Impulse: The Premier Journal for Undergraduate Publications in the Neurosciences June 2004, 1 (1): 1-58 Olfaction, Emotion & the Amygdala: arousal-dependent modulation of long-term autobiographical memory and its association with olfaction: beginning to unravel the Proust phenomenon? Mark Hughes1 1University of Edinburgh, Centre for Neuroscience, No. 1 George Square, Edinburgh, UK The sense of smell is set apart from other very old autobiographical memories sensory modalities. Odours possess the (episodes known anecdotally as Proust capacity to trigger immediately strong phenomena). Particular emphasis is placed emotional memories. Moreover, odorous on the diversity of functions attributed to stimuli provide a higher degree of memory the amygdala. It’s role in modulating the retention than other sensory stimuli. Odour encoding and retrieval of long-term perception, even in its most elemental memory is investigated with reference to form - olfaction - already involves limbic lesion, electrophysiological, immediate structures. This early involvement is not early gene, and functional imaging studies paralleled in other sensory modalities. in both rodents and humans. Additionally, Bearing in mind the considerable the influence of hormonal modulation and connectivity with limbic structures, and the the adrenergic system on emotional fact that an activation of the amygdala is memory storage is outlined. I finish by capable of instantaneously evoking proposing a schematic of some of the emotions and facilitating the encoding of critical neural pathways that underlie the memories, it is unsurprising that the sense odour-associated encoding and retrieval of of smell has its characteristic nature. emotionally toned autobiographical The aim of this review is to analyse current memories. understanding of higher olfactory information processing as it relates to the ability of odours to spontaneously cue Key Words: Olfaction; Emotion; Proust highly vivid, affectively toned, and often phenomenon; Amygdala. Introduction epilepsy, in states of hypnosis or psychosis, and, less radically, in everybody in response to the mnemonic stimulus of certain words, “Smell is a potent wizard that transports us sounds, scenes, and especially smells. Odours across thousands of miles and all the years we are rarely perceived in a purely objective and have lived… odours, instantaneous and neutral way, often being loaded with appealing fleeting, cause my heart to dilate joyously or or repugnant feelings. Olfactory pathways contract with remembered grief.” Helen involve anatomical structures that are also Keller (blind and deaf educator, 1880-1968) heavily implicated in emotion and memory. This quotation articulates a neurophysiological The amygdala is one such structure. The interaction between the systems of olfaction, challenge is to propose a model of the neural emotion and memory. Hughlings Jackson mechanisms that underlie encoding and (1880), arguably the founder of neurology as a retrieval of odour-cued emotionally toned science, termed this kind of forced autobiographical memories (see figure one). reminiscence as “a doubling of consciousness.” This occurrence is not uncommon during attacks of migraine or Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes Proust Phenomena it was encoded results in a better memory performance than encoding and retrieving in different contexts (Eich et al., 1985). Schab The ability of odours to spontaneously cue (1990) used ambient odour as a context, highly vivid, affectively toned, and often very demonstrating that reinstating the same odour old autobiographical memories is known as at test as that present at study appears to the Proust phenomenon (Chu & Downes, improve memory performance in humans over 2000) and is derived from the experiences of conditions where the ambient odour is not Proust himself (1922): reinstated at test. By manipulating odours that “And soon, mechanically, weary after a dull were appropriate or inappropriate to a day with the prospect of a depressing morrow, situation, Herz (1997) has also shown in I raised to my lips a spoonful of tea in which I humans that distinctive odours result in greater had soaked a morsel of cake. No sooner had context effects. the warm liquid, and the crumbs with it, These studies indicate that ambient odours touched my palate than a shudder ran through present at the time of an event can be encoded my whole body, and I stopped, intent upon the in parallel with event details and consequently extraordinary changes that were taking be used as cues in the retrieval of those event place…I was conscious that it was connected details. The types of memory commonly with the taste of tea and cake, but that it reported in connection with Proust phenomena infinitely transcended those savours, could are typically especially vivid, emotional and not, indeed, be of the same nature as theirs.” old. Therefore, several critical aspects of these Such experiences are not limited to the realm studies do not address the core of Proustian of artistic license. A survey by the National phenomena. Firstly, the age of experiences is Geographic (Gilbert & Wysocki, 1987) gave limited to time between study and test readers a set of six odours on scratch-and-sniff (typically only a few minutes). Secondly, cards. From a sample of 26 200 (taken investigators predetermine the target event, randomly from >1.5 million responses), ~55% instead of allowing a subject to freely select an of respondents in their 20s reported at least autobiographical episode that comes to mind one emotional memory cued by one of the six in response to an odour. Lastly, the subject is odours. not always made aware of the presence of the odour during the retrieval phase. Most Studying odour-cued memories examples of Proustian recall involve an awareness of the odour. For empirical substantiation, other paradigms Many of the approaches used could be that more closely match the Proustian situation described as experiments in odour-cued must be applied. In the standard paired- context-dependent memory rather than associate learning paradigm, odours and other autobiographical memory. Context-dependent stimuli are associated with target information. memory is the effect whereby retrieving Later, the efficacy of each cue type is information in the same environment in which compared in a cued recall test. Herz and Figure 1 Simplified schematic illustrating neuroanatomical and systems-level interactions during the generation of odour- evoked emotional memories. Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes Cupchik (1995) asked subjects to associate analysis of the pleasantness of memories odours or odour labels with emotional yielded mixed and inconclusive results. paintings. Later, they were presented with the Herz and Cupchik (1992) aimed to formulate a odours or words and asked to describe the characterization of odour-evoked memories painting with which each had been associated. rather than compare the efficacy of different A higher degree of emotional tone was found cue types. Their results characterize odour- in the recollections of paintings cued by evoked memories as highly emotional, vivid, odours than those cued by words. Here, specific, rare, and relatively old but leave subjects are actively using odour as an aid to unanswered the question as to whether odour- retrieval of a past experience. However, evoked memories are, for example, more autobiographical memories are usually formed emotional. Herz and Schooler (2002) have through a passive encoding process: separate now answered this question. The emotional experiential aspects of an incident become and evocative qualities of autobiographical associated without any purposeful effort on memories cued by odours and visual cues were the part of the subject. Therefore, there are compared using a new repeated measures likely to be key differences in the underlying paradigm. Results demonstrated that memories processes recruited during encoding (and the evoked in the context of odours were nature of the resulting mnemonic significantly more emotional than those representation) in the above study as compared recalled in the context of the same cue with naturally acquired autobiographical presented visually and by the verbal label for memories. the cue. This is the first unambiguous demonstration that naturalistic memories evoked by odours are more emotional than Examination of ‘true’ those evoked by other cues. Aggleton and Waskett (1999) provided an autobiographical memories excellent demonstration of the potency of olfactory cues. Their experiment focused on a Owing to problems of quantitative display at a Viking museum (York) in which measurement and application of close unusual odours were used to enhance the experimental controls, only a limited number impact of the display. They asked whether re- of studies exist which have directly examined presenting these Viking odours to individuals Proustian phenomena. who had visited the museum a number of years Laird (1935) wrote about a survey of odours ago would allow them to remember more ‘as revivers of memories and provokers of about the display than those presented with thoughts in 254 living men and women of non-Viking odours or a no-odour control eminence.’ More than four out of five of those group. Mean memory performance for those surveyed reported olfactory-cued memory receiving the Viking odour was numerically experiences that were described as vivid, higher than for those receiving the non-Viking emotional and old. 76% of women and 46% of odour and the no-odour control group. men reported that odour-cued memories were However, due to the expected large variation amongst their most vivid with only 7% of in performance, the difference failed to reach women and 14% of men describing these statistical significance. memories as hedonically neutral. This type of study design is getting closer to a Contemporary studies employ more scientific fully ecologically valid approach to Proustian and empirical approaches, for example the phenomena. Naturally occurring experiences single-cue comparison method. Data is are used and the association between collected on memories that are retrieved in experience and ambient odour is passively response to odours and are directly compared formed. Several clear-cut results strongly with those retrieved in response to stimuli support the proposition that odours are from other sensory modalities. Rubin et al., effective reminders of autobiographical (1984) found that odour-cued memories were experience. thought of and spoken of with less frequency The apparent emotional, vivid and deep-rooted than those cued by other stimuli. Contrary to nature of odour-evoked memories can be Proustian expectations, no differences were explained by two hypotheses. (1) The found between different cue types, and ‘differential cue affordance value’ hypothesis Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes suggests that different sensory modalities vary to be mediated by the action of the amygdala in terms of cue affordance value (or the (Cahill et al., 1995, 1996). efficiency with which they access Now equipped with a behavioural background autobiographical event details). Olfactory cues to odour-cued emotional memory, we move on are assumed to have a higher cue affordance to analyse the subject from a neurobiological than other cue types. (2) The ‘differential perspective. encoding bias’ hypothesis proposes that cues do not differ in terms of their efficacy in retrieving event details, but in terms of the The amygdala and olfactory types of event with which they become processing associated and consolidated in memory. Olfactory details may become associated only, or preferentially, with more complex The amygdala refers to a highly differentiated (emotional, personal, and unusual) region near the temporal pole of the autobiographical episodes. Consequently, the mammalian cerebral hemisphere. Burdach presentation of an olfactory cue will bias discovered and named the amygdala nucleus in retrieval towards more complex episodes. the early nineteenth century (Burdach, 1819- The encoding specificity principle (Tulving & 1822). Subsequent microscopic examination Thomson, 1973) proposes that consolidated began to reveal a progressively complex memory traces include not only target structural differentiation. The extent of the information, but also salient contextual amygdala’s outer border, and the number and features in the immediate environment in classification of its subdivisions, remains which the episode was experienced. Later controversial today. Swanson and Petrovich presentation of such contextual features (1998) suggest that the amygdala is neither a (which include odours) can therefore act as structural nor a functional unit. One part is a retrieval cues for related details that comprise specialized ventromedial expanse of the the original episode. If this alone were the striatum, a second part is caudal olfactory case, then all cue types would function in the cortex (nucleus of the lateral olfactory tract, same manner and with equal effectiveness. cortical nucleus, and postpiriform and However, assuming the superior ability of piriform-amygdalar areas) and a third part is a olfactory cues to evoke emotional ventromedial extension of the claustrum. autobiographical memories, encoding Cell groups within the amygdala are derived specificity alone does not provide a from different regions: their connectivity and satisfactory explanation. Here lies the the distribution of neurotransmitters within challenge. each grouping form the basis of Olfaction is mediated by a number of differentiation. Consequently, different groups anatomical structures that are also heavily can be distinguished functionally into four implicated in emotion and memory. The obvious systems – accessory olfactory, main olfactory bulb, for example, projects to olfactory, autonomic and frontotemporal structures including the amygdala, cortical. hippocampus, and thalamus. These structures The amygdala receives highly processed have been shown to be involved in memory sensory data and is in a position to integrate function and the modulation of emotions. this data both within and across modalities. Proustian effects may also be linked to the The only sensory modality with direct access ability of odours to elicit affective reactions. to the amygdala is smell (see figure two). All Hinton and Healey (1993) compared reactions other modalities connect with the amygdala to stimuli presented in visual, lexical, and via a series of sensory association cortices. olfactory modalities. Odours elicited by far the The amygdala can also influence autonomic, most affective reactions. hormonal and motor function via its Some (Herz, 1997; Aggleton & Waskett, subcortical connections. 1999) have ascribed the efficacy of odours in memory retrieval, at least partially, to the acknowledged connection between emotional arousal and the information associated with such affective reactions. This link is expected Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes Amygdala involvement in non- tests used to date must lack the requisite specificity to pick up amygdala-dependent pheromonal forms of olfactory aspects of olfactory function. related behaviour Electrophysiological studies: In one study 229 neurons were recorded in the lateral and Lesion studies involving rodents: Combined basolateral nuclei of rats during olfactory electrolytic lesions of the posterior lateral discrimination learning (Schoenbaum et al., olfactory tract and anterior amygdala in rats 1999). Of these, 60 were found to show showed no effect on the retention or learning selective responses depending on whether a of new olfactory discrimination tasks (Slotnik, particular olfactory cue was paired with a 1985). Bilateral amygdala lesions showed no pleasant or an unpleasant outcome. Selectivity effect on learning of multiple odour often occurred early in learning and many discriminations (Eichenbaum et al., 1986). units could be reversed, indicating that the Failure of amygdala lesions to result in amygdala neurons encoded the motivational impaired performance seems surprising given significance of the cues. Main olfactory bulb Other main olfactory system Accessory olfactory system Medial prefrontal, COApl TR PAA COAa agranular insular perirhinal, FS hippocampal cortex ACB PA BLAp BMAp BMAa CEA MEA SI (r) (r,i) (r) (d,r) MDm LHAcl THpg PB Figure 2 Major neural inputs and outputs of the main olfactory system as they relate to the rat amygdala (adapted from Swanson and Petrovich, 1998). Abbreviations: ACB, nucleus accumbens; BLAp, basolateral nucleus amygdala, posterior part; BMAa,p, basomedial nucleus amygdala, anterior, posterior parts; CEA, central nucleus amygdala; COAa,pl, cortical nucleus amygdala, anterior part, posterior part, lateral zone; d, medial hypothalamic defensive behavior system; FS, fundus of the striatum; i, medial hypothalamic ingestive behaviour system; LHAcl, lateral hypothalamic area, caudolateral part; MDm, mediodorsal nucleus thalamus, medial part; MEA, medial nucleus amygdala; PA, posterior nucleus amygdala; PAA, piriform-amygdala area; r, medial hypothalamic reproductive behavior system; SI, substantia innominata; THpg, thalamus, perigeniculate region (includes medial geniculate complex, posterior limiting nucleus, and parvicellular subparafascicular nucleus); TR, postpiriform transition area. the extensive connections of the olfactory Immediate Early Gene (IEG) studies: c-fos system illustrated previously and the finding imaging data supplements evidence for that approximately 40% of rat amygdala involvement of the basolateral amygdala neurons respond to olfactory stimuli (Cain & (BLA) as olfactory learning progresses (Hess Bindra, 1972). The lack of obvious deficits et al., 1997). Different stages of olfactory appears to be caused, in part, by the existence discrimination learning by rats were compared. of parallel olfactory inputs to other regions Initial exploration (no specific odours) led to that are able to counterbalance the destruction raised medial (medial, anterior, and posterior of amygdala olfactory routes. The olfactory cortical nuclei) and, to a lesser extent, raised Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes basolateral activation. In contrast, central matching are severely impaired, although nucleus levels stayed low. Following initiation H.M. can detect weak odours and appreciate of cued olfactory discrimination training there odour intensity (Eichenbaum et al., 1983). In was high basolateral but relatively low medial both instances, it is possible that the activation. In over-trained rats, the differences combination of amygdala and entorhinal in these amygdala regions were reduced. pathology is critical for identification. These Balance of amygdala activation shifts in a and related case studies suggest that the dynamic manner that depends on the demands involvement of the human amygdala in of the task and upon the stages of learning. olfaction is often not made apparent by lesion Lesion studies involving primates: The studies. Presumably, this reflects the presence contribution of the primate amygdala, and in of multiple olfactory projection routes (i.e. particular the human amygdala, to olfactory parallel routes to orbitofrontal, thalamic, and processing remains poorly understood. Group other temporal regions), just as is seen in studies have shown that comprehensive, rodents. To further investigate involvement of unilateral temporal lobectomies can confuse the human amygdala in olfaction, the use of olfactory identification (Rausch & functional imaging studies and the Serafetinides, 1975) and the recall of odours development of more selective tests is (Rausch et al., 1977). However, these necessary. experiments provide limited direct evidence Functional imaging studies: Zald and Pardo for the specific involvement of the amygdala. (1997) reported that exposure to a highly As a rule, studies recording the effects of aversive odourant produces a strong increase surgery to remove just the amygdala or parts in cerebral blood flow as measured by positron of the amygdala have described no disruption emission tomography (PET) in both to the sense of smell (Scoville et al., 1953; amygdalae as well as in the left orbitofrontal Narabayashi, 1977), though these studies lack cortex (see figure three). proper test details. A more systematic study Interestingly, activity within the left amygdala has investigated odour discrimination in three was associated significantly with subjective groups of unilateral temporal lobe resection ratings of aversiveness, suggesting that cases where the site of surgery was classed as amygdala activity is related to the perceived either (1) primarily neocortical, (2) hedonic value of the stimulus. Tests conducted amygdalohippocampectomy, or (3) anterior with pleasant smells (fruits, spices, and florals) temporal lobe with impingement on the only produced a non-significant increase in amygdala or hippocampus (Jones-Gotman et blood flow in the right anterior al., 1997). All three groups were impaired, amygdala/periamygdaloid area. This study although no clear correlations could be drawn suggests that amygdala activation occurs when between the presence of an impairment and olfaction engages strong negative emotional amygdala damage. reactions. Human electrophysiological studies: These Aversive odours can affect amygdala have confirmed the responsiveness of activation via associative processes, as has amygdala neurons to odours (Andy & Jurko, been shown by Schneider et al., (1999) in a 1975) and shown that amygdala activity can functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) lead to olfactory auras (Andy et al., 1975). A study. The presentation of a neutral face that small body of single-case clinical studies also had previously been paired with an aversive implicates the amygdala in aspects of olfactory odour led to a decrease in amygdala activity in processing. One case involves a woman who a group of 12 normal subjects, whereas had received an extensive, bilateral increased amygdala activation was found in a amygdalotomy that had involved the group of 12 subjects suffering from social entorhinal cortex in one hemisphere (Andy et phobia. al., 1975). Although her detection of odours Activation of the amygdala by aversive smells was largely unchanged, her identification of and their associations may suggest other ways odours was impaired. Similarities are seen in in which smells can influence memory. patient H.M., in whom the amygdala, Aggleton’s real world study (Aggleton & entorhinal cortex, and hippocampus were Saunders, 2000) examined the long-term removed bilaterally (Corkin et al., 1997). retention of specific smells by people who had Odour discrimination, identification, and previously visited a Viking museum. People Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes who had visited the museum many years al., 1995). A substantial body of evidence previously almost always recalled that there suggests, however, that lasting had been smells. However, only those that explicit/declarative memory for emotionally were independently rated as being most averse arousing information is stored in other brain were remembered consistently. It was also regions and that the activation of the amygdala found that the same cues acted as contextual modulates the storage of memory in those cues to help subjects to recall details of the brain regions (e.g. Cahill & McGaugh 1998; museum, despite having not visited it for many Weinberger, 1995). years (Aggleton & Waskett, 1999). Figure 3 (above and right) Cerebral activation during aversive olfaction. Changes in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) are rendered in colour with white indicating the greatest magnitude (Z score > 5) of activation. The relative positions of coronal sections (A, B, and C) through the frontal (a and b) and temporal (c) lobes are shown schematically to the right of this text. Maximal areas of rCBF change are displayed superimposed on a standard T- 1-weighted magnetic resonance image. The rCBF maxima map to the amygdala bilaterally and the left posterior lateral orbitofrontal cortex. The right side of this figure shows the left side of the brain. Abbreviations: VCA, vertical line through anterior commissure; CACP, intercommissural line (reproduced with kind permission from Zald & Pardo, 1997, © 1997 by PNAS). Intriguingly, this suggests that the amygdala Hormonal modulation of memory enhances learning when a subject is storage emotionally aroused, and that arousing smells might also be effective contextual cues Gerard (1961) proposed “…epinephrine…is through a similar process. released in vivid emotional experiences, such This pathway is a strong candidate for having that an intense adventure should be highly a role in the neural underpinnings of Proustian memorable.” He was the first to advance this phenomena. An in depth look at arousal- hypothesis. Gold and van Buskirk (1975) dependent memory modulation by the found that systematic post-training injections amygdala follows. of adrenaline administered to rats enhances long-term retention of inhibitory avoidance. These findings have been confirmed by The amygdala and modulation of experiments using many different types of memory storage: animal studies learning tasks, all in support of the hypothesis that endogenously released adrenaline There is now general agreement that the modulates memory storage (e.g. Liang, 2000; amygdala is involved in the learning of Williams, 2000). As with adrenaline, post- emotionally arousing information (Cahill et training administration of moderate doses of Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes glucocorticoids also induce dose- and time- clenbuterol into the amygdala produces dose- dependent modulation of memory storage dependent enhancement of memory storage (Roozendaal & McGaugh, 1996a). These (Hatfield & McGaugh, 1999). This strongly findings strongly suggest that glucocorticoids supports the hypothesis that the amygdala released by strongly emotionally arousing plays a critical role in memory consolidation. experiences enhance the consolidation of the In the periphery, β-adrenoceptors are located memory for those experiences (Roozendaal, on vagal afferents that project to the nucleus of GABAergic agonists and antagonists α- and β-adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists NTS GABA β cAMP Projections to STRIA GR NA other brain OP α GR TERMINALIS? structures 1 Opioid agonists and antagonists BASOLATERAL AMYGDALA vagus nerve BLOOD-BRAIN BARRIER Peripherally acting β- adrenergic Corticosterone antagonists Adrenaline Figure 4 Schematic summarizing the interactions of neuromodulatory influences in the basolateral amygdala on memory storage. Abbreviations: α1 = α1-adrenoceptor; β1 = β1-adrenoceptor; cAMP = cyclic 3’5’ adenosine monophosphate; GR = glucocorticoid receptor; NA = noradrenaline; NTS = nucleus of the solitary tract; OP = opioids (adapted from McGaugh et al., 2000). 2000). the solitary tract (NTS) (Schreurs et al., 1986). Amygdala mediation of Projections from the nucleus of the solitary tract are known to release NA within the neuromodulatory influences on amygdala (Ricardo & Koh, 1978). Adrenaline memory storage does not readily pass through the blood brain barrier. It seems likely that adrenaline effects Noradrenaline (NA) release in the brain may memory storage by activation of peripheral β- regulate memory consolidation (Kety, 1972). adrenoceptors on vagal afferents. In support of Gold and van Buskirk’s finding (1978) that this theory, inactivation of the NTS with adrenaline and footshock induce the release of lidocaine blocks adrenaline effects on memory NA in the forebrain of rats has provided storage (Williams & McGaugh, 1993). evidence consistent with Gerard and Kety’s Recent findings also indicate that the suggestions. Contemporary studies now amygdala is involved in mediating indicate that NA release within the amygdala glucocorticoid influences on memory storage is critically important in enabling (Roozendaal, 2000). Lesions of the stria neuromodulatory influences on memory terminalis or of the amygdala block the storage. Post-training intra-amygdala infusions memory enhancing effects of post-training of β-adrenoceptor antagonist propranolol systematic injections of synthetic block adrenaline effects on memory storage glucocorticoid dexamethasone (Roozendaal & (Liang et al., 1986) and post-training infusions McGaugh, 1996a,b). These glucocorticoid of NA or the β-adrenoceptor agonist Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes effects involve noradrenergic activation of the system-activating) learning situations should amygdala. engage amygdala participation in memory Noradrenergic activation in the amygdala is formation, independently of whether the also critical for other neuromodulatory particular emotions involved are positive or influences on memory storage. Post-training negative. Hence, an emotionally toned systemic injections of opioid peptides and olfactory stimulus is ‘predisposed’ to opiates generally impair memory storage and preferential consolidation by the amygdala and opiate antagonists enhance memory storage therefore improved storage. (Izquierdo & Diaz, 1983; McGaugh, 1989; Although there is strong evidence in humans McGaugh et al., 1993). GABAergic for amygdala participation in LTM storage for antagonists and agonists administered post- emotionally arousing events, evidence for its training enhance and impair retention, participation in memory retrieval, or for its respectively (Brioni & McGaugh, 1988; production of emotion, per se, is currently less Brioni et al., 1989). Lesions of the amygdala clear. or stria terminalis block opioid peptidergic and GABAergic influences on memory storage (McGaugh et al., 1986). Also, and perhaps Emotionally influenced LTM: the most importantly, intra-amygdala infusions of role of the adrenergic system the β-adrenoceptor antagonist propranolol block opioid peptidergic and GABAergic Propranolol (a synthetic β-adrenergic receptor control of memory storage (McGaugh et al., blocking agent) attenuates the enhanced LTM 1988). These neuromodulatory interactions associated with emotionally arousing within the amygdala are summarized information without affecting memory for schematically in figure four. more emotionally neutral information (Cahill Studies that have not involved inhibitory et al., 1994). Hence, activation of the avoidance training have yielded similar results adrenergic system in humans is required for to those summarized above. Hence, there is the enhancing effect of arousal (either evidence that amygdala influences on memory emotionally or physically induced) on storage are not restricted to the learning of memory. The action of propranolol is aversive information. mediated centrally (van Stegeren et al., 1998). Adrenergic stimulation can act in a retrograde manner to enhance memory in humans Modulation of long-term memory (Soetens et al., 1995). (LTM) in humans: adrenergic activation and the amygdala Emotionally influenced LTM: the I begin here with the assumption that the brain role of the amygdala must possess a means to ‘weight’ or moderate the actual information storage mechanisms in Scoville and Millner examined memory in ten approximate proportion to the importance of patients, including some with relatively the information being stored (Gold & selective amygdala damage and concluded McGaugh, 1975). Without modulatory “removal of the amygdala bilaterally does not mechanisms, the brain would be unable to appear to cause memory impairment” distinguish the important from the trivial in (Scoville & Millner, 1957). However, their LTM. There appear to be, at minimum, two tests involved short-term memory, and/or interacting neurobiological components memory for non-emotionally arousing mediating the modulatory actions of emotional material. These findings fit well with the arousal on memory: the adrenergic system and ‘memory modulation’ view of amygdala the amygdala (especially BLA). An interaction function, which predicts that amygdala activity between endogenous stress hormones activated should not be critical for STM, or for LTM of by emotional experiences and the amygdala non-emotionally arousing events or material. appears critical. According to animal studies, A rare patient with damage confined almost we can hypothesize that sufficiently exclusively to her amygdala reported deficits emotionally arousing (i.e. sympathetic nervous Pages 18-37 Olfaction, Emotion and the Amygdala: Hughes in memory for emotional material (Babinsky et observed either pleasant or unpleasant al., 1993). Enhanced LTM associated with an arousing images. Memory for the images one emotionally arousing story is impaired in month later correlated very highly with patients with selective (Cahill et al., 1995) or amygdala activity while viewing either the nearly selective (Adolphs et al., 1997) pleasant or unpleasant images. Amygdala amygdala damage. Amnesic patients with activity whilst viewing emotionally neutral undamaged amygdalae show reasonably intact images (including images of novel, but not enrichment of memory for emotional data emotionally arousing objects) did not correlate despite their overall impaired memory with recall. The arousing dimension of the performance (Hamann et al., 1997). The pictures was essential to amygdala emotional reactions of patients with amygdala engagement in memory, as indicated in damage to emotionally provocative stimuli are another recent fMRI study (LaBar et al., not significantly different from those of 1998). controls even though LTM for emotional Although activity of both the left and right material is impaired (Cahill et al., 1996). amygdala correlated with recall of arousing Electrodermal responses to emotionally material, the degree appeared greater on the stressful events are intact in humans with right for negative slides (Hamann et al., 1999). amygdala damage (Adolphs et al., 1997). On Cahill et al. (1996), who used emotionally the basis of these and PET studies described, negative material and found a unilateral effect, Cahill et al., (1996) propose that amygdala also suggest that differences in left versus right activity in humans “may be more important amygdala participation in memory for for the translation of an emotional reaction emotionally arousing events may relate to their into heightened recall than it is for the pleasant/unpleasant nature. Left amygdala generation of an emotional reaction per se.” activation appears to be produced consistently Brain imaging studies: In one study, subjects by any of a variety of aversive stimuli used in received two PET scans: one while viewing a human brain imaging studies (Cahill & series of relatively emotionally arousing films, McGaugh, 1998; Zald & Pardo, 1997, 2000 for another while viewing a series of relatively olfactory examples). No compelling emotionally neutral films (Cahill et al., 1996). explanation exists at present for such Memory for the films was tested in a surprise asymmetries in amygdala function. free recall test three weeks later. Activity in Canli and colleagues (1999) reported that the right amygdala correlated very highly amygdala activity in response to emotionally (r=0.93) with the number of emotional films negative pictures correlated with long-term (2- recalled three weeks later, but not with recall 14 months) retention of pictures. In an of the neutral films (see figure five). individual fMRI study conducted in Figure 5 (A) Amygdala activity while watching a series of emotionally arousing films correlated very highly with long- term (three-week) recall of the films. (B) Amygdala activity in the same subjects while viewing a series of relatively emotionally neutral films did not correlate significantly with recall (reproduced with kind permission from Cahill et al., 1996, © 1996 by PNAS). Hamann and colleagues (1999) used PET to collaboration with the above, it was found that study cerebral blood flow while subjects increasing the degree of emotional arousal
Description: