ebook img

Personality risk for antisocial behavior PDF

16 Pages·2017·0.59 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Personality risk for antisocial behavior

DevelopmentandPsychopathology,2017,page1of16 #CambridgeUniversityPress2017 doi:10.1017/S095457941700061X Personality risk for antisocial behavior: Testing the intersections between callous–unemotional traits, sensation seeking, and impulse control in adolescence FRANKD.MANN,aSARAL.PAUL,aJENNIFERL.TACKETT,bELLIOTM.TUCKER-DROB,aAND K.PAIGEHARDENa aUniversityofTexasatAustin;andbNorthwesternUniversity Abstract Thecurrentprojectseekstointegrateliteraturesonpersonalityriskforantisocialbehavior(ASB)byexamininghowcallous–unemotionaltraitsrelateto(a)the developmentofdisinhibitedtraitsand(b)theassociationbetweendisinhibitedtraitsandASB.InStudy1,usinganationallyrepresentativesampleof youth(N.7,000),weexaminedwhetherconductproblemsandlackofguiltassessedduringages4–10yearspredictedlevelsofandchangesindisinhibited traitsoverthecourseofadolescence,andmoderatedassociationsbetweenthesetraitsandASB.Highlevelsofchildhoodconductproblemswere associatedwithhigherlevelsofimpulsivity,sensationseeking,andASBinearlyadolescence,whereaslackofguiltwasassociatedwithlowerlevelsof sensationseeking.Neitherconductproblemsnorlackofguiltsignificantlypredictedchangesinimpulsivityorsensationseeking,andassociationsamong changesinsensationseeking,impulsivity,andASBwerealsoconsistentacrosslevelsofconductproblemsandlackofguilt.InStudy2,usingacross-sectional sampleofadolescents(N¼970),wetestedwhethercallous–unemotionaltraitsmoderatedassociationsbetweendisinhibitedtraitsandASB.Consistent withtheresultsofStudy1,associationsbetweendisinhibitedpersonalityandASBwereconsistentacrossacontinuousrangeofcallous–unemotionaltraits. Antisocial behavior (ASB), which is deviant behavior that Frick&White,2008;Moffitt,1993).Thegoalofthecurrent violatessocialnormsand/ortherightsofothers,ismorecom- paperistocontributetoanintegrationoftheseliteratures,by monduringadolescencethananyotherpointinthelifespan. testingtherelationsamongdisinhibitedtraits,callous–unemo- GalvanizedbythehighcostsofASB,bothtoindividualwell- tionaltraits,andASBintwoindependentsamples. being and to society as a whole, considerable research has been devoted to understanding the personal risk factors that underlie adolescent ASB. One constellation of risk factors SensationSeekingandImpulsivityPredict that predicts adolescent engagement in ASB is disinhibited AdolescentASB personalitytraits,includingimpulsivityandsensationseeking On average, adolescents experience marked age-related per- (e.g., Byck, Swann, Schalet, Bolland, & Mustanski, 2015; sonalitychange, particularly in the personality facets of im- Cooper,Wood,Orcutt,&Albino,2003;Waltonetal.,2016; pulsivityandsensationseeking.Forthepurposesofthecur- Zuckerman,2007).Cross-sectionalandlongitudinalresearch rentpaper,wedefineimpulsivityasafailuretoexertcognitive (Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011; Steinberg et al., 2008) has controloverbehavioralimpulsesandatendencytoactwith- demonstrated that mean levels of disinhibited traits show out considering potential consequences, whereas we define marked age-related change during adolescence, and adoles- sensation seeking as the tendency to prefer and seek out centswhoshowespeciallyrapidincreasesindisinhibitedtraits novel, exciting, rewarding, and/or dangerous experiences also show more rapid increases in ASB (Harden, Quinn, & (Steinberg et al., 2008; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). Impul- Tucker-Drob, 2012). Previous studies have also shown that sivity monotonically declines across adolescent develop- ASBisaheterogeneousconstruct,withchildhoodcharacter- ment,whilesensationseekinginitiallyincreasesduringearly istics, including early-onset conduct problems and callous– adolescence, but then declines during the transition into unemotional traits, differentiating more severe forms of adulthood. These divergent patterns of personality change ASBthataremorelikelytopersistacrossthelifespan(Frick, have been documented in both cross-sectional and longitu- Ray,Thornton,&Kahn,2014,2015;Frick&Viding,2009; dinalsamples,usingbothbehavioralandself-reportmeasures (Cauffmanetal.,2010;Galvan,Hare,Voss,Glover,&Casey, 2007;Harden&Tucker-Drob,2011;Lynne-Landsman,Gra- Addresscorrespondenceandreprintrequeststo:FrankD.Mann,Department ber, Nichols, & Botvin, 2011; Shulman, Harden, Chien, & ofPsychology,UniversityofTexasatAustin,108EastDeanKeetonStop A8000,Austin,TX78712-1043;E-mail:[email protected]. Steinberg,2015;Steinbergetal.,2008). 1 Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X 2 F.D.Mannetal. Personalitychangeduringadolescencehasbeenexplained & Iacono, 2006; Hittner & Swickert, 2006; Littlefield & in terms of the dual systems model, and the related matura- Sher,2010;Littlefield,Sher,&Steinley,2010). tionalimbalancemodel,whichpositsthatadolescentbehav- ioral development is shaped by a “maturity gap” between Callous–UnemotionalityandLife-Course neurobiological systems (Casey, 2015; Shulman et al., PersistentASB 2016).Thecognitivecontrolor“topdown”systemincludes corticalregionsthatregulateimpulsecontrol,whilethesocio- While research testing the predictions of the dual-systems emotionalor“bottomup”systemincludessubcorticalregions model continues to accumulate, a largely separate literature (e.g.,theventralstriatumandamygdala),whichareinvolved has identifiedanotherform ofpersonalityriskforASB:cal- in regulating responses to emotional and motivational cues. lous–unemotional traits, which reflect “a lack of empathy The cognitive control system has a protracted maturational and affective processing” (Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Mof- course throughout adolescence, resulting in slow but steady fitt,&Viding,2011,p.730)andan“interpersonalstylechar- gainsinimpulsecontrol.Incontrast,thesocioemotionalsys- acterizedbyalackofguiltandempathyandacalloususeof temdevelopsrapidlyinearlyadolescence,resultinginaspike others”(Frick&Viding,2009,p.1115).Inabehavioralge- insensation-seekingbehavior.Thisdevelopmentalasymme- neticanalysisof9-yearoldtwins,Viding,Jones,Paul,Moffitt, try between top down and bottom up systems is thought to andPlomin(2008)foundthatcallous–unemotionaltraitsdis- drivetheriseinrisk-takingbehaviorthatistypicalofadoles- tinguished a highly heritable subtype of childhood conduct cence,includingantisocialexpressionsofrisktaking. problems;geneticinfluencesaccountedfor71%versus36% This model has largely focused on normative adolescent of the variance in conduct problems among children with development,inthatitseekstodescribepersonalityandbe- andwithoutcallous–unemotionaltraits,respectively.Inaddi- havioral changes that are (a) typical of most adolescents tion, Fontaine et al. (2011) found that callous–unemotional and(b)developmentallyuniquetoadolescence. Asaresult, traitsinchildhooddistinguishedasubgroupofantisocialchil- the changes in disinhibited traits described by the dual sys- drenwhohadthemostnegativeprofileoffamilyriskatage4 tems model may best account for adolescent-onset trajecto- and who showed the worst peer problems, emotional prob- riesofASB(Moffitt,1993,2003,2006).Thatis,thematura- lems, and parent–child relationships at age 12. These results tional imbalance between sensation seeking and impulse suggest the possibilityof differential etiological pathwaysto control may characterize the etiology of ASB among teen- ASBamongyouthhighincallous–unemotionaltraits. agerswhohaveminimalhistoriesofbehavioralproblemsin Within Moffitt’s (1993, 2003, 2006) developmental tax- childhood,whofirstbegintoengageinrule-breakingbehav- onomy, children with high and persistent levels of callous– ior during adolescence, and who engage predominantly in unemotionaltraitsmaybeconsideredasubtypeoflife-course risk-taking(asopposedtoaggressive)formsofASB. persistent ASB, characterized by the continuation of behav- Consistentwiththisperspective,HardenandTucker-Drob ioralproblemsacrossdevelopmentalperiods.Thelife-course (2011) demonstrated that,in additionto mean-level patterns persistent trajectory is conceptualized as a more virulent of change, there are individual differences in the rapidityof strainofASB.Forexample,life-coursepersistentindividuals change in impulsivity and sensation seeking across adoles- engageinmoreaggressiveandviolentacts,withearlyonset cence.Moreover,suchindividualdifferencesinchangepre- ofconductproblemsinchildhoodpredictingalifelongcourse dict the development of ASB and related externalizing phe- ofaggressivebehaviorandcriminal offending(Laheyetal., notypes. Specifically, adolescents who show more rapid 1998; Lynam, 1996; Odgers et al., 2008). Acknowledging increases in sensation seeking between ages 12 and 16 also theimportanceofcallous–unemotional traitsintheetiology showmorerapidemergenceofASBduringthatsameperiod, anddevelopmentalcourseofASB,DSM-5addeda“limited alongitudinalassociationthatcannotbeaccountedforbyre- prosocialemotions”specifiertothediagnosisofconductdis- ciprocaleffectsofinvolvementinASBonsubsequentlevels ordertocharacterizeyouthatheightenedriskforaseveresub- ofsensationseeking(Hardenetal.,2012).Thesefindingsare typeofASB(AmericanPsychiatricAssociation,2013). consistentwith abreadthofpreviousevidenceshowingele- vated sensation seeking among antisocial youth (Byck IntegratingDevelopmentalModelsofASB et al., 2015; Mann, Kretsch, Tackett, Harden, & Tucker- Drob, 2015; Newcomb & McGee, 1991; Sijtsema et al., Although disinhibited personality and callous–unemotional 2010;Zuckerman,2007).Previousresearchhasalsodemon- traits have all been established as risk factors for youth stratedapositiverelationshipbetweenimpulsivityanddelin- ASB,howthesetraitsintersectwithoneanotherislessclear. quency(Bechtold,Cavanagh,Shulman,&Cauffman,2014; Asmallnumberofstudieshaveshownthatconductproblems Cooperetal.,2003;Frick&Viding,2009;Giannotta&Ry- accompanied by callous–unemotional traits are associated dell,2015;Moffitt,1993;Snowden&Gray,2011;Vitacco& withadistinctpatternofpersonalitycorrelates,ascompared Rogers, 2001). Moreover, both impulsivity and sensation to child-onset conduct problems in the absence of callous– seeking have been implicated in the development of related unemotional traits. For example, controlling for comorbid externalizing behaviors in adolescence, including alcohol conduct problems, callous–unemotional traits were nega- and substance use (Dicket al., 2010; Elkins, King, McGue, tively associated with neuroticism, agreeableness, and trait Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X Personalityandadolescence 3 anxiety,whereasconductproblemswithoutcallous–unemo- literature in formulating our predictions.Specifically,we pose tionaltraitswereassociatedwithelevatedanxietyandneurot- twoquestions.First,doyouthwithlowlevelsofguiltinchild- icism (Frick, Lilienfeld, Ellis, Loney,& Silverthorne, 1999; hood (a component of callous–unemotional traits) also show Lynametal., 2005;Pardini, Lochman,& Powell,2007;see higher levels of and more rapid changes in disinhibited traits Frick&Ray,2014;Frick&White,2008,forreviews).Cal- inadolescence?Putdifferently,dolowlevelsofguiltinchild- lous–unemotionaltraitswerealsouniquelyandpositivelyas- hoodpredictindividualdifferencesinthedevelopmentofsensa- sociated with fearlessness and thrill seeking (Essau, Saga- tionseekingor impulsivityinadolescence? Second,doeslow waga,&Frick,2006;Fricketal.,1999;Pardini,Obradovic, guiltinchildhood,orhighlevelsofcallous–unemotionaltraits & Loeber, 2006). Based on these results, children with cal- inadolescence,moderatetherelationshipbetweendisinhibited lous–unemotionaltraitswerehypothesizedtoevincea“fear- traitsandASBinadolescence?Thatis,dotherelationshipsbe- less, thrill seeking, and behaviorally uninhibited tempera- tweendisinhibitedtraitsandASBgeneralizetoyouthwithlow ment”(Frick&Viding,2009,p.1116). levelsofguiltinchildhoodorwhoshowhighlevelsofcallous– Empiricalandtheoreticalworkonadultpsychopathyhas unemotionaltraitsinadolescence?Weexaminethesequestions alsoprovidedsuggestive,butinconsistent,supportforarela- usingtwoindependentsamples.Study1usesalarge,nationally tion between callous–unemotionality and disinhibited traits representative sample of youth followed prospectively from of impulsivity and sensation seeking. Integrating conceptu- childhood through adolescence. Due to measurement con- alizationsofpsychopathy,thetriarchicmodelconceptualizes straintsassociatedwithusingsecondarydata,inStudy1wefo- adult psychopathy as encompassing three traits: meanness, cusononeaspectofcallous–unemotionaltraits:lackofguilt.In boldness, and disinhibition (Patrick & Drislane, 2015; Pa- addition,tocontrolforpotentialconfounds,wealsosimultane- trick, Fowles, & Krueger, 2009). Meanness, defined by Pa- ouslyassessedtheroleofbiologicalsexandconductproblems trick et al. (2009, p. 913) as “aggressive resource seeking inchildhood.Study2measurescallous–unemotionaltraitsmore without regard for others,” is clearly related to, although comprehensivelyinacross-sectionalsampleofadolescents. notperfectlysynonymouswith,theconstructofcallous–un- Based on the small bodyof previous research describing emotionality.Sensationseekingisanaspectofboldness,de- childrenwithcallous–unemotionaltraitsasfearlessandthrill finedas“thenexusofsocialdominance,emotionalresiliency, seeking,anddrawingontheoreticalmodelsofpsychopathyin and venturesomeness,” while disinhibition, defined as “pro- adulthood, we hypothesized that youth with low levels of pensity toward problems as impulse control,” corresponds guilt in childhood and high on callous–unemotional traits to the construct of impulsivity as defined here. Relevant to inadolescencewouldshowhigherlevelsofdisinhibitedtraits ourcurrenthypotheses,thetriarchicmodelpositsthatmean- andASBinadolescence.Inaddition,basedonthetheorythat ness overlaps with both disinhibition and boldness. In par- childrenwithhighlevelsofcallous–unemotionaltraitsshow ticular, the meanness–boldness association is hypothesized morepersistentandstableexpressionsofASB,wehypothe- toreflectthesharedrootsofbothtraitsinadiminishedphys- sized that the relationship between disinhibited traits and iologicalsensitivitytothreat. ASBwouldbeattenuatedforyouthhighoncallous–unemo- Consistent with the triarchic conceptualization, analyses tionaltraits.Thatis,wehypothesizedthatyouthhighoncal- of the Psychopathy Checklist—Revised (Hare, 2003) have lous–unemotional traits would demonstrate ASB in adoles- foundthatFactor1,whichcapturestheinterpersonalandaf- cence regardless of whether or not they experienced high fective symptoms of psychopathy and includes callousness levelsofdisinhibitedpersonalityrisk. andlackofremorse,andFactor2,whichincludesimpulsivity and irresponsibility, are moderately correlated with one an- other ((cid:2).5; Hare et al., 1990; reviewed by Patrick et al., Study1 2009). In contrast, factor analyses of the Psychopathic Per- sonality Inventory in adults (PPI; Lilienfeld & Andrews, Method 1996) found that “cold-heartedness” was uncorrelated with Participants. eitheroftheothertwoPPIfactors,whichincludetraitsrelated toimpulsivityandfearlessness(Benning,Patrick,Hicks,Blo- Mother generation:The National LongitudinalSurveyof ningen, & Krueger, 2003), although the authors suggested Youth (NLSY79). The Bureau of Labor Statistics designed thatthePPIcold-heartednessscalecaptureslackofsentimen- andfundedtheNLSY79inordertostudyworkforcepartici- tality, rather than meanness or callousness. Translating the pation in the United States. A complex survey design was predictionsofthetriarchicmodeldownwardtotheyouthper- used to select a nationally representative sample of 3,000 sonalityconstructsexaminedhere,wehypothesizethatyouth householdscontaining6,111youth,plusanadditionalover- highoncallous–unemotionaltraitswillshowelevatedlevels sample of 3,652 African American and Hispanic youth, ofbothsensationseekingandimpulsivity. aged 14–21 years as of December 31, 1978. The response Insummary,thecurrentstudyaimstocontributetotheinte- ratefortheinitialNLSY79surveywasover90%oftheeligi- gration of two lines of research on the etiology of ASB (the ble sample, and participants have been reinterviewed an- dual–systemsmodel,ontheonehand,andcallous–unemotional nuallyfrom1979to1994andbienniallysince1994.Reten- traits,ontheother),whilealsodrawingontheadultpsychopathy tion rates for follow-up assessments of the NLSY79 sample Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X 4 F.D.Mannetal. weregreaterthan90%forthefirst16wavesandgreaterthan variety of demographic and maternal characteristics: socio- 80%forsubsequentwaves. economicstatus,measuredusingself-reportedtotalfamilyin- come(Mdn¼$22,500/year),includinggovernmentsupport Adolescentgeneration:TheNLSY79ChildrenandYoung andfoodstampsbutexcludingincomereceivedbyunmarried Adults (CNLSY).Beginningin1986,thebiological children cohabitingpartners,whenthemotherwas30yearsold;ma- of the NLSY79 women were assessed biennially (Chase- ternal cognitive ability, measured in the 1980 assessment Lansdale,Mott,Brooks-Gunn,&Phillips,1991).Theinitial using composite scores on the word knowledge, paragraph participation rate was 95%, and the average retention rate comprehension, math knowledge, and arithmetic reasoning through 2006 was approximately 90%. Beginning in 1988, subtestsoftheArmedServicesVocationalAptitudeBattery; childrenovertheageof10completedindividualsupplemen- maternal education, measured using maternal report of the talinterviewsthatassessed theirattitudes andbehaviors.Fi- number of years of school they had completed (M ¼ 13.4 nally, beginning in 1994, older children who were age 15 years,SD¼2.50years;approximately9%ofthesamplere- bytheendofthesurveycalendaryear(termedyoungadults ported11yearsorless);andmaternalageatfirstbirth(M¼ bytheNLSYsurveyadministrators)wereadministeredasep- 21.9,SD¼4.52,range¼11.7–38.3years).Thisprocessen- arateinterview.Asof 2006,11,466childrenwereidentified suresthatassociationsbetweenchildhoodcharacteristicsand ashavingbeenbornto6,283NLSY79women.Afterweight- disinhibited traits are not confounded by these household ing for sample selection, the average NLSY79 woman had characteristics. It also ensures that the associations between 1.9childrenasof2006,whichismorethan90%oftheirul- disinhibitedtraitsandASBarenotsimilarlyconfounded.Re- timate predicted childbearing. The current study uses data sidualizedsumscoreswerethenstandardizedtozscoresprior from youth who had nonmissing data on conduct problems toconductinganalyses. or lack of guilt in childhood at least once between the ages of4and10,andwhoreportedondelinquentbehaviorordis- AdolescentASB.Beginningin1988,childrenwhowereat inhibitedpersonalityatleastoncebetweenages12and16. least10yearsoldbutnotyet15wereadministeredtheChild Self-Administered Supplement, which included six items from the Self-Report of Delinquency (SRD; Elliott & Hui- Measures. zinga, 1983): hurt someone bad enough to need bandages Disinhibited personality. Impulsivity was measured by or a doctor; lied to a parent about something important; youthself-reportonthreeitems:“Ioftengetinajambecause tooksomethingfromastorewithoutpayingforit;intention- Idothingswithoutthinking,”“Ithinkthatplanningtakesthe ally damaged or destroyed property that did not belong to funoutofthings,”and“Ihavetousealotofself-controlto you;hadtobringyourparent(s)toschoolbecauseofsome- keep out of trouble.” Sensation seeking was measured by thingyoudidwrong;andskippedadayofschoolwithoutper- youthself-reportonthefollowingthreeitems:“Ienjoytaking mission.Beginningin1994,youthwhowere15yearsoldor risks”;“Ienjoynewandexcitingexperiences,eveniftheyare olderwereadministeredtheYoungAdultAssessment,which alittlefrighteningorunusual”;and“Lifewithnodangerinit alsoincludedthesixSRDitems.Becauseofbudgetarycon- wouldbetoodullforme.”Thesesixitemscomprisedascale straints,theSRDitemsweredroppedfromtheYoungAdult intended to measure propensity for risk taking, which were Assessment (but not the Child Self-Administered Supple- drawnfrommultipleinventories(CenterforHumanResource ment)in2000only.EachoftheSRDitemswasdichotomized Research,2009),andallitems were ratedona4-point scale asneverversusatleastonceormoreandsummed.Symptom ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Note that countsthusrangedfrom 0to6(Mdn¼1,M ¼1.29,SD¼ previouslypublishedfactoranalyticworkoftheseitemsindi- 1.39). Previous analyses of CNLSY data (described in cates that they comprise distinct factors (Harden & Tucker- Harden et al., 2009) have tested the criterion validityof the Drob, 2011). Moreover, the pattern of correlations between SRD items and found that these items significantly predict, sensationseeking,impulsivity,andBigFivepersonalitytraits forbothmalesandfemales,theoddsofbeingconvictedfor inthefullCNLSYsamplewereconsistentwithpreviousem- a nontrivial criminal offense (excluding drug possession), piricalresearch,withsignificantandpositivecorrelationsbe- controllingforabroadvarietyofdemographicandcontextual tweensensationseekingandconcurrentmeasuresofextraver- backgroundvariables.TheCronbachavalueforthesixSRD sionandopenness,andsignificantandnegativecorrelations itemswas0.64–0.68ateachassessmentwave. between impulsivity and concurrent measures of conscien- tiousnessandemotionalstability(thereverseofneuroticism). Child conduct problems. Biennially beginning in 1986, The internal consistencies of the impulsivity and sensation mothers reported on children’s conduct problems using seeking scales, averaged across waves, were moderate to nineitems:cheatsortellslies;bulliesoriscruel/meantooth- high in magnitude (a ¼ 0.52 for moderate, a ¼ 0.69 for ers; breaks things deliberately; is disobedient at school; has high). troublegettingalongwithteachers;arguestoomuch;isdis- Following previously published analyses of these vari- obedient at home; is stubborn,sullen, or irritable; and has a ables (Harden, Quinn, & Tucker-Drob, 2012), sensation- strongtemperandlosesiteasily.Itemswereratedona3-point seeking and impulsivity sum scores were residualized for a scalerangingfromnotatalltruetoverytrue.TheCronbacha Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X Personalityandadolescence 5 value for the nine items was 0.81–0.83 across assessment McArdle & Nesselroade, 2003; Meredith & Tisak, 1990) waves.Abroadcompositescoreforchildhoodconductprob- in the software program Mplus version 7.11 (Muthe´n & lemswasconstructedbycalculatingthemeanresponseacross Muthe´n, 1998–2012). Absolute model fit was evaluated us- nineitemsandfouragegroups:4–5,6–7,8–9,and10–11(M ing model x2 and root mean square error of approximation ¼0.40,SD¼0.29).Table1summarizesthestabilitiesacross (RMSEA), which measures the amount of misfit on the childhoodforconductproblems,descriptivestatisticsateach modelpermodeldegreeoffreedom(Steiger,1990).RMSEA age,andthezero-orderrelationsbetweenthecompositescore valuesoflessthan0.05indicateaclosefit,andvaluesupto and scores at each age. For subsequent analyses, conduct 0.08 represent reasonable errors of approximation. Model problemsscoreswerestandardized. comparisons were made using change in model x2 and Akaike information criteria (AIC), which provide a parsi- Childlackofguilt.Bienniallybeginningin1986,mothers mony-adjustedindexofpredictivefit,withlowervaluesindi- reportedonwhethertheirchild“doesnotseemtofeelsorry catingpreferredmodels. about misbehaving” on a 3-point scale ranging from not at ThetrivariategrowthcurvemodelisdepictedinFigure1. all true to very true. The mean rating on this item across ChangesinadolescentASB,impulsivity,andsensationseek- ages4–5,6–7,8–9,and10–11(M¼0.39,SD¼0.42)was ingfromages12–13yearsto15–16yearswereeachmodeled used to measure average lack of guilt across childhood. asafunctionofalatentinterceptfactor,representingindivid- Table 2 summarizes the stabilities across childhood (lack ualdifferencesininitiallevelsofeachconstruct,andalatent of) guilt, sample statistics at each age, and the relations be- slopefactor,representingindividualdifferencesinwithin-in- tweenthecompositescoreandscoresateachage.Toeasein- dividual change across the three waves of data collection. terpretation,lackofguiltscoreswerestandardizedforusein Consistent with previously published analyses of these data subsequentanalyses. (Hardenetal.,2012),toaccountfordeparturesfromlinearity, changewas modeled using a “latent basis” model, in which Analytic methods. Patterns of missing data are complex and theloadingsofeachslopefactoronthefirstandlastmeasure are described in detail in the CNLSY user’s guide (see ofeachconstructwerefixedto0and1,respectively,whilethe http://www.nlsinfo.org/childya/nlsdocs/guide/intro/Retention. loadings onthe intermediatemeasurementswere freelyesti- htm). Of the 7,106 youth included in the current analysis, matedfromthedata(McArdle,2009).Next,thelatentinter- 80% contributed data on ASB at ages 12–13, 78% at ages ceptandslopefactorswereregressedonthreevariables:bio- 14–15, and 70% at age 16–17. All models were estimated logical sex, child conduct problems, and child lackof guilt. using full information maximum likelihood, which has These regressions test whether average levels of childhood been recommended as the preferred method for accounting conductproblems,lackofguilt,andmalesexpredictindivid- formissingdata(Schafer&Graham,2002).Inaddition,stan- ualdifferencesinthedevelopmentofdisinhibitedpersonality darderrorsandmodelfitstatisticswereadjustedfornoninde- inadolescence. pendenceofdataonchildrenfromthesamefamily(i.e.,sib- Next, we expanded the trivariate growth curve (main ef- lingclusters;Rabe-Hesketh&Skrondal,2006).Dataweremod- fects)modeltoatrivariategrowthcurve(moderation)model eled using a series of latent growth curve models (LGMs; that allowed for the correlations among the latent intercept Table1.Descriptivestatisticsandobservedcorrelationsamongmeasuresofconductproblemsandlowguiltinchildhood (National Longitudinal Surveyof Youth) DescriptiveStatistics ConductProblems LowGuilt ChildhoodMeasure M(SD) 4–5 6–7 8–9 10–11 Avg. 4–5 6–7 8–9 10–11 Avg. Conductproblems Age4–5 0.42(0.34) 1.00 Age6–7 0.37(0.32) .55 1.00 Age8–9 0.39(0.34) .51 .61 1.00 Age10–11 0.39(0.35) .47 .56 .65 1.00 Childhoodmean 0.40(0.29) .77 .80 .84 .84 1.00 Lowguilt Age4–5 0.44(0.63) .36 .23 .21 .22 .31 1.00 Age6–7 0.39(0.61) .23 .37 .28 .26 .34 .20 1.00 Age8–9 0.37(0.60) .24 .29 .42 .30 .38 .19 .26 1.00 Age10–11 0.37(0.59) .24 .27 .33 .45 .40 .18 .23 .28 1.00 Childhoodmean 0.39(0.42) .40 .44 .50 .50 .55 .64 .65 .66 .66 1.00 Note:Cross-time,same-measurecorrelationsareitalicandcross-measure,same-timecorrelationsarebold.Allcorrelationsaresignificantlydifferentthanzeroat p,.05. Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X 6 F.D.Mannetal. Table2.Observedcorrelationsamongchildhoodcharacteristics,disinhibitedpersonality,andASB(NationalLongitudinal Surveyof Youth) IMP-12 IMP-14 IMP-16 SS-12 SS-14 SS-16 ASB-12 ASB-14 ASB-16 ChildCP ChildLG IMP-12 1.00 IMP-14 .27 1.00 IMP-16 .24 .28 1.00 SS-12 .29 .16 .15 1.00 SS-14 .07 .28 .18 .34 1.00 SS-16 .12 .06 .30 .26 .43 1.00 ASB-12 .28 .20 .15 .20 .11 .08 1.00 ASB-14 .17 .33 .22 .18 .25 .12 .37 1.00 ASB-16 .14 .18 .28 .14 .20 .28 .26 .41 1.00 ChildCP .19 .16 .19 .09 .06 .08 .26 .21 .21 1.00 ChildLG .11 .08 .10 .01 .02 .04 .16 .13 .14 .55 1.00 Note:Cross-time,same-measurecorrelationsareitalicandcross-measure,same-timecorrelationsarebold.ASB,antisocialbehavior;IMP,impulsivity;SS,sen- sationseeking;CP,conductproblems;LG,lowguilt(codedsuchthathigherscoresindicatelowerlevelsofguilt).Correlationswithanabsolutevalueof,.03 arenotsignificantlydifferentthanzeroatp,.05(twotailed). factorsandlatentslopefactorstovaryaslinearfunctions of the data well (x2 ¼ 101.29, df ¼ 25, p , .001; RMSEA ¼ sex, childhood conduct problems, and lack of guilt. This 0.02,comparativefitindex[CFI]¼0.98,AIC¼84,011.71). model was used to test whether (level–level and slope– Figure 2 showsthe predicted levels of impulsivity, sensation slope1) associations between disinhibited personality and seeking,andASBimpliedbythegrowthcurvemodel,ascom- ASB are moderated by individual differences in levels of paredtotheobservedmeansateachage. guilt.Becausecallous–unemotionaltraitsarehigher,onaver- The model captures the observed pattern of mean-level age,inmalesandyouthwithchildhoodconductproblems,we changequitewell,accountingfor41%–54%oftheobserved includedthemoderatingeffectofsexandconductproblems, variance in sensation seeking at each age, 26%–40% of the inordertoensurethatanymoderatingeffectsoflackofguilt observed variance in impulsivity, and 39%–42% of the ob- were not artifacts of sex differences or comorbidity with served variance in ASB. As previously reported (Harden childhoodconductproblems.Allmoderationeffectswerees- et al., 2012; Harden & Tucker-Drob, 2011), mean changes timated simultaneously using moderated structural equation in sensation seeking and ASB during adolescence were modeling (for an overview and detailed example, see positive,whereasmeanchangeinimpulsivity was negative. Tucker-Drob,2009),suchthateacheffectcontrolsforpoten- Figure3showstheestimatedcorrelationsamongtheintercept tialmoderationbytheothercovariates.Thesemoderationef- andslopefactorsforthethreeconstructs. fectstestwhethertherelationsamonglevelsofandchangesin Thereweresignificantlevel–levelassociationsamongim- disinhibited personality and ASB are consistent across bio- pulsivity,sensationseeking,andASB:adolescentswhowere logicalsex,aswellasindividualswithahistoryofchildcon- higher,onaverage,onanyoneofthesephenotypestendedto duct problems and low levels of guilt. Given the numberof be higheron all of the others. Moreover, therewere signifi- moderationeffectstestedinthismodel(m¼18),todecrease cantchange-changeassociations:adolescentswhoincreased thechanceofTypeIerrors,weadoptedaBonferroni-correc- morerapidlyinsensationseekingalsoincreasedmorerapidly ted threshold for statistical significance (a/m ¼ a ¼ in delinquent behavior, and adolescents who declined more corrected 0.002), which is more stringent than conventional standards slowly in impulsivity also showed more rapidly increasing (a¼0.05). delinquentbehavior.Finally,therewasaninverseassociation betweenlevelandchangeforeachvariable:individualswho ResultsofStudy1 hadinitiallyhigherlevelsshowed,onaverage,lessrapidin- Doeslowlevelsofguiltinchildhoodpredictthedevelopment creases (for sensation seeking and ASB) or more rapid de- ofdisinhibitedpersonalityinadolescence?ThefirstLGMfit creases(forimpulsivity).Suchnegativelevel–slopeassocia- tionsarefrequentlyobservedinLGMresults.Thus,resultsof the trivariate latent growth curve model are consistent with 1. Cross-variablelevel–slopeassociationsweretested.However,usingAIC previousliterature. andBICasanindicatorofmodelfit,amodelthatfreelyestimatedcross- variablelevel–slopeassociationsbetweenimpulsivity,sensationseeking, Figure4plotsthemainparametersofinterestfromModel and antisocial behavior fit the data worse (AIC ¼ 84,020.39, BIC ¼ 1,thatis,thestandardizedregressionsofthelatentintercept 84,446.25)thanamodelthatconstrainedcross-variablelevel–slopeasso- andslopefactorsonlackofguilt,malesex,andchildconduct ciationstoequalzero(AIC¼84,011.71,BIC¼84,396.35).Changein problems. Regarding the development of sensation seeking modelx2confirmedthatconstrainingcross-variablelevel–slopeassocia- tionstoequalzerodoesnotresultinsignificantmisfittothedata(Dx2¼ (shownintheleftpanelofFigure4),malesexandchildcon- 2.93,Ddf¼6,p¼.82). duct problems predicted higher levels of sensation seeking, Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X hD ttpow s:/wnlo wwade .camd fro bm ridge http .orgs:/w /coww re/te.cam rms. https://dbridge.org/co oi.org/1re. Univ 0e .1017/S0rsity of T 9e 5x 457941700061Xas Libraries, on 0 6 Ju l 2 0 1 7 a t 1 6:0 7 9 :5 6 , su b je ct to th e C a m b rid g e C o re te rm s o f u se , a v a ila b le a Figure1.Thetrivariategrowthcurvemodel(NationalLongitudinalSurveyofYouth). t 8 F.D.Mannetal. Figure2.Thepredictedlevelsofimpulsivity,sensationseeking,andantisocialbehaviorimpliedbythegrowthcurvemodel,ascomparedtothe observedmeansateachage(NationalLongitudinalSurveyofYouth). whereas low levels of guilt in childhood predicted lower biological sex: male sex and child conduct problems, but levelsofsensationseeking.Inaddition,malesshowedmore not lack of guilt, predicted higher levels of ASB in adoles- rapid increases in sensation seeking, compared to females. cence.However,noneofthevariablessignificantlypredicted Neither child conduct problems nor lack of guilt predicted change in ASB (ps . .10). Thus, while biological sex and change in sensation seeking. Regarding impulsivity (shown conductproblemsinchildhoodpredictedinitiallevelsofdis- in the right panel of Figure 4), male sex and child conduct inhibitedtraits,individualdifferencesinchangeintheseper- problems predicted higher levels of impulsivity, whereas sonalitytraitswerenotsignificantlyassociatedwithbiologi- child lack of guilt was unrelated to levels of impulsivity. calsex,conductproblems, or levels ofguilt. Moreover, low Noneofthethreevariablespredictedchangeinimpulsivity. levelsofguiltinchildhoodpredictedlowlevelsofsensation Note that these effects were estimated while simultaneously seekinginadolescence,butdidnotpredictlevelsofimpulsiv- accounting for associations between disinhibited traits and ity or ASB. In other words, higher conduct problems pre- ASBacrossadolescence(reportedinFigure5),aswellasas- dictedhighersensationseeking,impulsivity,andASB;con- sociations between adolescent antisocial behavior and con- trolling for these effects, low levels of guilt predicted lower duct problems in childhood, lack of guilt in childhood, and levelsofsensationseeking. Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X Personalityandadolescence 9 Figure3.Theestimatedcorrelationsamongtheinterceptandslopefactorsforthethreeconstructs(NationalLongitudinalSurveyofYouth). Dochildconductproblemsandlackofguiltmoderateasso- fromModel2aretheinteractiontermsrepresentingwhether ciations between disinhibited personality development and male sex, child conduct problems, and child lack of guilt ASB in adolescence? The interaction model (Model 2) fit moderate the associations among disinhibited personality the data significantly better than Model 1 (Satorra–Bentler and ASB in adolescence. The interaction terms are scaled scaled chi-square difference test [Dx] ¼ 36.85, Ddf ¼ 18, such that they represent the extent to which the level–level p¼.005,AIC¼84,004.79).Thekeyparametersofinterest orslope–slopeassociationsbetweentwoconstructsincrease Figure4.(Coloronline)ThemainparametersofinterestfromModel1,thatis,thestandardizedregressionsofthelatentinterceptandslope factorsonlackofguilt,malesex,andchildconductproblems(NationalLongitudinalSurveyofYouth). Downloaded from https:/www.cambridge.org/core. University of Texas Libraries, on 06 Jul 2017 at 16:09:56, subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at https:/www.cambridge.org/core/terms. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941700061X hD ttpow s:/wnlo wwade .camd fro bm ridge http .orgs:/w /coww re/te.cam rms. https://dbridge.org/co oi.org/1re. Univ 0e .1017/S0rsity of T 9e 5x 457941700061Xas Libraries, on 0 6 Ju l 2 0 1 7 a 1 t 1 0 6 :0 9 :5 6 , su b je ct to th e C a m b rid g e C o re te rm s o Figure5.Thestructuralequationmodel(TexasTwinProject). f u se , a v a ila b le a t

Description:
Abstract. The current project seeks to integrate literatures on personality risk for antisocial behavior (ASB) by examining how callous–unemotional traits relate to (a) the . vated sensation seeking among antisocial youth (Byck et al., 2015 . also provided suggestive, but inconsistent, support fo
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.