ebook img

Perspectives in Abnormal Behavior PDF

541 Pages·1974·10.06 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Perspectives in Abnormal Behavior

Perspectives in Abnormal Behavior Edited by RICHARD J. MORRIS Department of Psychology Syracuse University PERGAMON PRESS INC. New York • Toronto • Oxford • Sydney PERGAMON PRESS INC. Maxwell House, Fairview Park, Elmsford, N.Y. 10523 PERGAMON OF CANADA LTD. 207 Queen's Quay West, Toronto 117, Ontario PERGAMON PRESS LTD. Headington Hill Hall, Oxford PERGAMON PRESS (AUST.) PTY. LTD. Rushcutters Bay, Sydney, N.S.W. Copyright © 1974, Pergamon Press Inc. Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Morris, Richard J comp. Perspectives in abnormal behavior. (Pergamon general psychology series, PGPS-37) Includes bibliographies. 1. Psychiatry-Addresses, essays, lectures. I. Title. [DNLM: 1. Psychopathology-Collected works. WM100 M877p 1974] RC458.M63 1974 616.8'9 73-7975 ISBN 0-08-017738-7 ISBN 0-08-017739-5 (pbk.) All Rights Reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form, or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without prior permission of Pergamon Press Inc. Printed in the United States of America To my mother and in memory of my father Preface There have been many changes in the field of abnormal psychology over the past 20 years. Changes in treatment have ranged from a general disenchantment with insight oriented psychotherapy to treatment techniques based on learning theory, and to encounter groups. Changes with respect to when treatment should be initiated have also occurred, giving rise to the community mental health movement with its emphasis on prevention and the handling of everyday "problems in living." In addition, there have been changes in terms of the focus of therapy. Instead of treating the individual in vacuo, many therapists have begun to modify the individual's environment as well as his maladaptive behavior within that environment. Moreover, the recent emphasis in abnormal psychology on the role of social, environ- mental, cultural, genetic, and biochemical factors as contributing variables to the develop- ment and maintenance of maladaptive behavior has led many professionals into disciplines with which they were previously little concerned. Thus, within the last 20 years we find a movement in the field of abnormal psychology away from the strict psychological theoriz- ing of the pre-1950s and toward an interdisciplinary approach to the understanding and treatment of maladaptive behavior. This is not to say that mental health professionals have totally abandoned such psychological theorizing as psychoanalysis to understand their fellow man, but that within the last decade or so they have begun to realize that there are factors other than the individual's psyche that are contributing to the development and maintenance of his maladaptive behavior. Just as Freud's position in the early 1900s was considered to be revolutionary, so must the developments in abnormal psychology since 1950. One cannot deny the research findings that have consistently demonstrated the effects of non-psychological variables as contributors to the development, maintenance, and treatment of maladaptive behavior. Nor can one overlook the importance of the exponentially increasing research that has evolved from the learning theories of Hull and Skinner as well as Pavlov and social-learning theory. This research, with little exception, has shown that behavior can be modified by its consequences. Before 1950, most psychotherapists viewed insight oriented psychotherapy as the method of choice for the treatment of various behavior disorders, the emphasis being on the medical model. However, over the last 20 years, the literature has shown that techni- ques derived from other models are as effective and perhaps more efficient in the treatment of many of the same maladaptive behaviors. There have been changes in abnormal psychology in addition to those concerned with xi xii Preface etiology, treatment, and prevention. Particularly noteworthy over the last decade has been the development among clinicians of a feeling of disillusionment with psychiatric labels and diagnoses—reflecting the growing body of research which has demonstrated the question- able reliability of psychiatric diagnoses. Intimately tied to this development is the concern by most professionals with the stigma attached to those people who have been labeled men- tally ill. This concern is reflected in the number of articles that appeared recently, in the professional literature on the consequences of being labeled mentally ill. It was because of these various changes in the area of abnormal psychology that a decision was made to compile a book of readings which would communicate these changes to students. In order to present this material in a balanced manner, articles were chosen which reflected the major perspectives now subscribed to in abnormal psychology. Two main criteria were used in the selection of material. First, the article had to be readable, wihout a technical orientation, and had to maintain the tenets of scientific rigor. Second, the article had to reflect the present direction(s) of the particular area covered and suggest further avenues of investigation. The intent was to present a compelling compila- tion of challenging and informative articles from various perspectives in the area of abnormal psychology. As is the case with any book, but especially with a book of readings, many people deserve acknowledgment and thanks. I would like to thank the authors and publishers who permitted their material to be reprinted, and many of my students who were interested enough to discuss critically the issues covered in the book. A number of people worked behind the scenes to ready this book for publication. Among them, appreciation is notably due Donna Petta and Ellen Kasher, Catherine LaPlante, Michael Dolker, and Kenneth Suc- kerman for their assistance in the preparation of the manuscript. I also thank my colleague and friend, Dr. Mark Sherman, for reviewing the manuscript and offering comments. The major typing of the manuscript and the letters of permission was expertly done by Vera Richardson who donated a significant amount of her time to this project. I thank her very much. I also wish to express my gratitude to Dr. Arnold P. Goldstein, consulting editor at Pergamon Press, for his support and many helpful suggestions concerning the manuscript. Finally, special thanks are due my wife, Vinnie, for her editorial assistance as well as her everpresent encouragement and support from the formulation to the completion of this book. RICHARD J. MORRIS Syracuse, New York About the Author Richard J. Morris (Ph.D., Arizona State University) is presently Associate Professor of Psychology, Syracuse University, and a member of the Clinical Faculty, Department of Pediatrics, State University of New York, Upstate Medical Center. He has also taught at Arizona State University. Dr. Morris is a member of several professional associations and serves as a Consulting Editor for Rehabilitation Psychology and as a Consultant for the Veterans Administration Hospitals. His published works and papers are in the areas of behavior therapy and experimental personality. UNIT I Issues in Identifying Abnormal Behavior INTRODUCTION The assumption that has been made traditionally by most clinicians is that it is necessary to diagnose and classify a person's abnormal behavior before treatment can take place. This assumption is based on the procedure followed in medicine regarding when people with physical illnesses are treated. Mental health profes- sionals who use this approach generally believe that once the basic cause, or underlying malady, has been diagnosed from a patient's symptoms (maladaptive behaviors), then the best possible treatment procedure can be prescribed. They further point out that since a patient's symptoms are signs of an underlying psychological disorder or neurological defect direct treatment of these symptoms would not be appropriate. Rather, they feel that the underlying disorder should be the focus of treatment. The use of this approach in the diagnosis and treatment of abnormal behavior is known as the medical model. According to this model, before techniques can be developed to successfully treat various abnormal behaviors, a system must be developed whereby these be- haviors can be identified and classified. One of the first systematic attempts to classify abnormal behavior was performed by Emil Kraepelin in the last half of the nineteenth century. He defined two major categories of mental illness—manic- depressive psychosis and dementia praecox (later called schizophrenia). These categories were used, with some modification, by mental health professionals until the classification system was completely revised in the early 1950s by a committee of the American Psychiatric Association. This revision resulted in the publication in 1952 of a new and more elaborate classification system called the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-I). In 1968, this manual was revised by a similar committee of the American Psychiatric Association resulting in the publication of the new classification system, also called the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-II). This revised system is the one currently in use in psychiatric settings. In recent years the validity of the medical model approach has been questioned by an increasing number of professionals in the field of mental health (e.g., Ull- mann & Krasner, 1965). They point out that because there may be an underlying basis to the symptoms of a patient with a particular physical illness, it does not l 2 Perspectives in Abnormal Behavior necessarily follow that there must also be an underlying cause for the symptoms of a patient with a mental health problem. They further maintain that the maladap- tive behaviors (symptoms) a patient shows are, in fact, his psychological disorder, that is, there is no underlying cause to these behaviors. This view implies that the clinician should direct his efforts toward the treatment of maladaptive behaviors and not the treatment of a presumed underlying cause. For example, if a patient has a fear of driving over bridges, a fear of flying, a fear of elevators, and a fear of becoming interpersonally close to another person, the psychologist or psychiatrist should consider these maladaptive behaviors as the patient's disorder and treat these problems directly (either separately or all at one time), instead of trying to find the underlying problem causing each of these behaviors. One prominent person who has questioned the appropriateness of the medical model is Thomas S. Szasz, author of Article 1. He views mental illness as a myth; that is, he feels that there are no underlying neurological causes to mental illness. He points out that the construct "mental illness" has led mental health profession- als away from the very problems they should be treating, namely, the patient's maladaptive behaviors. Thus, this construct should either be redefined in terms of maladaptive behaviors that can be observed or be removed from the language of the mental health professions. Szasz also suggests that the symptoms that clini- cians observe in patients represent nothing more than "problems in living," or deviations from the social norm, and he reiterates his point that such deviations are what constitute the mental illness. A similar position is taken by Thomas J. Scheff, author of Article 2, who directs his discussion to that diagnostic category called schizophrenia, the diagnosis given to the majority of individuals who are hospitalized for psychiatric problems. Mak- ing use of labeling theory, he states that schizophrenia is a label applied by people in our society (and by societies who share our values) to those individuals who break the established social rules of acceptable conduct. Since behaviors that deviate from the social customs of a society are not usually tolerated by members of that society, and since these unacceptable modes of behavior do not fit into conventional categories such as crime, drunkenness, prostitution, etc., members of the society utilize a socially acceptable approach in dealing with people who show these unacceptable modes of behavior. Specifically, the society makes use of a residual category that involves labeling these people as mentally ill (e.g., as schizophrenic) and thus enables society to maintain what Scheff refers to as the "public order." In addition to questioning the validity of the medical model, some researchers have taken a critical look at the reliability of the diagnostic classification system used in the mental health field. They are particularly interested in discovering the extent to which clinicians agree on the diagnosis given to a person in a psychiatric setting. The general findings of this research, based on a large number of studies published since 1950, have shown that the reliability of specific psychiatric diag- noses is not impressive. Article 3, by Edward Zigler and Leslie Phillips, is addressed to the issue of the reliability of psychiatric diagnoses, and discusses the Issues in Identifying Abnormal Behavior 3 advantages and disadvantages of these diagnoses. Zigler and Phillips' review of the diagnostic system shows that the degree of agreement among professionals re- garding such general and inclusive psychiatric categories as psychosis, neurosis, or character disorders is at an acceptable level; however, as the classification sys- tem narrows to specific diagnoses within these general categories (e.g., anxiety state neurosis or paranoid schizophrenia), the level of agreement decreases. The authors also discuss reasons why many professionals have questioned the utility of psychiatric diagnoses—for example, one reason relates to the lack of clarity regarding which symptoms are associated with each diagnostic category. They conclude that there is merit in the use of psychiatric diagnoses providing that their use is solely descriptive, that they are based on empirically derived behavior correlates, and that they are not used for purposes of inferring cause. Article 4 by Frederick H. Kanfer and George Saslow, extends the discussion of psychiatric diagnoses an additional step. The authors reject the use of the be- havioral correlate approach to diagnoses since they feel that this approach is not reliable, is of limited prognostic value, and does not differentially determine which type of treatment a particular patient should receive. They feel that the clinician should use a classification system that will help him in the planning of a treatment approach for his particular patient. As an alternative to the current diagnostic practice, these authors suggest that the clinician should undertake a behavior analysis of each patient. Such an analysis would include an extensive interview with the patient, as well as the gathering of information about the patient from various people with whom he interacts, the observation of his interactions with others, and a review of the results of his psychological tests. This differs from traditional diagnostic approaches, but the authors feel that once all the informa- tion from this behavior analysis is processed and organized the therapist will be in a position to determine the best treatment procedure for the patient. This ap- proach to diagnosis follows quite closely from Kanfer and Saslow's theoretical position concerning the conduct of therapy. Commonly called behavior modifica- tion, or behavior therapy, their position utilizes techniques based on various theories of learning. It also rejects the use of labels to summarize a patient's be- havior and favors the clinician's detailed description of the patient's behavior. References American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1952. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (2nd ed.) Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1968. Ullman, L., & Krasner, L. Case studies in behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1965. 1 The Myth of Mental Illness* THOMAS S. SZASZ My aim in this essay is to raise the question "Is there such a thing as mental ill- ness?" and to argue that there is not. Since the notion of mental illness is extremely widely used nowadays, inquiry into the ways in which this term is employed would seem to be especially indicated. Mental illness, of course, is not literally a "thing"—or physical object—and hence it can "exist" only in the same sort of way in which other theoretical concepts exist. Yet, familiar theories are in the habit of posing, sooner or later—at least to those who come to believe in them—as "objective truths" (or "facts"). During certain his- torical periods, explanatory conceptions such as deities, witches, and microorganisms ap- peared not only as theories but as self-evident causes of a vast number of events. I submit that today mental illness is widely regarded in a somewhat similar fashion, that is, as the cause of innumerable diverse happenings. As an antidote to the complacent use of the notion of mental illness—whether as a self-evident phenomenon, theory, or cause—let us ask this question: What is meant when it is asserted that someone is mentally ill? In what follows I shall describe briefly the main uses to which the concept of mental illness has been put. I shall argue that this notion has outlived whatever usefulness it might have had and that it now functions merely as a convenient myth. MENTAL ILLNESS AS A SIGN OF BRAIN DISEASE The notion of mental illness derives its main support from such phenomena as syphilis of the brain or delirious conditions—intoxications, for instance—in which persons are known to manifest various peculiarities or disorders of thinking and behavior. Correctly speaking, however, these are diseases of the brain, not of the mind. According to one school of thought, all so-called mental illness is of this type. The assumption is made that some neurological defect, perhaps a very subtle one, will ultimately be found for all the disorders of thinking and behavior. Many contemporary psychiatrists, physicians, and other scientists hold this view. This position implies that people cannot have troubles—expressed in what are now called "mental illnesses"—because of differences in personal needs, opinions, social aspirations, values, and so on. All problems in living are attributed to physicochemical processes which in due time will be discovered by medical research. "Mental illnesses" are thus regarded as basically no different than all other diseases (that is, of the body). The only difference, in this view, between mental and bodily diseases is that the former, affecting the brain, manifest themselves by means of mental symptoms; whereas the latter, affecting other organ systems (for example, the skin, liver, etc.), mani- •Frorn Szasz, T. S., "The myth of mental illness," American Psychologist, 1960, 15, 113-118. Copyright © 1960 by the American Psychological Association, and reproduced by permission. 4

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.