael Ga- briel, University of Texas at Austin; William T. Greenough, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign; John E. Kelsey, Indiana University; Richard A. King, University of North Car- olina at Chapel Hill; Dale W. McAdam, University of Rochester; and Margaret White, California State University at Fullerton. Our editors, and the people at Academic Press, deserve special thanks. Those on the publishing end of a book rarely receive enough credit, yet their ideas, suggestions, and long, hard hours of work are an important component of the book's quality. We would like to thank Raelene Seelbach and Melissa Preston for assistance with the seemingly endless details of writing, typing, filing, and obtaining copyright permissions. For moral support, we thank Julian Jones and the sorely missed and lovingly remembered Poukah. Last, but far from least, we gratefully acknowledge the contri- preface xiii butions of our students, for their suggestions and advice, their challenging questions, and their sometimes astonishing in- sights. Some of their suggestions are worth quoting: "More graphics, not necessarily pictures, but graphs and diagrams." "Less jargon . . . please." "Write for us, not for other professors who already know it." "Why don't you write a book on human sexuality?" "How could anyone know enough to write an entire text- book?" "Give it some humanity . . . I'm not just a black box filled with nuts and bolts." We took most of these suggestions to heart, and especially tried to "give it some humanity." We hope we have succeeded. Thomas S. Brown Patricia Wallace xii preface INTRODUCTION The Mind-Body Problem Localization of Function Nature versus Nurture 1 The Organization of this Book KEY TERMS SUGGESTED READINGS introduction introduction *n r e c et nyear/s news stories have carried titles such as "Emotional Stress and Sudden Death" (Engel, 1977), "Mind Controllers: CIA Testing" (Getlein, 1977), "Biorhythms: A Key to Your Ups and Downs (Boich, 1977), and "Pain Control with Hypnosis" (Holden, 1977). Reading through these, one feels a combination of emotions, some- times outrage and indignity, sometimes fear, and sometimes a ner- vous optimism about what scientists can accomplish. It is one thing to learn that our moods and actions are related to environmental events. But it is quite another to learn that these same moods and actions may be related to the action of certain chemicals in specific parts of the brain. Can our very thoughts, our most private emotions, be studied by a detached scientist and reduced to the mere action of a few chemi- cals in the brain? Worse, can they be controlled by simply changing brain chemistry or neural activity? The glimmerings of a major assault on the very nature of human beings are on the horizon. This book is about physiological psychology—the physiological ba- ses of behavior. In many ways, the fears and hopes that people are developing about this field are a credit to its enormous growth and success in learning about the brain. In this book, we will discuss what is known about how physiological events relate to behavior, and often how behavior can be changed, even controlled, by modifying the physiological events. But the experiments and findings we are about to explain will have a large impact on the way that human beings per- ceive themselves, and their relationship to the world. So before we begin, we will examine some of these philosophical issues. the mind—body problem Historically, philosophers have hypothesized that the mind and the body are two different elements. Even though the brain is highly com- plex, and certainly has a lot to do with our behavior, it is still part of the body, and thus merely matter. The "mind," the "consciousness," the "I" that thinks, decides, feels, and understands is something non- physical—or a different element. Descartes (1596-1650) was one of the foremost proponents of this dualistic position. While physiologically oriented psychologists were studying simple behaviors like reflexes, this issue remained in the background. Even later, with the advent of behaviorism in the beginning of this century, scientists limited themselves to the study of overt behavior, and the issue still did not present too much of a problem. "Mind" and "con- sciousness" were not studied by scientists because they are not overt behaviors. They are hidden, subjective experiences. During the beha- vioristic era, they were considered not suitable for scientific inquiry. But in recent years, there has been an incredible burgeoning of knowledge about how the brain works, and how its workings are re- lated to our behavior. And very recently, scientists have dared to enter 2 physiological psychology the previously forbidden terrain of the study of "consciousness/' As we learn more about the brain, we find that physiological events are related to emotions like anger, fear, love, and even to decision mak- ing, perception, and intellect. Right now, the knowledge that we have about the physiological correlates of the hidden thoughts and feelings of a human being are very rudimentary. But we have enough so that people are beginning to wonder whether "mind" is indeed something separate. Is it possible that the workings of the brain can account for the subjective experience of "mind?" Can physical events account for "me?" Scientists are very often conservative people, and do not like to take a stand without data to back them up. There are not yet any data avail- able to settle this issue, and some would say that there never will be. Many scientists prefer to think of the mind-body problem as a philo- sophical issue that does not concern scientists. But others have entered the debate. Wilder Penfield (1975), for example, has performed hundreds of neurosurgical operations on epileptic patients, some of which in- cluded electrical stimulation of brain tissue. His observations have led him to the belief that mind and brain are intimately related, but that the workings of the brain cannot, by themselves, explain the mind: When I have caused a conscious patient to move his hand by applying an electrode to the motor cortex of one hemisphere, I have often asked him about it. Invariably his response was, "I didn't do that. You did!" When 1 caused him to vocalize, he said, "1 didn't make that sound. You pulled it out of . me." . . . For my own part, after years of striving to explain the mind on the basis of brain action alone, 1 have come to the conclusion that it is simpler (and far easier to be logical) if one adopts the hypothesis that our being does consist of two fundamental elements. (Penfield, 1975) Other scientists argue that the mind is not a separate element, that the mind can be explained by the workings of the brain. Sagan (1977) states this position quite forcefully: My fundamental premise about the brain is that its workings—what we sometimes call "mind"—are a consequence of its anatomy and physiology, and nothing more. "Mind" may be a consequence of the action of the components of the brain severally or collectively. Some processes may be a function of the brain as a whole. Λ few students of the subject seem to have concluded that, because they have been unable to isolate and localize all higher brain functions, no future generation of neuro anatomists will be able to achieve this objective. But absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. (Sagan, 1977) Steven Rose (1973) also takes this position: In particular, consciousness and mind are seen . . . as being an inevitable consequence of the evolution of particular brain structures which developed in a series of evolutionary changes in the pathway of man's own emergence. The hypothesis is advanced that consciousness is a consequence of the evolution of introduction 3 a particular level of complexity and degree of interaction among the nerve cells (neurons) of the cerebral cortex, while the form it takes is profoundly modified for each individual brain by its development in relationship with the environment. (Rose, 1973) Rose suggests that the mind-body problem may be one caused by semantic difficulties. The study of mind on a mentalistic level (which might on the surface make it seem as though there are two fundamen- tal elements underlying mind and brain) is really simply a different hierarchical analysis of the correlates of human behavior. Neurobi- ology understands human behavior on one level, and psychology and sociology understand it on quite another. The implications of the proposition that mind and brain are one and the same are profound. If it is true, and it becomes widely accepted, human beings will have to change the way that they view themselves. Perhaps even our own values have physiological roots. Wilson (1978) argues this point in his book On Human Nature: Figure 1.1 Innate censors and motivators exist in the brain that deeply and An anatomical drawing by unconsciously affect our ethical premises: from these roots, morality evolved as Albertus Magnus (1206-1280). instinct. If that perception is correct, science may soon be in a position to [From Clarke and Dewhurst investigate the very origin and meaning of human values, from which all (1972).] ethical pronouncements and much of political practice flow. . . . Because the guides of human nature must be examined with a complicated arrangement of mirrors, they are a deceptive subject, always the philosopher's deadfall. The only way forward is to study human nature as part of the natural sciences, in an attempt to integrate the natural sciences with the social sciences and humanities. I can conceive of no ideological or formalistic shortcut. Neurobiology cannot be learned at the feet of a guru. . . . (Wilson, 1978) To many people, these are frightening, even revolutionary ideas. They attack the very core of human philosophy and come into conflict with ideas about religion, free will, and the concept of the human soul. As more and more is learned about the workings of the brain, the question of who and what we are will loom larger. A major rethinking may be needed. localization off unction Another issue which has been disputed throughout the centuries, and which reflects in some ways the mind-body problem, is the issue of localization of function. Two opposing viewpoints hold that (1) con- sciousness and other functions of human beings cannot be localized within particular areas of the brain, and (2) different areas within the brain handle different kinds of behaviors. The concept of localization of function is a very old one, and even during the Middle Ages, people believed that different areas of the brain served different functions. During this time, they concentrated 4 physiological psychology

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.