ebook img

Neural Systems for Reading Aloud PDF

17 Pages·2010·1.08 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Neural Systems for Reading Aloud

CerebralCortexAugust2010;20:1799--1815 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp245 AdvanceAccesspublicationNovember17,2009 Neural Systems for Reading Aloud: A William W.Graves1, Rutvik Desai1,Colin Humphries1, MarkS. Seidenberg2and JeffreyR. Binder1 Multiparametric Approach 1Department ofNeurology, MedicalCollege of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI53226, USAand 2Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison,WI 53706, USA Reading aloud involves computing the sound of a word from its readingsystem.Theregressionapproach,incontrast,allowsfor D o visual form. This may be accomplished 1) by direct associations thesimultaneousinvestigationofmanyvariablesofinterest,but w n between spellings and phonology and 2) by computation from inpractice,thesevariablesareoftencorrelatedwitheachother lo a orthographytomeaningtophonology.Thesecomponentshavebeen (e.g., word frequency and length tend to be negatively d e sstuucdhieadsiwnobrdehfarevqioureanlceyx;pleenrigmthenitnsleetxtearmsinoirngpholenxeimcaels;prsoppeellritnige-s- canoyrrseilnagteled)v,amriaabklien.gThitedaipffipcroualtchtousaettdrihbeurteewaausntoiquineversotliegattoe d from sound consistency; semantic factors such as imageability, the simultaneous influence of multiple reading-related varia- h measuresoforthographic, or phonological complexity; and others. bles, with the critical difference that the factors were ttp s Effects of these lexical properties on specific neural systems, decorrelated from each other by careful stimulus selection. ://a c however, are poorly understood, partially because high intercorre- Thisapproach was used toinvestigate the neural systems that a d lations among lexical factors make it difficult to determine if they supportorthographic,phonological,andsemanticprocessesin em have independent effects. We addressed this problem by reading aloud, using both behavioral and functional magnetic ic .o decorrelating several important lexical properties through careful resonanceimaging (fMRI) data. u p stimulus selection. Functional magnetic resonance imaging data A major advantage of ensuring that the lexical factors of .c o revealed distributed neural systems for mapping orthography interest are uncorrelated is that any spatial overlap of brain m /c directlytophonology,involvingleftsupramarginal,posteriormiddle activation across factors is attributable to a shared neural e rc temporal, and fusiform gyri. Distinct from these were areas substrate, rather than a statistical correlation between factors. o rgeyflruesc/tiinnfgerisoer-mteamntpicoraplrosuclecsussin,gb,ilaitnecrlauldiannggulleafrtgmyriudsd,leantdemprpeocrua-l Sainxdfascetmoarsntoifcpparroticceuslsaersrewleevraendceectoorroerltahtoegdrainphtihci,spshtiomnuolluosgisceatl,: r/article neus/posterior cingulate. Left inferior frontal regions generally word frequency, spelling--sound consistency, imageability, -a b showed increased activation with greater task load, suggesting bigram frequency, biphone frequency, and length in letters. stra a more general role in attention, working memory, and executive Below,wedescribeeachfactor,itspossiblerelevancetoreading c processes. These data offer the first clear evidence, in a single aloud, and relevant behavioral and functional neuroimaging t/20 study,fortheseparateneuralcorrelatesoforthography--phonology evidence. /8/1 mappingand semantic access during readingaloud. 79 9 /3 Keywords:language, orthography, phonology, reading, semantics WordFrequency 98 1 Reading aloud is strongly influenced by the frequency with 1 7 whichwordsareencounteredinthelanguage.Low-frequency b y wordselicitlongerprocessingtimesandhighererrorratesthan g Introduction high-frequencywords.Thispatternobtainsnotonlyforreading ue s Reading single words aloud is usually construed as involving aloud (Monsell 1991) but with an even greater effect size for t o n the computation of a target phonological code from ortho- lexical decision (Forster and Chambers 1973; Schilling et al. 1 2 graphic(visual)input.Thephonologicalcodeistranslatedinto 1998;Balotaetal.2004)andpicturenaming(Huttenlocherand A p tphreonusenqcuiaetniocne. oSfemaratnictiuclat(omryeangeinsgtu)reisnfothrmatatuionndermlieayovaelsrot Ktiounbicoefkt1h9is83e;ffHeecntnaecsrsoesysantadskKsirissnerrel1e9va9n9t).bTehceaugseeneleraxliiczaa-l ril 20 1 contribute to this computation to some degree, particularly decision does not require a speech response, and picture 9 in cases where spelling--sound correspondences are unusual naming involves nonverbal input. Thus, although frequency (e.g., ‘‘yacht’’; Strain et al. 1995; Plaut et al. 1996). Numerous effectsmayalsobepresentatthelevelofovertarticulationor behavioralstudiesandafewfunctionalneuroimagingstudiesof visual encoding of letters, neither is necessary to elicit these reading aloud have used factors such as word frequency, effects. The longer response latencies elicited by reading low- spelling--sound consistency, and imageability to investigate frequencywordsmayarisefrommultiplesources,oneofwhich components of the reading process. In general, 2 main is the relative difficulty of mapping from orthography to approaches have been used: 1) a factorial approach in which phonology (fora reviewseeMonsell1991). 1 or 2 of these factors are manipulated while holding a few Several functional neuroimaging studies of single-word otherrelevantfactorsconstantand2)acorrelationalapproach reading have reported increased activation for low- compared (e.g., multiple linear regression) in which the influence of with high-frequency words in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) severalvariablesisinvestigatedatonce.Thefactorialapproach andanteriorinsulabilaterally(moreconsistentlyontheleft),in suffers from the limitation that investigation of only 1 or 2 the supplementary motor area (SMA), and in various left variables at a time can at best yield a partial picture of the temporal regions (Fiez et al. 1999; Joubert et al. 2004; (cid:1)TheAuthors2009.PublishedbyOxfordUniversityPress. ThisisanOpenAccessarticledistributedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttributionNon-CommercialLicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.5),whichpermits unrestrictednon-commercialuse,distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalworkisproperlycited. Kronbichler et al. 2004; Hauk, Davis, and Pulvermu¨ller 2008). itates performance on semantic decision tasks (e.g., deciding if Someofthelefttemporallobefocihavebeeninorneartheso- a word denotes an object belonging to a particular conceptual calledvisualword-formarea(VWFA;Cohenetal.2000).Word- category), suggesting that semantic information is more easily frequencyeffectsintheVWFAhavebeentakenasevidencefor available for high-frequency words (Monsell et al. 1989; Chee whole-word orthographic processing (i.e., activation of an et al. 2002). In word-association tasks, higher-frequency words orthographic lexicon) in this region (Joubert et al. 2004; are more likely to be produced as associates, suggesting that Kronbichler et al. 2004; Hauk, Davis, and Pulvermu¨ller 2008), they have stronger associative connections with other words whereas frequency effects in the IFG have generally been (NelsonandMcEvoy2000).Althoughsuchstudiesmightleadto interpreted as evidence of phonological processing in this the expectation of greater activation in semantic processing region(Bookheimer 2002). areas for high-frequency words, only 2 prior imaging studies It is important to note, however, that caution is required havereportedsuchapattern(Prabhakaranetal.2006;Carreiras when interpreting brain activations that are accompanied by et al. 2009). In general, reading studies that directly compared increases in task difficulty. Functional neuroimaging measure- high- and low-frequency words showed no relative activation D ments are very sensitive to differences in response time (RT), for the high-frequency condition (Fiez et al. 1999; Chee et al. o w accuracy, attention, working memory load, and level of effort 2002;Fiebachetal.2002;Joubertetal.2004;Kronbichleretal. nlo between tasks (see, e.g., Honey et al. 2000; Adler et al. 2001; 2004;Carreirasetal.2006;Hauk,Davis,andPulvermu¨ller2008). ad e Braveretal.2001;UllspergerandYvesvonCramon2001;Gould Thus, although it may be reasonable to expect activation for d et al. 2003; Binder et al. 2004; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; high-frequency words in brain areas that support semantic fro m Mitchell 2005; Desai etal. 2006; Lehmann etal. 2006; Tregallas processing, support for this from functional neuroimaging is h et al. 2006). These differences in task difficulty are thought to scarce. ttp s exert effects on brain activity by modulating domain-general ://a cognitive processes necessary for completing any task. Likely Spelling--SoundConsistency cad examples of such domain-general systems include a sustained e Spelling--sound consistency is another factor that affects the m attention network for maintaining arousal, a selective attention ic systemforfocusingneuralresourcesonaparticularmodalityor orthography--phonology computation. ‘‘Friends’’ of a word .ou share the same rime pronunciation, whereas ‘‘enemies’’ of p sensory object in the environment (e.g., a visual display), .c awordhavearimethatisspelledthesamebutispronounced o a working memory system for keeping task instructions and m task-relevant sensory representations accessible, a response- differently. Ingeneral, words withinconsistent spelling--sound /ce selection mechanism for mapping the contents of working mappings (e.g., PINT has many enemies such as MINT, HINT, rco memory to a response, a response-inhibition system for LINT,butnofriends)elicitlongernaminglatenciesthanwords r/a preventingprematureorprepotentresponsesfrombeingmade with consistent spelling--sound mapping (Baron and Strawson rtic 1976; Glushko 1979; Andrews 1982; Taraban and McClelland le inerror,andanerror-monitoringsystem for adjustingresponse -a 1987; Jared 1997, 2002). These effects are greater for lower- b criteriaand RT deadlinestominimize sucherrors. If this isthe s cgaesnee,raanldtasifkthdeemleavnedlso,fthaecntiviattfioonlloiwnsththesaet ascystitveamtisondecpaennndsevoenr f1r9e8q4u)e.ncCyomwpourdtastiocnoamlpamreoddewlsithofhirgehaedring(Seaidnednbaesrsgoceiatteadl. tract/2 behavioralevidencesuggestthatwhenawordisusedrelatively 0 beattributedwithcertaintytospecificlinguisticprocesseswhen /8 infrequently and the mapping between spelling and sound is /1 thisactivationhasresultedfromacontrastinwhichgeneraltask 7 highly atypical, semantic information is used to help achieve 9 demands differ. Specifically, brain areas whose task-related the correct phonological representation (Strain et al. 1995; 9/3 activation is thought to reflect attention and working-memory Plaut et al. 1996; Strain and Herdman 1999; Harm and 981 demands (e.g., SMA, anterior cingulate, IFG, anterior insula, 1 Seidenberg 2004; Woollams 2005). Hence, brain activation 7 precentralsulcus, and dorsal parietallobe;Corbetta etal. 1998; b elicited by reading low-frequency, low-consistency words y Carter et al. 1999; LaBar et al. 1999; Duncan and Owen 2000; g might be interpreted in 2 ways. It may reflect increased use u Derrfuss et al. 2005; Grosbras et al. 2005; Owen et al. 2005) e largely overlap those whose activation increases for low- of neural resources for orthography--phonology mapping for st o low-frequency, low-consistency words, or, alternatively, re- n compared with high-frequency words (Fiez et al. 1999; Hauk, 1 cruitment of the semantic system. Several studies have 2 Davis,andPulvermu¨ller2008).Moreover,thesesameareasshow A reported activation in language-related prefrontal cortical p a(Bctinivdaetiro,nMceodrlreerl,ateedt wali.th20in0c5r)e.asHeesnicneR,Tneduurrainlgerffeeacdtisngoaf,lofuodr regions such as IFG for reading inconsistent compared with ril 20 consistentwords(Herbsteretal.1997;Fiezetal.1999;Binder, 1 example, decreasing word frequency may be indistinguishable 9 Medler, et al. 2005; Mechelli et al. 2005), and such results are fromattentionorworking-memoryprocessestotheextentthat typically interpreted as reflecting neural systems for orthogra- theyspatiallyoverlapwitheffectsofRT. phy--phonology mapping. However, because low-frequency, Althoughthefocusinneuroimagingstudieshasbeenonthe low-consistencywordsalsoelicitlongernaminglatencies,such greater activation associated with decreasing word frequency, activations may be confounded with attention and executive increasing word frequency may also be expected to have processes, as described above (Binder, Medler, et al. 2005). positive effects on activation, particularly in lexical--semantic Functional imaging evidence for recruitment of semantic systems. Word frequency is correlated with the extent of processing areas in reading low-frequency, low-consistency repeated exposure to a lexical concept; thus, access to word words, onthe otherhand,is scarce. meaning is likely to be more extensive and more automatic in the case of high-frequency words. Higher-frequency words appearinmorecontexts(Adelmanetal.2006)andarejudgedto Imageability be more familiar (Toglia and Battig 1978; Baayen et al. 2006) Imageability,whichreferstotheeasewithwhichawordevokes compared with lower-frequency words. Word frequency facil- a mental image, is a semantic factor that facilitates word 1800 NeuralSystemsforReadingAloud d Gravesetal. recognition in the lexical decision task (James 1975; Kroll and Monselletal.1989;Strainetal.1995;Jared1997;Weekes1997; Merves 1986; Kounios and Holcomb 1994; Binder, Westbury, HinoandLupker2000;Jared2002;O’MalleyandBesner2008). etal.2005).Highlyimageablewordsarethoughttohavearicher The study of bigram effects in the functional neuroimaging ormoreeasilyaccessedsemanticrepresentation(Shallice1988; literaturehasbeenmotivatedbythetheorythatfamiliarletter Paivio1991;Schwanenflugel1991).Althoughsemanticprocess- combinations (e.g., bigrams and trigrams) are represented in ingisusuallyconsideredtohaveaminimalroleinreadingaloud, the brain, particularly in VWFA, with more frequently thereisevidencethatitplaysarole,particularlyforlessfamiliar, encountered combinations having stronger representations more difficult words (Patterson et al. 1985; Patterson and (Dehaene et al. 2005; Binder et al. 2006; Vinckier et al. 2007). Hodges 1992; Strain et al. 1995; Strain and Herdman 1999; Activationofthesesublexicalorthographiccodesspeedsletter Woollams 2005). According to connectionist models of word recognition, perhaps through an interactive activation mech- reading,ifbothwordfrequencyandspelling--soundconsistency anism (McClelland and Rumelhart 1981), and damage to their are low, the orthography--phonology computation is both less neural representations results in letter-by-letter reading accurateandlessefficient.Computingthecorrectphonological (Binder and Mohr 1992; Leff et al. 2001; Cohen, Henry, et al. D code requires additional input via the orthography--semantics-- 2004). Relevant functional brain-imaging data for bigram o w phonology pathway in such models (Plaut et al. 1996). Input frequencyeffectscomesfrom2recentstudiesusingnonwords, nlo along this semantically mediated pathway is greater for words bothofwhichfoundincreasedactivitywithincreasingbigram ad e that are highly imageable. This theory predicts faster latencies frequency in left mid-fusiform gyrus (Binder et al. 2006; d for low-frequency, low-consistency words that are highly Vinckieret al. 2007). fro m imageable compared with those that are less imageable. This h pattern has been observed in multiple studies of reading aloud ttp BiphoneFrequency andLetter Length s (Strain et al. 1995; Strain and Herdman 1999; Shibahara et al. Biphonefrequencyandletterlengthwereincludedprimarilyto ://a 2003;Woollams2005). ensure that effects of the other variables could not be cad Severalfunctionalneuroimagingstudiesofimageabilityhave e attributed to correlations with these factors. Biphone fre- m also been performed. In a single-word reading aloud study ic similartotheoneperformedhere,Binder,Medler,etal.(2005) quencyisameasureofphonotacticcomplexityshowntoaffect .ou speech production tasks such as nonword pronunciation p foundincreasedactivationinbilateralangular,superiorfrontal, .c (Majerus et al. 2002; Goldrick and Larson 2008; Graves et al. o and precuneus/posterior cingulate gyri as word imageability m increased.Activationsforlower-imageabilitywordswerefound 2008) and picture naming (Vitevitch et al. 2004), with low- /ce inbilateralanteriorcingulatecortex,leftprecentralgyrus,and biphone-frequency words showing a processing disadvantage rco left IFG/anterior insula, similar to the activations reported for comparedwithhigh-biphone-frequencywords.Effectsofletter r/a lower-frequency words in the studies mentioned previously. length (number of letters) could in principle also relate to rtic phonotactic processing in that longer words may be more le Similar inferior frontal activations for low- relative to high- -a difficult to pronounce. Given that the stimuli in the present b imageabilitywordshavebeenreportedinseveralstudiesusing s lFerxieicdaelricainedt asle.m20an0t0i;cFideebcaicshionandtaFsrkisede(Priecrian20i0e3t; Naol.pp1e9n9e9y; eexxppeercitmleentttera-rleenagllthmeofnfeocstysllatboica,rihsoewpervimera,riitlyisatretahseonlaebvelel toof tract/2 visual encoding, as suggested by functional neuroimaging 0 and Price 2004; Binder, Westbury, et al. 2005; Sabsevitz et al. /8 studies showing increased activation in primary visual cortex /1 2005). Several reading-related studies also reported activation 7 forlonger words (Mechelli et al. 2000;Wydellet al. 2003). 9 for high-imageability words in precuneus/posterior cingulate 9/3 andangulargyrus(Binder,Westbury,etal.2005;Sabsevitzetal. 98 2005; Bedny and Thompson-Schill 2006), although exceptions Aimsofthe Study 11 7 to this pattern have also been reported (Pexman et al. 2007; Theprincipalaimofthepresentstudywastoexamineinmore b y Hauk,Davis,Kherif,andPulvermu¨ller2008).Onbalance,these detail the neural systems supporting reading aloud. One g u findings suggest that precuneus/posterior cingulate and unresolved issue is whether there are regions in the IFG that e s angular gyrus play a prominent role in processing word are specifically modulated by word frequency, spelling--sound t o n meaning, a notion further supported by a recent large-scale consistency, or imageability. IFG activation has been reported 1 2 meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of word- for low values of all of these variables (i.e., more difficult A p relatedsemantics (Binder etal. 2009). ccoonndcuitriorennst)l,yalttohoaussgehssnothsetuddeigerseheaovef oevxearmlaipneodfatlhle3avcatriivaabtleeds ril 20 1 regions. Similar IFG regions are also modulated by response 9 BigramFrequency latency, suggesting that at least some of this activation may A final primary factor of interest, bigram frequency, was represent general executive and attention processes modu- included as a measure of orthographic familiarity. One of the lated by task difficulty. We predicted that common brain few behavioral studies to examine the impact of bigram regions would be modulated by task difficulty across all 3 frequency on reading aloud found no effect (Strain and lexicalvariables,aswellasbyRT,andthatthesewouldinclude Herdman 1999). Bigram frequency has, however, been shown areas previously identified with attention, working memory, toaffectotherreadingphenomenasuchastachistoscopicword and other general executive processes. In addition, there may and letter perception (Biederman 1966; Broadbent and be areas of IFG overlap restricted to the word frequency and Gregory 1968; Rumelhart and Siple 1974; Rice and Robinson consistencyvariables,whichwouldsuggestamorespecificrole 1975; Binder et al. 2006) and lexical decision (Gernsbacher forthese areas inorthography-to-phonology mapping. 1984;WestburyandBuchanan2002),leadingmanyresearchers A second issue concerns activation of the semantic system. to control for this variable in studies of reading aloud (e.g., Current neuroimaging evidence for engagement of the Waters and Seidenberg 1985; Taraban and McClelland 1987; semantic system during reading aloud is limited (Binder, CerebralCortexAugust2010,V20N8 1801 Medler,etal.2005),despitemodelingandbehavioralevidence additional monosyllabic words in the Seidenberg and McClelland thatsemanticinformationplaysaroleinthistask.Wepredicted (1989)listinordertodecorrelatethesampleintermsofbigramand 2 effects concerning the semantic system. First, increasing biphone frequency. This enlarged the sample to 465 words. Two correlation matrices are given in Table 2. The upper half presents values of word frequency and imageability were expected to correlation valuesamongthe 6variablesofinterestacrossthe1650 produce increasing activation in semantic regions such as the words in the starting corpus. The lower half presents correlation angular gyrus, ventral temporal lobe, and precuneus/posterior valuesamongthesamevariablesacrossthefinalsetof465words.A cingulate, indicating more extensive activation of semantic list of the 465 stimuli and their associated values is given in the information with increasing concept familiarity and image- supplementalmaterial(TableS1). ability. Second, we expected increased activation in the semantic system as spelling--sound consistency decreases. Participants Although the predicted effects of frequency and imageability The20participants(13females)wereallhealthy,literateadultswith could be attributed to incidental processing of semantic normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were right handed on the information, semantic activation associated with decreasing Edinburgh handedness inventory (Oldfield 1971), and spoke English D consistencywouldbestrongevidenceforadirectcontribution as a first language. Mean age was 23.2 (standard deviation, SD: 3.4), o w fromsemantics inreading aloud. afrnodmmtehaenyWeaercshoslfeerduTceasttioonfwAadsu1lt6.6Re(SaDdi:n3g.1()W.AecvhersblearlI2Q00e1st)imwataes nloa availablefor19ofthe20participants,withameanstandardscoreof de Materials and Methods 1ac0c9o.6rdi(nSgDt:o8lo.3c)a.lPInasrttiitcuiptiaonntaslRpervoiveiwdeBdoawrdripttreontocinoflosramneddwecroenpseanidt d fro m STthiemsutlimusulMiwateerreia46l5monosyllabicEnglishwords,whichwereselected anhourlystipend. https toensurethatthefollowing6factorswereuncorrelated:letterlength, TaskandImaging ://a word frequency, spelling--sound consistency, imageability, bigram ThefMRIexperimentusedafastevent-relateddesignwithcontinuous ca frequency,andbiphonefrequency(seeTable1forextremeexamples). acquisition. On each trial, a word was displayed for 1000 ms, then de m Word-frequencyvalueswereobtainedfromCELEX(Baayenetal.1995) replaced with a fixation cross. Approximate horizontal viewing angle ic in terms of occurrences per million and log transformed. Consistency subtendedlessthan7(cid:2).Participantswereinstructedto‘‘readeachword .o u wCoamsdpeafirinsoendsawsethreenbuasmedbeornofphfroiennedtiscmtriannussctrhipetinonums fbreormofC‘E‘eLnEeXmitehsa.’t’ aMloRuI-dcoamsqpuaitcibklleymanicdroacpchuornaeteplylaacsedponsesaibrlteh.’e’Pmaortuicthipaanndtsssepcoukreedintotothane p.co were transformed, when necessary, into standard American English m head coil. The fMRI session included 5 runs of single-word reading /c pstrroaninuendc.iaFtoiornesa.chBi2g-rlaemtterfrceoqmuebnincaietisonwienreawleonrgdt,htheanfdreqpuoesnitcioiensocfoanl-l atrloiaulsd.wEearcehrraunndloamstleydi8ntmerimniaxneddcwointhsist1e3d9obfa9s3elirneead(ifinxgattrioianls);tthrieaslse, erco words of the same length containing the same bigram in the same resultinginavariableintertrialintervalrangingfrom2to34s(mean: r/a positionweresummedandlogtransformed.Aftercalculatingthisvalue 4.9,SD:3.72).Followingthiswere5runsofpseudowordreadingaloud. rtic fdoivrideeadchbybtighreamtotianlntuhmebwerorodf,btihgerasmesfiignurtehsewweorredttohegnivesuammmeeadnlaongd- Twhilelnposteubdeodwisocrudssdeadtafuwrtehreer.not included in the current analyses and le-ab transformed positional bigram frequency. The same procedure was MRI data were acquired using a 3.0-T GE Excite system (GE stra pbieprhfoornmeefdreqfourenbciyp.hCoonmespatroedywieiltdh umnecaonnstloragi-nteradnsbfioprhmoende fpreoqsiutieonncayl Hfreeqaluthencacrye,reWceauivkeeshcoa,il.WHI)ighwitrhesoalnutio8n-c,haTn1n-weleigarhrtaeyd haenaadtomraidciaol ct/20 (cf.VitevitchandLuce2004),thismethodismorepredictiveofreaction referenceimageswereacquiredasasetof134contiguousaxialslices /8 timeinanauditorypseudowordrepetitiontask(Gravesetal.2008). /1 (0.938 30.9383 1.000mm)usinga spoiled-gradient-echosequence. 7 Imageability values were obtained from a database of imageability 9 Functional scans were acquired using a gradient-echo echoplanar 9 ratingscompiledfrom6sources(Paivioet al.1968;TogliaandBattig imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters: 25-ms time /39 1978;GilhoolyandLogie1980;Birdetal.2001;ClarkandPaivio2004; 8 echo,2-stimerepetition,192-mmfieldofview,64-364-pixelmatrix, 1 Cortese and Fugett 2004), the first 3 available through the MRC 1 in-planevoxeldimensions3.033.0mm,andslicethicknessof2.5mm 7 PsycholinguisticDatabase(Wilson1988). with a 0.5-mmgap.Thirty-twointerleavedaxialslices wereacquired, by Stimulusselection beganwithacorpus of1650 nonhomographic, andeachofthe5functionalrunsconsistedof240whole-brainimage g u monosyllabic words containing 4--6 letters, a subset of the mono- volumes. es syllabic words used by Seidenberg and McClelland (1989). This t o corpuswasdividedinto8orthogonalcellsbyfullycrossinghighand n 1 lowlevelsofwordfrequency,consistency,andimageability.Thecell 2 with the smallestnumberofitems, the low-frequency/inconsistent/ Table2 A hseiglehc-timedagaetarbainlidtyomceflrlo,mcotnhteaiontehder378cwelolsr,dysi.elNdeinxgt,a3n8ucilteeumssofw3e0r4e Cfoorrrwelhaitcihondmataatroixn(arllvafalucetosr)sfowrefarectaovrasiloafbilnete(urepsptearchraolsf)satnhdeifnoirtiathlese4t6o5fs1t6im50ulcuasnwdiodradtse(wloowrdesr pril 20 words for which frequency, consistency, and imageability were half) 19 uncorrelated. Finally, words were selected from a list of 895 Length Frequency Consistency Imageability Bigrams Biphones Correlationsacrossnearlytheentiresourcecorpus Length 1 Table1 Frequency (cid:1)0.074 1 Examplewordsfromextremeendsofthedistribution,alongwithoverallrangeandmeanvalues Consistency (cid:1)0.166 (cid:1)0.106 1 foreachstimuluspropertyofinterest Imageability (cid:1)0.019 (cid:1)0.023 0.087 1 Bigrams (cid:1)0.178 0.407 0.031 (cid:1)0.053 1 Property High Low Max Min Mean Biphones 0.156 0.127 0.110 (cid:1)0.024 0.195 1 Correlationsacrossthestimulusset Length(letters) Straight(8) Fix(3) 8 3 4.42 Length 1 Wordfrequency(log[count/million]) Might(4.12) Wilt(0.00) 4.72 0 2.35 Frequency 0.057 1 Consistency(friends--enemies) Chill(27) Wash((cid:1)20) 29 (cid:1)20 7.35 Consistency (cid:1)0.069 (cid:1)0.088 1 Imageability(ratingon1--7scale) Beach(6.59) Moot(1.40) 6.59 1.4 4.39 Imageability (cid:1)0.032 0.086 0.043 1 Bigramfrequency(log[count/million]) Thing(5.01) Gym(1.85) 5.01 1.84 4.15 Bigrams (cid:1)0.072 0.090 0.078 (cid:1)0.087 1 Biphonefrequency(log[count/million]) Cyst(34.47) Apt(0.95) 34.47 0 9.05 Biphones 0.072 0.037 0.090 (cid:1)0.025 (cid:1)0.003 1 Note:Numbersinparenthesesaftereachexampleitemrepresentthevariablevalueforthatitem. Note:Allnonsignificantcorrelations(P[0.05)areinbold. 1802 NeuralSystemsforReadingAloud d Gravesetal. DataProcessingandAnalysis were very infrequent (2.6% overall) and not analyzed further. Image analysis was performed using AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov/ Using simultaneous multiple linear regression analysis for afni)(Cox1996).Foreachsubject,thefirst6imagesineachtimeseries which the dependent measure was each subject’s mean- were discarded prior to regression analysis to avoid initial saturation centered RT to each word, we examined effects of the 6 effects.Imageswereslicetimingcorrectedandspatiallycoregistered. variables of interest, as well as interactions of word frequency Estimatesofthe3translationand3rotationmovementsateachtime point, computed during registration, were saved for use as noise with letter length, consistency, bigram frequency, and image- covariates. Image volumes containing artifacts were identified using ability,foreffectsonRT.TheoverallmeanRTwas588ms(SD: AFNI’s 3dToutcount program and subsequently removed from the 123). Unique variance wasexplained by letter length (b= 7.3, analysis. A local Pearson correlation algorithm (Saad et al. 2009) was P < 0.0001), frequency (b = –22.6, P < 0.05), and consistency used to align each T1-weighted structural volume to the same EPI (b=–0.4,P <0.05).Asexpected,thedirectionsoftheseeffects referencevolumethatwasusedtoalignthefunctionalscans. weresuchthatwordswithmoreletters,lowerfrequency,and Audio recordings of the reading responses were processed using moreenemieswereassociatedwithlongerlatencies.Noother acombinationofafreelyavailablecorrelation-basednoisesubtraction algorithm (Cusack et al. 2005) and custom software developed in- main effects or interactions were significant. In addition to D house. This approach suppressed scanner noise while leaving the includingallvariablesofinterestinthesameregressionanalysis o w speech signal intact and automatically paired reading responses with to determine the unique effects of each variable, it is also nlo stimulus onset markers. RTs were calculated from stimulus onset to informativetoexamineseparatepairwisecorrelationsbetween ad responseonset.Valuesmorethan2SDsfromeachsubject’sownmean e eachvariableandRT.Thismethodbroadlyagreedwiththefull d wauedrietocrhyeicnkspedecatniodn,wofhtehnenaeucdeisosafirlye,.mReasnpuoanllsyedsewteerrmeicnoendsibdyerveisduearlraonrds regression model, with letter length, word frequency, and from if the subject stuttered, produced a mispronunciation, failed to consistency showing reliable correlations with RT. They h respond, or responded with an RT more than 3SDs from the group differed only in that imageability showed a reliable pairwise ttp s mean.TheseRTs,calculatedforeachitemrespondedtoindividuallyfor correlationwithRTbutdidnotexplainuniquevarianceinthe ://a eachsubject,wereusedasacovariableinthefMRIregressionanalysis. fullmodel.Pairwisecorrelationswereasfollows:letterlength, ca Voxelwise multiple linear regression was performed using 3dDe- d r = 0.174, P < 0.001; word frequency, r = –0.193, P < 0.0001; e convolve(Ward2006).Thisanalysisincludedthefollowingcovariables m otrfenndos,itnhteer6espt:reaviofouuslrythc-aolrcduelratepdolmynootimonialpatroammeotedresl,alonwd-afreteqrumenfcoyr c0o.0n5s;isbtiegnracmy,sr,r==–0–0.0.09023,6P,P< 0>.005.0;5im;aangdeabbiiplihtyo,nres=,r–0=.0–907.0,2P54<, ic.ou p signal in the ventricles used to model noise. Covariables of interest P >0.05. .c o were modeled using a gamma variate estimate of the hemodynamic m response function and consisted of the following 14 terms: binary /c e variablesfor1)successfulreadingaloudtrialsand2)trialsinwhichthe Imaging rco subject made an erroneous response; continuous, mean-centered The general contrast of all successful reading responses r/a v7a)luimesagfoerab3il)itRy,T8,)4)bilgertatemr lferenqguthe,n5c)y,waonrdd9f)rebqiupehnocnye, 6fr)eqcuoennsicsyte;nacnyd, compared with fixation baseline (left side of Fig. 1) revealed rticle 10--14)interactiontermsofinterest.Interactiontermsincludedinthe activation in the standard overt reading network as seen in -a b modelwerethosethatwepredictedtohaveaneffectonbrainactivity numerous prior studies (e.g., Fiez and Petersen 1998; Turkel- s (interactionsofwordfrequencywithletter-length,consistency,bigram taub et al. 2002). Activation was observed bilaterally in peri- trac frequency, and imageability). Detailed consideration of these inter- Rolandiccortices(preandpostcentralgyri),inferiorfrontaland t/2 actions,however,isbeyondthescopeofthecurrentreportandwillbe insularcortex,superiortemporalgyri,SMA,anddorsalanterior 0/8 presentedinasubsequentarticle. /1 cingulatecortex,intraparietalsulci(IPS),occipitalandinferior 7 The resulting coefficient maps for each participant were linearly 9 resampledinstandardstereotaxicspacetoavoxelsizeof1mm3and occipito-temporalcortices,aswellassubcorticalnucleisuchas 9/3 spatially smoothed with a 6-mm full-width-half-maximum (FWHM) thethalamus.Therewasnoobviouslateralizationofactivation. 98 Gaussiankernel.Thesesmoothedcoefficientmapswerethenpassedto Areas showing greater activation for fixation than successful 11 7 arandomeffectsanalysiscomparingthecoefficientvalueswithanull responses included several areas often reported to show task- b hypothesis mean of zero across participants. The resulting group induceddeactivation,suchasbilateralprecuneusandposterior y g activationmapswerethresholdedatavoxelwiseP <0.01,uncorrected. u cingulate gyri, bilateral ventromedial prefrontal cortex, left e ApercfloursmterM-eoxntteenCtatrhloressihmouldlatwioanssethsteimnactianlcgutlhaetepdroubsainbgilitAylpofhaspSiamtialtloy angular gyrus, and right parahippocampus. Table 3 gives st on contiguous voxels for a range of alpha values. Input arguments to a complete list of coordinates for activation maxima, where 1 2 AlphaSiminclude1)thevoxelwisethreshold(P <0.01inthiscase),2) positive z-scores represent areas activated for reading aloud A aclculsutsetresracroencnoencstiidoenreradddiuisstisnpcetc(ifhyeinregrth=e4m.2in4immumm,tdhiestdainacgeonfoarllwenhgicthh accotmivpaatereddfowritvhiefiwxiantgiofinxaantidonnecgoamtipvaerezd-scwoirtehsrienaddicinagteaaloreuads. pril 20 along the face of a single voxel), and 3) the level of smoothing. 1 Coordinates are listed for extreme maxima (in the case of 9 ConsideringthefactthatrawMRIdatacontainadegreeofsmoothness positive values) or extreme minima (for negative values) that introduced, for example, during image reconstruction from k-space (Friedman et al. 2006), the actual smoothness of the images was areatleast30mmapartandappearwithinsignificantclusters. calculatedfromerrorresidualsusingtheAFNIprogram3dFWHMx.The With the exception of the contrast between successful resultingFWHMvalues(inmm)in3directionsofx=9.2,y=9.1,and readingaloudandfixation,allotherresultsarefromanalysesof z=7.7wereinputtoAlphaSim.Thisresultedintheremovalofclusters continuouscovariables.Resultsoftheseanalysesaredescribed smaller than 2052 lL (76 contiguous voxels in the original image in terms of correlations between each regressor and blood space),forawhole-braincorrectedprobabilitythresholdofP <0.05. oxygen level--dependent (BOLD) signal, not activation in comparison with a baseline. In Figures 1 and 2, hot colors Results indicateareaswhereBOLDsignalcorrelatedpositivelywiththe covariable, and cool colors indicate areas where BOLD signal Performance correlated negatively with the covariable (i.e., greater BOLD Errors in reading aloud (mispronunciations, false starts, signal for decreasing values of the covariable). For RT (right omissions, and latencies greater than 3SDs from the mean) side of Fig. 1), the only significant effects were increases in CerebralCortexAugust2010,V20N8 1803 D o w n lo a d e d fro m Figure1. Areasofsignificantactivationforsuccessfulreadingaloudresponsescomparedwithfixationbaseline(leftsideoffigure)andareasofactivationpositivelycorrelated h withreactiontime(rightsideoffigure).Noareaswerenegativelycorrelatedwithreactiontime. ttp s ://a c a d Effects were obtained for each of the 6 stimulus properties e Table3 m Peakpoints(positiveornegativeextremes)withinsignificantlyactivatedclustersshowing of interest. Correlations for the 4 primary factors (word ic asignificantmaineffectofeithersuccessfulreadingaloudtrialscomparedwithfixation(upper frequency, spelling--sound consistency, imageability, and .ou p rows)oraparametriceffectofRT(lowerrows) bigram frequency) are shown in Figure 2, complete list of .c o cluster maxima is in Table 4. Positive correlations between m Locations x y z Z-score BOLDsignalandletterlengthwereobservedinbilateralmedial, /ce SucLcPersescfuelntrreaaldginygruasloud[fixation (cid:1)44 (cid:1)14 35 7.18 ventral, and polar occipital cortices. The signal in left para- rco LInferioroccipito-temporal (cid:1)33 (cid:1)60 (cid:1)22 6.96 hippocampus was negatively correlated with length. See the r/a RPrecentralgyrus 43 (cid:1)10 33 6.83 supplementary figure fora map of thesecorrelations. rtic RAnteriorinsula 42 5 (cid:1)2 6.67 le RLateraloccipital 33 (cid:1)87 (cid:1)9 6.28 Positive correlations for word frequency occurred in bi- -a b RThalamus 13 (cid:1)28 (cid:1)5 6.22 lateralangulargyri;bilateralposteriorcingulategyri,subparietal s LLLCTAhenarteelabrmieolrulussmuperiortemporalgyrus (cid:1)(cid:1)(cid:1)114346 (cid:1)(cid:1)21496 (cid:1)(cid:1)12666 655...187237 scuolrcruesla,tainodnsprfeocruwneourds;afrnedquleefntcsuyp(ei.reio.,rinfrcornetaaslinsuglcBuOs.LNDegsiagtnivael tract/2 0 LAnteriorCingulate (cid:1)13 6 40 5.70 intensity for lower-frequency words) were found in left IFJ, /8 LSuperiortemporalgyrus (cid:1)61 (cid:1)28 5 5.53 /1 LSupramarginalgyrus (cid:1)30 (cid:1)43 25 5.02 IFG,anteriorinsula,IPS,andsubgenualcingulate,andbilaterally 79 RRLPISnluatrpnauepmraiorripeottealamlrsepuolrcaulssulcus (cid:1)333602 (cid:1)(cid:1)255783 (cid:1)42122 444...944461 itnemSMpoAr,atlhcaolarmteuxs,(lmefetdi>alriogchcti)p.italcortex,andventraloccipito- 9/3981 RCuneus 2 (cid:1)90 19 4.28 Correlations with spelling-to-sound consistency (number of 1 7 RInferiorfrontalsulcus 25 31 20 4.22 friends minus number of enemies) were all in the negative b RIFG,parstriangularis 55 25 19 3.85 direction, indicating increasing neural activity for words with y g Llateraloccipitalcortex (cid:1)29 (cid:1)92 12 3.80 u LMTG (cid:1)60 (cid:1)61 8 3.26 more enemies than friends. These areas were all in the left es LMiddlefrontalgyrus (cid:1)47 41 21 2.99 hemisphere and included IFJ, IFG/anterior insula and cortex t o Fixation[successfulreadingaloud n RAnteriorcingulate 2 42 (cid:1)1 (cid:1)5.10 along the inferior temporal sulcus (ITS) and middle temporal 12 RSubgenualcingulate 1 7 (cid:1)1 (cid:1)5.02 gyrus (MTG). A RLAPnogstuelarirorgycrinugsulate (cid:1)430 (cid:1)(cid:1)5722 3320 (cid:1)(cid:1)53..1760 broImadalgyesaibmiliiltayrwtoasthasossoecioaftewdowrdithfrecqourreenlactye,dthaocutigvhitythcehaanrgeeass pril 20 1 Note:CoordinatescorrespondtotheatlasspaceofTalairachandTournoux(1988).L5leftand modulated by imageability were less extensive. Activity 9 R5right. positively correlated with imageability occurred in bilateral angular gyri and bilateral precuneus. Areas negatively corre- lated with imageability included left IFJ and left lateral and activity with increasing RT (i.e., positive correlations). These ventral occipital cortex, spreading to the posterior inferior includedbroadlysimilarpatternsinleftandrighthemispheres, temporal gyrus. withsomewhatgreateractivationontheleft.Thispatternwas Bigramfrequencyelicitedonlynegativelycorrelatedactivity seenintheIFGextendingbothtothemiddlefrontalgyrusand (greater activity for words with lower bigram frequency), anteriorinsula,theinferiorfrontaljunction(IFJ,thejunctionof which occurred in bilateral posterior MTG and left supra- theprecentralandinferiorfrontalsulci),peri-Rolandiccortices, marginal gyrus. Biphone frequency was not significantly SMA, thalamus, and posterior superior temporal sulci. Exclu- correlated with activity in anybrain region. sivelyleft-sidedactivationappearedinIPS,supramarginalgyrus, We also reran the analysis with a model that included all andtemporo-occipitalsulcus.SeeTable4foracompletelistof variables exceptRTtoexaminethepossibility, asdiscussedin clusters alongwith their activation maxima andcoordinates. Wilson et al. (2009), that inclusion of an RT regressor might 1804 NeuralSystemsforReadingAloud d Gravesetal. D o w n lo a d e d fro m h ttp s ://a c a d e m ic .o u p .c o m /c e rc o r/a rtic le -a b s tra c Figure2. Brainareasshowingasignificantmaineffectofthevariouswordpropertiesofinterest.Hotcolorsrepresentpositivecorrelationsbetweenneuralactivityandthe t/2 0 regressor(e.g.,increasingactivitywithincreasingvaluesofwordfrequency),andcoolcolorsrepresentnegativecorrelations(e.g.,decreasingactivitywithincreasingvaluesof /8 wordfrequency). /1 7 9 9 /3 have distorted effects of the psycholinguistic variables with aspect of the pars orbitalis and the more dorsal in pars 98 1 which it was correlated. Results of this analysis were nearly triangularis. Several areas outside the frontal lobe also showed 1 7 identical tothat of thefull model. extensiveoverlapbetweenpositiveRTandnegativefrequency b y There was widespread spatial overlap of areas exhibiting effects.TheseincludedtheleftIPS,bilateralanteriorcingulate g u amaineffectofsuccessfulnamingandthoseshowingincreased gyrus,bilateral calcarine sulcus, andbilateral thalamus. e s activation with increasing RT. Positive effects of RT also Effectsofdecreasingwordfrequency,consistency(toasmall t o n overlapped with negative effects of word frequency, consis- extent),andincreasingRT,butnotimageability,alsooverlapin 1 2 tency,andimageability,primarilyinleftIFJ.RToverlappedwith theleftmid-fusiformgyrus(ventralsurfaceintheupperpartof A p nanetgeartiiovreincosunlsai.stIenncthyeaunpdpferreqpuaertncoyf Feifgfeucrtes3inarleeftcoImFGpoasintde Foifgv.i3su).alTwhiosradrefoarhmassbpurtevhiaosunsloytbtyepenicaimllypblieceatnedassinocpiarotecdeswsiinthg ril 20 1 maps showing effects of lexical variables that overlapped general performance effects (although RT effects in this area 9 extensivelywitheffectsofRT,raisingthepossibilitythatsome for reading aloudwerereported previously byBinder, Medler, of these effects could be related to general performance et al.2005). processes (e.g., attention, cognitive control, and working Acombinationofeffectsthatmayinvolvelexicalprocesses memory). As mentioned in the Introduction, specific lexical morespecificallyisshowninthelowerpartofFigure3.Areas andgeneraltask processes aredifficulttodisentanglebecause showingincreasingactivityforwordsofincreasingfrequency both are related to time on task. Areas of the left IFG and IFJ overlap mainly with areas showing increasing activity for havebeenimplicatedinbothtypesofprocesses,andbothareas words of increasing imageability. This overlap is seen show increased activity for reading words with decreasing primarily in bilateral angular gyri and left precuneus (light imageability,decreasingconsistency,decreasingfrequency,and green in the lower part of Fig. 3). Neither of these regions longerRT.This4-partoverlapisshowninthetoppartofFigure shows any RT effects. Similarly, posterior temporal and 3. In contrast, there were 2 regions in left IFG that showed inferior parietal areas showing increasing activity with isolated effects of decreasing spelling--sound consistency (red decreasing bigram frequency show no overlap with areas areas in Fig. 3). The more ventral of these is in the anterior modulated by RT. Finally, inferior temporal regions showing CerebralCortexAugust2010,V20N8 1805 Table4 Table4 Peakpoints(positiveornegativeextremes)withinclustersshowingsignificantcorrelationswith Continued BOLDsignalforeachoftheparametricfactorsofinterest Location Cluster x y z Z-score Location Cluster x y z Z-score of size of size extremepoint (lL) extremepoint (lL) LIFJ (cid:1)46 3 30 (cid:1)4.65 PositiveRTcorrelations Occipito-temporal 3169 Fronto-parietal 36437 LLateraloccipitalcortex (cid:1)35 (cid:1)82 (cid:1)10 (cid:1)4.16 LPrecentralgyrus (cid:1)38 (cid:1)5 37 5.29 Negativebigramfrequencycorrelations LAnteriorinsula (cid:1)33 10 (cid:1)2 4.11 RPosteriortemporal 3029 LIntraparietalsulcus (cid:1)42 (cid:1)42 47 3.89 RPosteriorMTG 64 (cid:1)62 11 (cid:1)4.17 LMiddlefrontalgyrus (cid:1)30 46 27 3.69 LPosteriortemporal 2368 Precentralandprefrontal 17382 LPosteriorMTG (cid:1)53 (cid:1)51 11 (cid:1)4.42 RIFG,parsorbitalis 43 19 (cid:1)8 4.37 Temporo-parietaljunction 2090 RPrecentralgyrus 48 (cid:1)15 36 4.37 LSupramarginalgyrus (cid:1)57 (cid:1)31 28 (cid:1)3.61 D Medialfronto-parietal 12613 o LSMA (cid:1)2 7 50 4.69 w n RCallosalsulcus 7 14 21 3.75 lo LParacentrallobule (cid:1)7 (cid:1)42 52 3.15 increasingactivityforlessconsistentwordsalsoshowlittleor ad LSuperiortemporal 5312 e SubLcPoortsictearliorsuperiortemporalsulcus 4444 (cid:1)49 (cid:1)38 3 3.98 no overlap with areas modulated by RT. d fro m RSLRuTpSheuarpiloaermriotuersmtepmorpaolralgyrus 3456 (cid:1)690 (cid:1)(cid:1)1156 (cid:1)102 33..6523 Discussion https MeRdiaClaolccacriipnietaslulcus 2745 12 (cid:1)71 8 3.48 Bothseparateandoverlappingpatternsofneuralactivitywere ://ac Ventraloccipito-temporal 2091 detected for the 6 uncorrelated factors of interest. The data ad LTemporo-occipitalsulcus (cid:1)47 (cid:1)40 (cid:1)17 3.91 suggest a neural architecture in which distinct orthography-- em PosMitievdeialletotcecripleitnaglthcorrelations 24259 phonologyandsemanticpathwaysareengagedduringreading ic.o RLingualgyrus 13 (cid:1)87 (cid:1)2 5.26 aloud. The results also clarify the role of several left inferior up NegatLivLeinlegtutaelrgleynrugsthcorrelation (cid:1)18 (cid:1)82 (cid:1)14 5.18 frontal regions in readingaloud. .com Medialtemporo-cerebellar 3565 /c PositivLeCweroerbdefllruemquencycorrelations (cid:1)11 (cid:1)41 (cid:1)13 (cid:1)4.11 Overlapping EffectsofIncreasing TaskLoad erco Medialparietal 17991 As illustrated in the upper part of Figure 3, effects in the r/a RLPPoosstteerriioorrcciinngguullaattee (cid:1)16 (cid:1)(cid:1)5259 2450 54..9233 negativedirection(relativelygreateractivityforlowerstimulus rticle RLateralparietal 14722 property values) for word frequency, consistency, and image- -a RAngulargyrus 45 (cid:1)63 39 5.42 b ability all overlapped with positive RT effects in left IFJ, and s LLRateSruaplrpamaraiertgainlalgyrus 13556 41 (cid:1)29 27 3.25 negativeeffectsoffrequencyandconsistencyoverlappedwith trac LAngulargyrus (cid:1)36 (cid:1)80 25 4.67 RT in left IFG. Previous studies of reading suggested in- t/2 LatLerSaulpprraemfroanrgtainlalgyrus 2097 (cid:1)62 (cid:1)41 31 4.10 volvementoftheseareasinphonologicalprocessing(De´monet 0/8/1 LMiddlefrontalgyrus (cid:1)30 22 43 5.11 etal.1992;FiezandPetersen1998;Fiezetal.1999,2006;Price 7 9 NeVgaetnivtrealwoocrcdipfitroe-qtueemnpcoyraclorrelations 47069 2000; Bookheimer 2002; Fiebach et al. 2002; Price, Gorno- 9/3 RFusiformgyrus 39 (cid:1)44 (cid:1)20 (cid:1)5.02 Tempini, et al. 2003; Joubert et al. 2004; Mechelli et al. 2005), 98 LFusiformgyrus (cid:1)38 (cid:1)39 (cid:1)18 (cid:1)4.75 whereas other studies have associated essentially the same 11 RLLCaatlecraarlinoeccsiuplictaulscortex (cid:1)1327 (cid:1)(cid:1)6758 (cid:1)207 (cid:1)(cid:1)44..0184 areas with more general functions such as cognitive control, 7 by Lateralfronto-parietal 29239 attention, and working memory (LaBar et al. 1999; Derrfuss g u LIFJ (cid:1)47 5 28 (cid:1)5.07 et al. 2005; Owen et al. 2005). Of the major computational e LLIMntirdadpleariferotanltasluglcyursus (cid:1)(cid:1)2442 (cid:1)(cid:1)546 4610 (cid:1)(cid:1)33..8551 modelsofreading(e.g.,Plautetal.1996;Coltheartetal.2001; st on Infero-medialandsubcortical 12000 HarmandSeidenberg2004;Perryetal.2007),weareawareof 1 LSubgenualcingulate (cid:1)4 20 (cid:1)1 (cid:1)5.56 none that attempt to disentangle reading-specific effects from 2 A LThalamus (cid:1)11 (cid:1)19 8 (cid:1)5.11 p MeRdiaTlhaplraemfruosntal 6045 16 (cid:1)30 (cid:1)1 (cid:1)4.15 mpoosrseibilgietynethraalt tpheerfaorremasanscheowenffeinctws.hiOteuranddatthaosseugigneostranthgee ril 20 LSMA (cid:1)7 7 47 (cid:1)5.23 1 within the RT outline in the upper part of Figure 3 may be 9 RAnteriorcingulate 9 22 26 (cid:1)3.38 Negativespelling--sound engaged in general task-performance processes such as consistencycorrelations cognitive control, attention, or working memory, which are Lateralprefrontal 10738 LIFJ (cid:1)42 6 30 (cid:1)5.25 sensitivetoanyincreaseintaskloadregardlessofthesourceof Middleandinferiortemporal 3162 the increased demand. LITS (cid:1)53 (cid:1)51 (cid:1)3 (cid:1)3.62 Incontrasttotheseleftinferiorfrontalregions,theleftmid- LFusiformgyrus (cid:1)40 (cid:1)24 (cid:1)10 (cid:1)3.56 Positiveimageabilitycorrelations fusiform gyrus shows areas of overlap between positive RT LLateralparietal 4181 effects and negative effects of word frequency and to some LAngulargyrus (cid:1)57 (cid:1)64 18 3.67 extentconsistencybutnotimageability.Weretheseactivations Medialparietal 2762 LPosteriorcingulatecortex (cid:1)4 (cid:1)43 38 3.80 purely related to general processing demands, activity nega- RLateralparietal 2531 tively correlated with imageability would be expected as RAngulargyrus 43 (cid:1)68 41 3.31 Negativeimageabilitycorrelations well (as is the case in left inferior frontal regions). This left Lateralprefrontal 9035 mid-fusiform gyrus area has been referred to as the ‘‘visual word-formarea’’(VWFA)becauseofitspreferentialresponseto 1806 NeuralSystemsforReadingAloud d Gravesetal. D o w n lo a d e d fro m h ttp s ://a c a d e m ic .o u p .c o m /c e rc o r/a rtic le -a b s tra c t/2 0 /8 /1 7 9 9 /3 9 8 1 1 7 b y g u e s t o n 1 2 A p ril 2 0 1 9 Figure3. Upperpartshowscompositeimagesoffactorsreflectingeitherlexicalorperformanceeffects.Notetheoverlapofformandsemanticvariableswith RTinleft prefrontalareasandthepresenceofwordfrequency,consistency,andRTbutnoimageabilityeffectsintheventraltemporal/fusiformarea.Thelowerpartshowscomposite imagesoffactorsreflectingadditionalactivationsandoverlapsthatmayindicatecontributionsfromsemantics,particularlyinbilateralprecuneusandangulargryiwherepositive word-frequencyandimageabilityeffectsoverlap.ColorcodesreflectareaswhereactivationforeachconditionreachedacorrectedmapwisesignificancethresholdofP\0.05. well-formedletterstrings(Cohenetal.2002;McCandlissetal. whereas a review that transformed coordinates to Montreal 2003; Cohen, Jobert, et al. 2004; Dehaene et al. 2005). Two NeurologicalInstitute(MNI)space(foradiscussionofMNIand reviews place the center coordinate for this area in Talairach Talairach spaces see http://imaging.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/imag- and Tournoux (1988) space at x, y, and z = –43, –54, and –12 ing/MniTalairach) gives it as –44, –58, and –15 (Jobard et al. (Cohen et al. 2002) and –42, –55, and –12 (Bolger et al. 2005), 2003). All report an SD of approximately 5 mm, placing these CerebralCortexAugust2010,V20N8 1807 coordinates within 1SD of each other. Although the nearest relation to uncommon words. Word frequency is highly localminimumforwordfrequencyandlocalmaximumforRT correlated with concept familiarity (Toglia and Battig 1978; aresomewhatanteriortothislocation,bothclustersextendto Baayenetal.2006),contextualdiversity(i.e.,theproportionof clearly include the VWFA coordinate. Not only has this area documents that contain the word; Adelman et al. 2006), and been shown to be positively correlated with graphotactic probability of word association (Nelson and McEvoy 2000). probability in functional brain-imaging studies of healthy Word frequency facilitates performance on semantic decision readers (Binder et al. 2006; Vinckier et al. 2007), but it is also tasks, suggesting that semantic information is more easily one of the major areas that, when damaged, leads to a type of available for high-frequency words (Monsell et al. 1989; Chee acquired dyslexia known as pure alexia (i.e., alexia without et al. 2002). Consistent with these observations, increasing agraphia;BinderandMohr1992;Leffetal.2001;Cohen,Henry, wordfrequencyproducedcorrelatedincreasesinBOLDsignal et al. 2004). There has been debate, however, about whether in essentially the same brain regions that were modulated by thisregionsupportsorthographicprocessingperseoramore increasing imageability and over an even larger spatial extent generalprocess(PriceandDevlin2003),perhapsrelatedtothe withintheseregionsthantheareasmodulatedbyimageability. D mapping between visual input and phonology (Price, Winter- Surprisingly,however,positiveeffectsofwordfrequencyhave o w burn, et al. 2003; Sandak et al. 2004; Hillis et al. 2005). Our only rarely been reported in previous neuroimaging studies. In nlo results, showing activation of this area with longer RT and one study, higher-frequency words activated left temporal and ad e lower values of word frequency and spelling--sound consis- parietal regions during reading and semantic decision tasks d tency but not imageability, suggest that it may support when compared with a low-level baseline task, whereas lower- fro m a relatively specific yet integrative function such as the frequency words did not (Chee et al. 2002). However, these h mapping between orthography andphonology. activations did not survive a direct contrast between high- and ttp s Analternatepossibilitythatcannotberuledoutinthisstudy low-frequency words. The left angular gyrus was activated in ://a is that top-down attention systems amplify processing of anotherstudycomparingsilentreadingofhigh-frequencywords ca d orthographic codes in order to help complete the mapping with consonant strings (Joubert et al. 2004), but again, this e m to phonology. This interpretation, however, rests on the activationdidnotsurviveadirectcomparisonbetweenhigh-and ic assumption that attention systems can selectively modulate low-frequencywords.Toourknowledge,only2previousstudies .ou p orthographic processing, and we are aware of no studies that havefoundpositiveactivationsrelatedtowordfrequency.Using .c o directly demonstrate this. Hence, the overlap of word fMRI during a lexical decision task, Carreiras et al. (2009) m frequency and consistency, but not imageability, effects in observed activation in the precuneus in a direct comparison /ce the putative VWFA suggests that this region supports in- between high- and low-frequency words, which they inter- rco tegration of orthographic and phonological information. preted as reflecting ‘‘more pronounced semantic associations’’ r/a Additionally, although this discussion of VWFA has focused forthoseitems.Similarly,usinganauditorylexicaldecisiontask, rtic le on properties of the area surrounding its center coordinate, Prabhakaranetal.(2006)observedactivationintheprecuneus, -a b thereisalsoevidenceofgradedfunctionalongtheleftfusiform along with left middle temporal and angular gyri, in a direct s gyrus, particularly in the posterior--anterior direction. For comparison between high- and low-frequency words. Other trac example, along the left fusiform gyrus Vinckier et al. (2007) studies that examined frequency effects,however, reported no t/2 0 reported posterior activity modulated by letter frequency, activations related to increasing frequency (Fiez et al. 1999; /8 /1 somewhat more anterior activity for bigram frequency, and Fiebachetal.2002;Kronbichleretal.2004;Carreirasetal.2006; 7 9 more anterior still for quadrigram frequency. Functional Nakicetal.2006;Hauk,Davis,andPulvermu¨ller2008).Threeof 9/3 heterogeneity within the left fusiform gyrus can also be seen these studies restricted their word frequency analyses to areas 98 1 intheworkofHauk,Davis,Kherif,andPulvermu¨ller(2008)and that were more active for words compared with a resting 1 7 Hauk,Davis,andPulvermu¨ller(2008),whoreportactivationfor baseline(Fiezetal.1999;Kronbichleretal.2004;Carreirasetal. b y words compared with baseline (viewing length-matched hash 2006). As can be seen by comparing the successful reading g u marks) within 1 cm anterior to the McCandliss et al. (2003) conditioninFigure1withtheword-frequencyresultinFigure2, e s coordinate, activity modulated by word frequency 2 cm if the word frequency analysis had been restricted to areas t o n anterior, and activity modulated by imageability 1 cm medial showingactivationforwordscomparedwiththerestingbaseline, 1 2 to the VWFA coordinate. Further study will no doubt help the areas more active for higher-frequency words would have A p clarify thefunctional heterogeneity inthis region. bseemenanetxicclsuydsetedm.Asapdpisecaursssteodbeelseawcthiveered(uBriinndgerreesttinalg.2a0n0d9o),ththeer ril 20 1 passivestates.Oneimplicationofthisisthatactivityinthesetof 9 Effectsof Increasing Word Frequency andImageability areas sometimes referred to as the default mode network Anothersetofoverlapsinvolvespositivecorrelations between (Gusnard and Raichle 2001), which includes bilateral posterior neuralactivation and increasing values of word frequency and cingulate/precuneus,dorsomedialprefrontalcortex,andangular imageability (lower part of Fig. 3). These regions, which gyri, may at least partially reflect semantic processes. Thus, include the angular gyrus and precuneus/posterior cingulate contraststhatusearestingbaselinearelikelytomisstheseregions. cortex bilaterally, have been strongly implicated in semantic Other studies that did not exclude brain regions from the processes(Binderetal.2009)andhaveshownactivationwith frequency contrast may have been less sensitive because increasing word imageability in previous imaging studies frequency was treated as a categorical variable (Fiebach et al. (Jessen et al. 2000; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; Binder, 2002; Nakic et al. 2006) or because of smaller stimulus and Westbury, et al. 2005; Sabsevitz et al. 2005; Bedny and subject sample sizes. The reason for the lack of activation for Thompson-Schill 2006). One can also intuit that higher- high-frequency words in the Hauk, Davis, and Pulvermu¨ller frequency words are more likely to elicit automatic activation (2008) study is less clear but may relate to the fact that their in a semantic network due to their extensive exposure in stimuliwerepresentedmorerapidly.Stimuliweredisplayedfor 1808 NeuralSystemsforReadingAloud d Gravesetal.

Description:
Multiparametric Approach. William W. were transformed, when necessary, into standard American English . auditory inspection of the audio file.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.