ebook img

Person Perception and Attribution PDF

323 Pages·1989·7.71 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Person Perception and Attribution

SSSP Springer Series in Social Psychology SSSP Hans-Werner Bierhoff Person Perception and Attribution With Contributions by R. Klein Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg New York London Paris Tokyo Hong Kong Hans-Werner Bierhoff Department of Psychology University of Marburg Gutenbergstr. 18 3550 Marburg/Lahn, FRG With 33 Figures ISBN-13: 978-3-642-74132-6 e-ISBN-13: 978-3-642-74130-2 001: 10.1007/978-3-642-74130-2 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data. Bierhoff, Hans Werner, 1948- Person perception and attribution/H.-W. Bierhoff. p. cm. - (Springer series in social psychology) Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. ISBN-13: 978-3-642-74132-6 1. Social perception. 2. Attribution (Social psychology) 3. Impression formation (psychology) 4. Stereotype (psychol- ogy) I. Title. 11. Series. HM132.B54 1989 302'.12-dc20 89-11389 This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, re-use of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in other ways, and storage in data banks. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is only permitted under the provisions of the German Copyright Law of September 9, 1965, in its version of June 24, 1985, and a copyright fee must always be paid. Violations fall under the prosecution act of the German Copyright Law. © Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1989 Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1s t edition 1989 The use of registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. Product Liability: The publisher can give no guarantee for information about drug dosage and application thereof contained in this book. In every individual case the respective user must check its accuracy by consulting other pharmaceutical literature. Typeset by K +V Fotosatz GmbH, 6124 Beerfelden, FRG. 2126/3130-543210 - Printed on acid-free paper Preface Person perception is of great importance in everyday life and human science. Judgment of other people's characteristics and intentions is important for suc- cessfully planning actions within a social environment. Questions about the formation of impressions and causal attributions are central to social psychology and the study of diagnostic judgment formation. The field of per- son perception deals with questions of how impression formation proceeds, what characteristics and intentions are attributed to other people, and how preformed schemata and stereotypes influence people's first impressions. Research on person perception developed rapidly after the Second World War. In the 1950s the precision and accuracy of person perception received special interest, but the problems concerning whether an individual's assessment of another personality is exact or not could not be solved. Another approach, which began in the 1940s and was derived from the Gestalt psychological tradi- tion, dealt with impression formation based on selected social cues. This ap- proach, which proved to be very useful, had considerable influence on both the research methods and the theoretical orientation of the research work. On the one hand, by using a combination of individual cues (like physical characteris- tics) researchers tried to ascertain how an impression of a person was formed. On the other hand, the Gestalt psychological orientation led to an interest in the process of person perception, which in the last 10 years has concentrated on questions concerning information reception and processing. In the 1960s a new field, attribution research, began to apply itself to the perceived causality of behavior. The distinction between internal and external causes led to a series of informative studies based on an average person's "naive" theory about social situations and people who act in these situations. Later analyses led to more refined multidimensional models of attributions. This preface was written during a transatlantic flight. Waiting for the jet to land, I had the opportunity to devote some time to the perception of people. For example, I made comparisons related to possible sex differences. The man who was sitting in front of me read a physics journal while the woman on my vi Preface left read a fashion magazine. These types of observations feed gender-related stereotypes, which are widespread in many societies. Is there a kernel of truth in gender stereotypes? If so, how can it be accounted for? These are some of the questions which are addressed in Chapters 3 and 5. In this book four issues are given special emphasis: first impression forma- tion, expectancy confirmation versus individuating processes, stereotypes, and attributions. The work on impression formation by Asch (1946) is taken as a starting point for a discussion of social cognition research. In this opening chapter - as in the whole text - a balance is sought between recent research and theory and basic studies and approaches which have contributed to the continuity of research on the perception of people over several decades. Person perception as an inference process is discussed in Chapter 2. Issues pertaining to "going beyond the information given" were already emphasized by Bruner (1957 a, b). We discuss selection of relevant data, strategies for sam- pling and classifying the data gathered, and SUbjective estimates of relation- ships between two variables. In addition, two inference models of social perception, one based on the diagnostic ratio derived from the Bayes theorem and the other on the anchor heuristic, are described in some detail. Stereotypes are understood as cognitive schemata or prototypes. Gender stereotypes, ethnic stereotypes, and stereotypes which refer to physical ap- pearance are discussed. The importance of the social structure as a determi- nant of the content of stereotypes is mentioned. Furthermore, motivational in- fluences on derogation of the outgroup are discussed with reference to Sherif's realistic conflict theory and Thjfel's social identity theory. Attributional phenomena are considered from a broad perspective. The pioneering ideas of Heider, Bern, Jones and Davis, and Kelley are discussed in some detail. The clarification of the covariation principle by the logical model of Jaspars and Hewstone and the analysis of commonsense attributions by Hansen are presented as examples of how knowledge and research in this area have advanced. Finally, the issue of attributions of responsibility is discussed in some detail. Responsibility attributions occupy an intermediate position be- tween causal attributions and blaming. Although an understanding of respon- sibility attributions is crucial to an understanding of mechanisms of social sanctioning, it is unfortunately often not the case that researchers working on the latter have a genuine understanding of the former - especially when com- plex causal chains are involved. In Chapters 3 and 5 expectancies which generalize over situations or people are contrasted with individuating processes and processes of self-verification. Issues of vividness of impressions, accuracy of social perception, and diagnostic information from single cases are related to this contrast between global schemata and individual evidence. Depending on their personal preferences, researchers have emphasized one or other end pole. More integra- tion of theory and research in this respect would be desirable, although the work of Snyder and Swann and of Thylor and Fiske is promising. In this con- nection, different values are attached to individual cases. While from the Preface vii perspective of the representativeness heuristic (lVersky and Kahneman, 1974) single cases are regarded as possible sources of distortion of the relevance of base-rate information used in forming rational judgments, from the perspective of stereotypes attention paid to single cases serves as a protection against overgeneralization and irrational distortions in person perception. In this book I have emphasized the positive. In other words, I have tried to focus on promising lines of research without criticizing other approaches exten- sively. Another factor which contributed to my selectiveness is related to my per- sonal biases and preferences. A second book, which might be titled Critical Issues in Person Perception Research, would certainly supplement this one very well. I am painfully aware of many methodological problems in the research reports. In fact, after reading the relevant literature I have become to some extent more skeptical on this point than I was before. I will mention here only replicability of results (especially of significant statistical interactions), the ques- tionable reliability of many popular dependent measures, which neglect the pro- cesses which are supposed to be being studied, and demand characteristics caus- ed by leading questions and a tendency to emphasize confirmation more than disconfirmation. Another problem relates to the small samples of subjects studied, which are also frequently not representative of people in general. I have tried to take these problems into account implicitly, but I am aware that I was only partially successful in this respect. Space limitations and the goal of pro- viding a general overview of theories and research on person perception did not allow me to discuss this critical aspect more extensively in the text. It is a truism that every study has its strengths and weaknesses. For example, naturalistic studies which promise a high ecological validity usually do not allow strict control of independent variables. In contrast, laboratory studies lack ecological validity. Therefore, different approaches complement each other, each one compensating for deficiencies which are inherent in another. Unfortunately, a tendency to concentrate on judgmental studies which allow easy and quick procedures for collecting data can be seen. Be this as it may, considerable efforts are being made to bring the study of person perception into the course of social interactions (see Chapter 5). I want to thank a number of people who helped me during the preparation and writing of this book. These include JOrg Albers, Christiane Block, Michael Hemer, Peter Kramp, Helmut Lamm, Angela Ludwig, Bemd Schafer, Franziska Schneider, and Wolfgang Stroebe. My special thanks go to two people. Nicola Preese helped me with the translation into English and improved my English in parts which were originally written in English. Renate Klein, who is my co- author in the second chapter on inference processes in impression formation, read the whole manuscript and improved it in many ways. In order to improve the readability of the text I have placed the more detailed reports on particular investigations and the details of some theoretical models in boxes (numbered 11-126 for investigations and T1-T13 for theoretical models). Hans-Wemer Bierhoff Contents 1. The First Impression. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Central Characteristics and Gestalt Perception. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 Implicit Personality Theories Complete the First Impression ... 9 Daring: Courage or Recklessness? Implicational Variety of 'Itait Adjectives. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 Cognitive Representation of Persons ........................ 22 Halo Effects: When Single Items of Information Are Dominated by the Overall Impression... ... . ....... . ... ... . . 35 Order of Presentation of Information. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43 The First Impression: Practical Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71 2. Reasoning in Impression Formation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 Uncovering Relationships. . ... . . . . . . ... . ... . . . . .. . . .. . . ... . 78 Subjective Probability Models of Stereotypes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94 3. Stereotypes. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 Definitions and History of Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 Areas of Stereotype Research .............................. 112 Theories About Stereotypes. ... . . . . . .... . .. . . . . ... . .. . . ... . 123 Stereotype Research - Practical Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141 4. Attribution: Theories ..................................... 147 Causal Relations ......................................... 154 The Naive Analysis of Behavior . . . . .... . ... . . . . .. . . .. . . ... . 155 Causal Schemata . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 163 Discounting and Augmentation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 167 The Covariation Principle. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181 The Theory of Correspondent Inference.. .... . .. .... .... .... 195 x References Self-Perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 198 Differing Perception of Causes by Actors and Observers. . . . . . . 202 Motivational Influences on the Attribution Process . . . . . . . . . . . 212 Attribution of Responsibility and Blame. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 221 5. Attribution: Applications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 225 Social Motives and Attribution: Achievement Behavior and Helping . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 227 Self-fulfilling Prophecies and Processes of Expectation Confirmation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 236 Vocal Communication: Nonverbal Cues and Social Perception. 248 Person Perception in Social Interaction: Practical Areas of Application. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 252 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 261 Author Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 301 Subject Index. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 309 Chapter 1 The First Impression The basic issues of person perception are forming a first impression about another person and drawing conclusions from this impression. These two topics are therefore treated in Chaps. 1 and 2. The first chapter emphasizes the processes involved in impression formation, whereas in Chap. 2 stress is laid upon the conclusions that are drawn from the first impression. Consider the following example: in the 1970s "the godfathers of heavy metal" (Rolling Stone, July 1985), Jimmy Page and Robert Plant - better known as Led Zeppelin -, were famous for their characteristic stage shows. One of the topics discussed in this chapter deals with the question of whether there are certain central cues that influence the first impression of a person more than other peripheral cues. The loudness of Led Zeppelin's music was probably one such central cue that strongly determined how the band was perceived. In addition, I shall discuss the influence of overall evaluations of a stimulus person on ratings of specific aspects of that person. In terms of the above example, when an observer holds a positive opinion about Jimmy Page or Robert Plant, he or she will tend to attribute positively evaluated character- istics to them (e.g., sincere, dynamic, exciting, attractive). Given a negative overall evaluation, an observer will tend to attribute less favorable characteris- tics (e.g., not sincere) to the stimulus person. Thus an implicit personality theory about the "godfathers" emerges that is inferred from initial cues. Central Characteristics and Gestalt Perception Each of us meets a wide variety of people in everyday life. Even during short encounters a first impression emerges and subsequently influences social behavior (Kelley, 1950; Widmeyer & Lay, 1988). Well-known public figures are good examples to illustrate how person perception functions. They may be politicians or actors and also heroes from fiction and comics. Consider Hagar the Horrible, by Dik Browne; Hagar is a 2 The First Impression mighty man but often expresses childish inclinations. His strength and vitality are manifested in his outer appearance, but the "born leader" often pursues infantile goals. For instance, Hagar gets extremely upset when he finds a leak in the wine cask, whereas he hardly takes any notice of a leak in the Viking ship. Another time he is eager to secure the largest piece of cake for himself. In spite of the seemingly contradictory characteristics attributed;10 Hagar, his stories are very popular and fascinate many readers in a way.-only rarely reported of students reading a psychology textbook. In fact, everyday sayings provide schemata for categorizing Hagar's behavior. Hagar reminds one of the German proverb which says that there is a child in every man, which means that even sincere and maybe frightening men exhibit childlike wishes or behavior. Using this notion, Hagar's contradictory characteristics are in- tegrated into a familiar overall impression that transcends the single traits. How Contradictions May Form a Unity First impressions do not emerge slowly over a long period of time but are formed almost immediately during the first encounter with the target person. Once an impression has been formed it is maintained over a long time, even if no further contact with the stimulus person takes place. From the very begin- ning an impression represents a homogeneous unity that develops quickly and usually lasts for a long time (Asch, 1946, p. 258; Hamilton, 1981). Impression formation is best considered as a process by which an organized overall impression emerges in which single traits receive specific meanings. Every single trait can have a variety of meanings when seen in isolation. For instance, daring may be interpreted as courageous on the one hand and as reckless on the other hand. Connotative meanings of crafty may range from clever to sly (see Hamilton & Zanna, 1974). It is only in context that the range of meanings narrows and a trait receives a specific interpretation. In the tradition of Gestalt psychology, Asch (1946) formulated the hypoth- esis that the impression of a person is more than the sum of his/her single characteristics. In "Forming Impressions of Personality" he contrasted three models of impression formation (see Box T 1) which since then have influenced research on person perception. Box T 1. Models of impression formation Several models describe the process by which different information may be integrated to give an overall impression. 1. Summation model· The overall impression is the sum of the indepen- dent impressions based on single items of information: overall impression = a+b+c+d+e (single items of information are represented by a-e). 2. Generalized halo effect: Rating scale judgments as measures of various traits and characteristics of a person tend to be strongly correlated. To the extent that these correlations are spurious and do not correspond to actual relationships between the rated traits, the overall impression is in

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.