Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 DOI10.1186/s40462-015-0030-0 RESEARCH Open Access Movement, resting, and attack behaviors of wild pumas are revealed by tri-axial accelerometer measurements Yiwei Wang1*, Barry Nickel1, Matthew Rutishauser2, Caleb M Bryce3, Terrie M Williams3, Gabriel Elkaim4 and Christopher C Wilmers1 Abstract Background: Accelerometers are useful tools for biologists seeking to gain a deeper understanding of thedaily behavior of cryptic species.We describe how weused GPS and tri-axial accelerometer(sampling at64 Hz) collars to monitor behaviors offree-ranging pumas (Pumaconcolor),which are difficult or impossible to observe in thewild. We attached collars to twelve pumas inthe Santa Cruz Mountains, CA from 2010-2012. By implementing Random Forest models, weclassified behaviors inwild pumas based ontraining data from observations and measurements ofcaptivepuma behavior. Results: Weappliedthesemodelstoaccelerometerdatacollectedfromwildpumasandidentifiedmobileand non-mobilebehaviorsincaptiveanimalswithanaccuracyrategreaterthan96%.Accuracyremainedabove95%even after downsampling ouraccelerometerdatato16Hz.Wewerefurtherabletopredictlow-accelerationmovement behavior(e.g.walking)andhigh-accelerationmovementbehavior(e.g.running)with93.8%and92%accuracy, respectively.Wehaddifficultypredictingnon-movementbehaviorssuchasfeedingandgroomingduetothesmallsize ofourtrainingdataset.Lastly,weusedmodel-predictedandfield-verifiedpredationeventstoquantifyacceleration characteristicsofpumaattacksonlargeprey. Conclusion:Theseresultsdemonstratethataccelerometersareusefultoolsforclassifyingthebehaviorsofcryptic mediumandlarge-sizedterrestrialmammalsintheirnaturalhabitatsandcanhelpscientistsgaindeeperinsightinto theirfine-scalebehavioralpatterns.Wealsoshowhowaccelerometermeasurementscanprovidenovelinsightsonthe energeticsandpredationbehaviorofwildanimals.Lastlywediscusstheconservationimplicationsofidentifyingthese behavioralpatternsinfree-rangingspeciesasnaturalandanthropogeniclandscapefeaturesinfluenceanimalenergy allocationandhabitatuse. Keywords:Pumaconcolor,Accelerometer,Behavior,Randomforest,Predation Background time and space remains limited [2]. For example, when Oneofthemajorlogistical challenges tostudyinganimal studying the impacts of habitat fragmentation on large behavior lies in our inability to continuously observe carnivores, it is important to understand how landscape free-ranging animals [1]. While recent technological ad- variables influence population connectivity and animal vancements in the design and versatility of bio-logging movement, resting, and hunting patterns [3]. Accurately devices (e.g., Global Positioning System (GPS) tags) have discerning these behaviors at a fine scale is almost im- substantially improved our capacity to monitor animals, possible fromlocationdataalone,but criticalforinform- our ability to document behavior continually through ingconservationmanagement decisions[4]. In the last decade, accelerometer sensors have emerged as useful tools for remotely monitoring animal behavior *Correspondence:[email protected] [5,6].Bycontinuouslymeasuringbodymovementandpos- 1EnvironmentalStudiesDepartment,CenterforIntegratedSpatialResearch, ture, accelerometers allow scientists to infer the behavior UniversityofCalifornia,1156HighStreet,SantaCruz,CA95064,USA Fulllistofauthorinformationisavailableattheendofthearticle ©2015Wangetal.;licenseeBioMedCentral.ThisisanOpenAccessarticledistributedunderthetermsoftheCreative CommonsAttributionLicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0),whichpermitsunrestricteduse,distribution,and reproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalworkisproperlycredited.TheCreativeCommonsPublicDomain Dedicationwaiver(http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/)appliestothedatamadeavailableinthisarticle, unlessotherwisestated. Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page2of12 and energy expenditure of the instrumented individual Table1Total2secondobservations(N)ofcaptivepuma [7]. Accelerometers have been used to study a wide range behaviorsclassifiedbymobilityclassandbehaviorclass of behavior and physiology research topics, including for- Mobility Behavior N aging,reproduction,activity,energybudgets,andlocomo- Yes Lowaccelerationmovement 564 tion[7,8].Whileaccelerometryhasbeenusedsuccessfully Yes Highaccelerationmovement 50 to differentiate behaviors across a variety of taxa, most No Resting 1167 accelerometer-based behavioral studies have focused on No Eating 284 marine animals including marine mammals, sea turtles, sharks,andseabirds[8]. No Grooming 77 Few studies have used accelerometers to document be- havioral budgets of wild terrestrial animals. Wilson et al. removed six individuals from analyses entirely and [9]recentlyusedacombinationofcomprehensivetri-axial extracted 4-26 days of accelerometer data from each of accelerometer measurements and extremely fine scale the remaining individuals (Table 2). GPS data to describe second-by second hunting behavior in cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus). Another study on chee- Mobilitymodelclassificationofcaptivepumaactivity tahs and one on oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus) Our mobility model, which segregated puma activity into also indicated that our understanding of wildlife behavior mobile and non-mobile periods, correctly classified move- isenhancedbyevenlimitedaccelerometerdatacompared ments 96.17% of observed movement behaviors. The toGPS dataalone[5,10].Bothof thesestudiesused short model identified Amp M (the amplitude of the dominant temporal segments (i.e., a few seconds) of accelerometer frequencyforthemagnitudeofthemeasurements;referto measurementseveryfewminutes,calibratedwithfieldob- Table3for variablenamesanddescriptions),DFZ(domin- servationstopredictbehaviorbetweensuccessiveGPSlo- antfrequencyoftheZ-axis),andSDM(standarddeviation cations.However,manyanimalscannotbeobservedeasily ofthemagnitude)asthemostimportantvariablesforpre- inthewildandsamplingbehaviorforafewsecondsevery dicting mobility. We observed little loss of predictive few minutes does not necessarily reflect the animal’s power down to 16 Hz for our mobility model when we primaryactivityduringthattimesegment. down-sampled our accelerometer measurements from Herewedescribetheuseofaccelerometermeasurements 64Hzto32,16,8,4and2Hz(Figure1). and observations of captive animals to predict behavior in wildpumas(Pumaconcolor).Likemostlargefelids,pumas Behaviormodelclassificationofcaptivepumaactivity are cryptic animals and infrequently observed in their Our second and more comprehensive behavioral model natural environment [11],makingit difficulttodocument categorizedthepuma’smovementbehaviorsintolowaccel- the fine-scale behavioral patterns of this species. Our pri- eration (e.g., walk) and high acceleration (e.g., trotting and mary objectives were to use continuous accelerometer running) movements, and the non-movement behaviors measurements (sampling at 64 Hz), in combination with into resting, eating, and grooming behaviors. This behavior periodic GPS readings, to distinguish different behaviors in free-roaming pumas (Puma concolor), describe the Table2TotalaccelerometerandGPSdatagatheredfrom accelerometer signatures of puma predation events, and free-rangingpumasfrom2010-2011 determinethesufficientaccelerometersamplingfrequency PumaID Sex Accelerometerdayssampled GPSAvailable forcategorizingthesebehaviors. 2 F 26.72 Yes 5 M 14 Yes Results Behavioralmeasurements 7 F 9.42 Yes We paired observations of captive pumas performing 16 M 16 Yes activitiesincluding resting, feeding,moving,andgrooming 17 M 14 Yes with accelerometer measurements to build a classification 28 F 4 Yes algorithmtocategorizethosebehaviorsinthewildanimals. 4 M 8.23** Yes Using 2 captive pumas (1 male and 1 female), we docu- 11 F 0.66 No mented 2142 discrete behavioral observations, including walking, grooming, resting, feeding, and fast movement 13 F 20.35* Yes and their corresponding accelerometer measurements 20 F 11.2 No (Table 1). From 2010-2011, we outfitted 12 wild pumas 27 M 0 Yes (5males,7females)withGPS collarsandaccelerometer 30 F NA** Yes sensors (Table 2). Due to mechanical or software failure *onlytwoaxesaccuratelymeasured.**date/timeinaccuratelyrecorded. in the onboard accelerometer sensors or SD cards, we Bolded,italicizedIDsindicateindividualsusedinanalyses. Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page3of12 Table3Labelsandexplanationsofparametersextracted model predicted captive puma resting, low, and high accel- fromaccelerometerdataandusedinRFmodels erationmovementbehaviorswith96.8%,93.8%,and92%ac- predictingpumabehavioralclassification[12,13] curacy, respectively. The model predicted feeding behavior Parameter Label Definition with 65.7% accuracy but failed to detect grooming (0%) Axes X,Y,Z X,Y,Zaxes (Table 4). This behavior model identified SDM, DFZ, and SDZ as the most important variables for classification. Magnitude M Squarerootofthesumsof squaresoftheaccelerationin theX,YandZaxes Modelpredictionsofwildpumaactivity Dynamicbody ODBAX, Meanofdynamic We found a strong positive relationship between our acceleration(ing) ODBAY, accelerationvaluealongX,Y, model predictions of percent mobility and the distance ODBAZ, andZaxes traveled by wild pumas between successive 15-minute Overalldynamicbody ODBA SumofODBAX,ODBAY, GPS points (β=5.245 standard error=0.174, p<0.001), acceleration ODBAZ which further supports the model’s overall classification Dominantpower AmpX, Amplitudeofdominant accuracy.Random effectswerenot significant. spectrum AmpY,Amp frequency Z,AmpM Our24-houractivitybudgetsforindividualpumasshow Dominantfrequency(Hz) DFX,DFY, Frequencyatdominant a general pattern of decreased movement activity in the DFZ,DFM powerspectrum daytime and increased activity at night (Figure 2). Males StandardDeviationof SDX,SDY, StandardDeviationof were more active than females (t=13.37, p=0) and more dynamicbody SDZ,SDM dynamicaccelerationand often spent over 50% of their hourly increments moving accelerationand magnitudeinwindow (15% for males versus 1% for females). Males were more magnitude activenocturnally(6PM to6AM; t =15.22, p=0)whereas females did not display a significant nocturnal movement preference(t=0.59,p=0.28).However,evenforpumasof thesamesex,individualsexhibitedconsiderablevariability intheiractivity.Forexample,2Fand7F,bothfemaleswith kittens, moved very little even during dawn and dusk, whereas 28F, a female without kittens, was much more active and even moved regularly throughout the daytime. A territorial male, 5M, exhibited one peak in activity around midnight whereas the two other males experi- enced a decrease in activity around midnight. When de- tailed GPS information was available, the actual distance traveled by pumas strongly corroborated the proportion oftimewepredictedmovement(Figures3and4). We tested whether puma predation events, identified from GPS data, were defined by clusters of high acceler- ation movements. We observed a sustained cluster of high acceleration movements, as identified by the behavior model,inthetimeperiodbetweenoneGPSsamplinginter- valbeforethestartofapredationeventthroughtheendof the first quartile of the event (Figure 5, Additional file 1: Figure S1). When compared to other clusters of high Table4Cross-validationofactual(rows)andpredicted (columns)behaviorsofcaptiveanimalsascategorizedby thebehaviormodel Feed Groom Rest High Low Percentaccurate Feed 179 0 67 0 38 63.7 Groom 20 0 54 0 1 0 Figure1Theaccuracyofpredictionsbythemobilitymodel Rest 26 1 1130 0 10 96.8 remainshighuntilthedataissampledbelow8Hz(topgraph). Thecorrelationbetweenthemobilitymodelpredictionsandthe High 0 0 0 46 4 92 distancetraveleddeclinessteadilyasthedataisdown-sampled Low 24 0 9 2 529 93.8 (bottomgraph). Highandlowrepresenthighandlowaccelerationmovements. Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page4of12 Figure2Predictedaveragehourlyactivityacrossa24-hourperiodforallpumas(+1SD).Thesebehavioraldairiesareaveragedover 25.64days(2F),13days(5M),8.42days(7F),13days(17M),16days(16M)and4days(28F). accelerationmovement,whichmaynotnecessarilybeasso- female puma predation incidents, likely because they were ciatedwithpredationevents,4ofour6potentialpredation subduing large, adult deer. In comparison, the male puma events were ranked among the top 10% for cluster size predation incidents we identified consisted of fewer high (or length) and 3 of 6 for maximum magnitude (Figure 6). acceleration movements because their targeted prey were We recorded sustained, high acceleration movements for fawnsduringthedatacollectionperiod. Figure315minutemovementdistancesmeasureddirectlyfromsubsequentGPSlocations(solid)andassociatedpredictedmovement activitybasedonaccelerometry(dashed)forpumas28Fand16M.Whilewewouldnotexpectactivitylevelstobeperfectlycorrelatedwith linearmovementdistances,thelevelorcorrespondencebetweenthesetwomeasuresprovidesafield-basedassessmentoftheabilityofaccelerometer measurementstopredictmovementactivity. Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page5of12 Figure415minuteGPSlocationsandassociatedpredictedmovementactivityfor28F. From the limited predation incidents we analyzed, we which is characterized by higher acceleration and period- notedthattheamountoftimeittooktokillpreywasrelated icity, from non-movement activities. The tight association to the age and size of both the predator and prey species. between footfall frequency and the dominant frequency of Based on the duration of the high acceleration movements, the accelerometer measurements bodes many promising fawnswerekilledbymalesinlessthanoneminutewhereas avenuesforcalculatingdailyenergeticexpenditure[15]. females took over two minutes to kill large bucks and an MostcurrentresearchonanimalmovementusesGPSor unknownpreyspecies. radio-telemetry collars [2], which only allow researchers to measure locations sporadically throughout each day. How- Discussion and conclusion ever, with GPS tags sampling at a low temporal resolution, Our aim in this study was to use accelerometer measure- it is difficult to distinguish between an animal that moves mentsrecordedoncaptiveanimalsasaproxytoclassifybe- 500metersinastraightlineinashorttimeperiodfroman havior in wild animals. Using Random Forest models, we animal that is active for longer but meanders only a short were able to accurately predict periods of non-movement, distanceinanonlinearfashion[17].Incontrast,accelerom- low acceleration (i.e., stalking, walking), and high acceler- eters take near continuous measurements, thus providing ationmovements(i.e.,trottingandrunning)inunobservable fine-scale documentation of animal behavior while the in- wild animals. This insight allowed us to better document strument is activated [1]. The enhanced dataset from con- pumamovementpatternsandactivitylevelsthroughoutthe tinuously sampling accelerometers can yield detailed dayandtoidentifyindividualandsexdifferences. information on behavior (e.g. whether the animal is travel- Our model identified Amp M, DFZ, and SDM as the ingorpotentiallyhunting)andenergyexpenditurebetween top ranked predictors of puma mobility. The first two successiveGPSfixes[15]. variables arestronglytied tothe periodicityof themove- With continual advancements in biologging technologies, ment since Amp M is the dominant power spectrum of sometagdesignsnowincludeGPSsensorsthatworksyner- the magnitude and DFZ is the dominant frequency of gistically with accelerometers (i.e. accelerometer-informed the Z-axis, which measures the heave (up-down motion) GPS), allowing for more flexible and intelligent GPS sam- of the animal. For terrestrial quadrupeds, movement be- pling intervals [9,17]. Such accelerometer-informed GPS haviors result in cyclic accelerometer patterns that are tags also reduce battery consumption (thereby prolonging dominated by one frequency because the accelerations field deployment) by only recording GPS data points when are primarily produced by footfalls and body movements the animal is actively moving [17]. Accelerometers could (Figure 7) [14]. Thesedominantfrequenciescorrespondto also be programmed to trigger onboard cameras like Kitty- footfallpatternsandcanbeusedforbiomechanicalanden- cam, which was used to document domestic cat predation ergetics analyses [15,16]. The third parameter identified is on wildlife [18], to more efficiently quantify hunting at- the standard deviation of the magnitude, which is higher tempts and kills of small prey that are difficult to identify during mobile than non-mobile behaviors. Taken together, usingGPSdataalone.Camerasonwildanimalscanalsobe theseparameterscanclearlydistinguishmovementbehavior, used to verify behaviors (e.g. footfall counts for energetics) Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page6of12 Figure5Plotsoftwopredationeventsbypuma2F.Thetoppanelforeachplotillustratesthenumber(N)ofhighaccelerationmovements perminuteoveraperiodoftwodays.Thedarkgreyrectanglehighlightstheperiodoftimeassociatedwiththepredationeventasverified independentlyfromfieldvisitstoclustersofGPSlocations.Thebottompanelshowstherawaccelerometermeasurementsinunitsofgravitygforthe Z-axis.Thebottominsertsmagnifyaone-minuteperiodofaccelerometermeasurementsfromselectedlargeclusterstoshowthemagnitudeand durationoftheaccelerationduringthosehighaccelerationevents.Thearrowindicateswhenwehypothesizethekilleventtohaveoccurred. predicted by models generated using accelerometer In our study, accelerometer sensors provided new in- data[19]. sights on the variability and duration of predation events We noted that puma predation events were associated identified by GPS locations and confirmed by subsequent with high acceleration movements, which were predicted fieldvalidations. Whilepredationbyfemales onadult prey by the behavior model. Although clusters of high acceler- occurred over several minutes and included some of the ation movement occurred throughout the day, the events highest magnitude ranges, events involving larger-bodied that we classified as kills had raw accelerometer readings malepumasandsmallerjuvenilepreywerenotasextreme. withmagnitudesthatweregenerallylongerandlargercom- More broadly, we may be able to characterize predation paredtoother events(Figures5and6).Additionally,there andattemptedpredationeventsofothermediumandlarge were several other prolonged high acceleration movement bodiedmammalsusingpairedaccelerometerandGPSdata clusters of similar or even larger magnitudes that were andcorroboratingthesepredictionswithfieldvisitationsof not associated with identified kills. These events may kill site GPS clusters [3]. Combining such detailed loca- have been unidentified kills or unsuccessful hunting tional and acceleration information can reveal the dur- attempts. ation, energetic expenditure, and chase sequence of Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page7of12 Figure6Histogramsshowinghowthesize(top)andmagnitude(bottom)ofclustersofpredictedhighaccelerationmovements associatedwithpredationeventscomparetothosenotassociatedwithpredationeventsforpumas2F,5M,7Fand16M.Binscontaining valuescorrespondingtoverifiedpredationeventsarehighlightedinredandaccentedbyanarrow.Killsby2Fand7Fatthefarrightofthe histogramwereofadultdeerwhereasthoseof16Mand5Minthecenterofthehistogramwereoffawns. puma predation events and help scientists gain more oystercatchers, cheetahs, and other species have used ac- insight about the hunting behavior of pumas [15]. celerometers to identify feeding bouts in terrestrial ani- Our behavior model was weak at predicting feeding mals with some success [5,10]. However, both studies events and was not able to predict grooming behaviors. were able to observe the collared individual engaging in This is most likely due to the complexity of identifying these behaviors and use that information to classify add- non-locomotive motion [7] and to the relatively few in- itional events. While we are unable to directly observe stances of these behaviors observed in the captive pumas in the wild, we may be able to record their feed- pumas. While grooming is a relatively unimportant be- ing behavior using cameras placed at bait sites or fresh havior to identify, accurately predicting feeding events is carcasses [20]. Using this information, we could then crucial in understanding behavior and energetics. We identify accelerometer sequences that correspond with believe these issues can be overcome in the future feeding in the field and then use that to identify future through a variety of innovative strategies. Studies on feeding events. Additionally, we could perform more Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page8of12 Figure7Two-secondwindowsofZ-axisaccelerationforfourbehaviorswithassociateddominantfrequenciesanddominant powerspectrums. feeding trials with captive pumas using whole deer and location, and energy expenditure has the potential to pro- raccooncarcassessoastomorecloselymimictheirdietary videnovelinsightsintohowlandscapestructureinfluences habitsinourstudyarea. the allocation of energy to different behaviors [25]. Such Integrating accelerometer technology into dataloggers information would be valuable for conservation and man- has great potential for animal behavioral research and is agement issues by revealing the detailed responses of being increasingly adopted in ecological and conserva- individual animals to their surrounding landscape. tion studies [4,8]. Data derived from accelerometers can also be used to assess how animals respond behaviorally and energetically to anthropogenic influences [15,21]. Methods For example, pumas and other large carnivores may Studyspeciesandarea change their overall activity levels and hunting patterns, Pumas are territorial, apex predators that live in diverse thus impacting their caloric demands, when moving habitatsthroughouttheAmericas[26].Individualsarepri- through human dominated habitats [3,22,23]. Addition- marily nocturnal and solitary, although females will typic- ally, caloric expenditure by pumas can be calculated ally raise and accompany cubs for 15-21 months after more accurately using our accelerometer derived activity birth. In our study area in the Santa Cruz Mountains of budgetsandfootfallfrequenciesthanfromGPSinforma- California (37° 10.00’ N, 122° 3.00’ W), pumas primarily tion alone [14,15,24]. feed on black-tailed deer(Odocoileushemionuscolumbia- Our research demonstrates that accelerometers can suc- nus) but occasionally on other species, including wild cessfully predict movement behaviors in animals that are boars (Sus scrofa), raccoons (Procyon lotor) and domestic difficult to observe in the wild. However, more complex cats[3]. behaviors,suchasfeeding,mightonlybeaccuratelyidenti- Our 1,700 km2 study area encompasses a diverse land- fiedwithadditionalobservationsfromcaptiveandwildan- scape ranging from dense, urban development to large imals. Accelerometer sensors can be used with any tracts of intact and relatively undisturbed native vegeta- terrestrial mammal to create a complete activity budget, tion primarily comprised of redwood and Douglas fir, catalogue behaviors, including predatory ones, and poten- oak woodland, or coastal scrub communities. It is tiallymeasureenergeticexpenditureaswellasforagingef- bisected by a large freeway and further crisscrossed by ficiency. We believe this ability to link behavior, spatial numerous smaller roads providing access to rural houses Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page9of12 and developments. The climate is Mediterranean, with tochangesinvelocityresulting from patternsoflocomo- precipitation concentrated between November and April. tion and generally reflects the movement of the animal. Elevationrangesfromsealevelto1155m. We subtracted static acceleration from collar measure- ments using 2-second windows [29], and then extracted Captiveanimaldatacollectionandanalysis 16 predictor variables from the three-accelerometer axes We used custom-built collars [15,27] equipped with a tri- and the magnitude from each 2-second segment of dy- axial accelerometer sampling continuously at 64Hz to namic acceleration (see Table 1). We also applied a Fast monitorbehaviorincaptiveandwildpumas.Thetri-axial Fourier Transform to accelerometer measurements to accelerometer was mounted such that the x-, y-, and z- extract the dominant frequency and dominant power axeswereparalleltotheanterior-posterior,thetransverse, spectrumvaluesofeachbehavior. and the dorsal-ventral planes of the animal, respectively. We selected Random Forests (RF) [30] as our modeling Captive pumas were housed and trained by the Colorado tooltopredictunobservedbehaviorsinwildanimalsbased Parks and Wildlife (Foothills Wildlife Research Facility) onmeasurementsofobservedbehaviorsincaptiveanimals. [15]. We outfitted one adult male and one adult female RFisarelativelynovelandpowerfulmachinelearningtool pumawithatestaccelerometercollarduringtrainingses- thatworkswellfornon-linearandcomplexecologicaldata sions. We observed the collared animals and recorded not easily fitted by traditional methods such as generalized their behaviors both manually and with a video camera. linearmodels[31].RFalsomakesitpossibletomakeaccur- We conducted 1-2 trials per animal during two different ate predictions from datasets with correlated variables and visits,totaling2hoursand40minutesofrecordedbehav- to compare conditional variable importance measures, ioral observations for collar-outfitted captive pumas. At which identify the extent to which specific predictor the end of each visit, we retrieved the collar and down- variables influence classification accuracy [31]. A higher loadedthedata. measureof variableimportanceindicatesthatthe variable We reviewed all recordings of captive pumas and cate- exerts greater influence on the response relative to other gorized behaviors into mobile (e.g., slow movement, fast predictorswithlowervalues[32]. movement) or non-mobile (e.g., resting, feeding, groom- Our first model (mobility model) segregated mobile ing) activities. We divided observations into 2-second from non-mobile behavior. To build our model using segments encompassing only one behavior type and ex- RF, we fit 500 classification trees to a randomly selected tracted the corresponding accelerometer data (Figure 7). subsample (n=1000, without replacement) of captive Animalactivitieswereconstrainedintotheabove5classi- puma data using a random subset of 5 predictor vari- fications,andwedidnotcataloguetransitionsbetweenbe- ables for each split in the tree [32,33]. Predictions made haviors since they occurred very quickly (≤1s). We chose by all trees for each observation were then tallied, with 2-second windows because this duration accommodated classification assigned by the majority result and ties at least 2 full strides of the animal within the 20 m test decided randomly. Model prediction accuracies were trackusedforcaptivepumafilmingandcalibrationtrials. calculated by comparing predicted and actual classifi- To compareacross collars we converted allaccelerom- cations. We then obtained unbiased variable importance eter data into units of g (1g=9.8 m s−2) using tag spe- estimates using a permutation procedure described in cific calibration values derived prior to deployment. The Strobl [32]. We built our second model (a more compre- processofcalibratingtheaccelerometersconsistsofgen- hensive behavior model) using the same methodology to tly tumbling the collar and measuring the body-frame predict five classes of behaviors: low acceleration move- output of the accelerometer triad [28]. In the case of a ment(e.g.,walking),highaccelerationmovements(e.g.trot- perfectly calibrated accelerometer, the locus of points ting, running), resting, eating, and grooming. We fit all of would all be attached to a sphere centered at the origin our models and calculated variable importance estimates with a radius of 1g. Due to null shift errors, the sphere is using the Party package [34] in the R statistical program centeredofftheorigin,andscalefactorerrorstransformthe (vers.2.15.1)[35]. sphereintoanellipsoid.WedevelopedacustomMATLAB Wecreateddown-sampleddatasetsofaccelerometerdata 8.0 (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, Massachusetts, United at 32, 16, 8, 4, and 2 Hz to explore the influence of sam- States)scripttoextractthenullshiftandscalefactorerrors pling frequency on model prediction accuracy. Using the byfittinganellipsetothedatatwoaxesatatime,andcom- down-sampleddatasets,webuiltadditionalmobilitymodels biningtheresultantparameters. andassessedmodelaccuracyasdescribedpreviously. Accelerometer measurements were deconstructed into static and dynamic components [7]. Static acceleration Wildanimaldatacollectionandanalysis relates to the inclination of the accelerometer with re- We captured wild pumas (5 males, 7 females) from specttotheearth’sgravitationalfieldandtherebyreflects 2010-2012 using trailing hounds, cage traps, or leg hold the posture of the animal. Dynamic acceleration relates snares as described in Wilmers et al. [3]. Each animal Wangetal.MovementEcology (2015) 3:2 Page10of12 was tranquilized using Telazol and outfitted with an off- points to be generally larger than if we mostly predicted the-shelf GPS/VHF collar (Vectronics Aerospace GPS non-movement. PLUS model) combined with the custom-built archival We constructed 24-hour movement budgets for all 3-axis accelerometer tag [27], which was incorporated pumas to document the proportion of time pumas spent into the battery casing (total collar weight=480 g) Data movingthroughoutthe day.To determine the proportion collected by the accelerometer were recorded in an on- oftimespentmovingforeachone-hourperiod(e.g.,1AM board 8GB microSD card, which is capable of storing to 2AM), we calculated the number of increments during more than 200days ofaccelerometermeasurements. whichwepredictedmobileactivityanddividedthatbythe We programmed each collar to acquire a GPS fix every total number of predictions we recorded between those 4 hours and had a mean fix rate of 86% (±1%) from 4 or time periods. From our behavior model, we generated moresatellites.ForanimalscapturedpriortoApril2011,we 24-hourbehavioralbudgetsforourfivebehaviorclasses. programmed accelerometers to record at a duty-cycle of Using the results from our behavior model, we tested 2 weeks on, 4 weeks off commencing immediately upon whether predation events were associated with periods capture. While accelerometers were recording, the collars of high acceleration movement. We used six feeding wereprogramedtoacquireadditionalGPSfixesat5-minute events by four pumas on five deer and one unknown intervals between 8PM and 9PM local time for one week species to examine the corresponding high accelerom- (GPS intensive sampling period). After April 2011, we eter movements. When feeding, pumas generally remain programmed accelerometers to operate at cycles of two with the carcass over several GPS acquisitions. We used consecutive days every week beginning 5 days after the this information to estimate the duration of the feeding animal was captured to extend battery life while optimiz- event as the interval of time between the first and last ing data collection. When accelerometers were recording, GPS location at the kill site. We also added the four- collarsrecordedadditionalGPSlocationsevery15minutes hour interval prior to the first GPS location associated during a 24-hour period from noon to noon (GPS inten- with the site to the feeding duration since it is possible sive sampling period). We retrieved all collars either dur- thatthepuma madeakillduringthis periodoftime. ingarecaptureoftheanimal(n=8)orfollowingitsdeath Five of these feeding events were visited and verified by (n=4;2depredations,2unknowncauses).TheAnimalCare field personnel, and one was classified as a kill with 80% andUseCommitteeatUCSantaCruzapprovedallanimal- probabilityusingapredationmodeldevelopedbyWilmers handlingprocedures(IACUCProtocol#Wilmc1101). etal.[3].Fromobservationsofcaptivepumabehavior,we We downloaded all available accelerometer data and know that faster and more intense movements, such as removed the first 24 hours of data following anesthesia. runningandjumping,leadtoaccelerometerreadingswith We then converted accelerometer data into units of g as ranges spanning 3-5 g or more. We expected that preda- describedintheprevioussection. tion events would be associated with clusters of high ac- Weusedourcaptivepuma-derivedmobilityand behav- celeration movements as pumas attack and wrestle with iormodelstopredictfree-rangingpumabehaviorfromac- their prey. To screen for possible predation events, we celerometer data obtained from wild puma collars that identified clusters of high acceleration movements with a collected at least one day of both accelerometer and GPS magnitude range exceeding 3.4g, or two standard devia- data. Because we could not observe behavior in wild ani- tions above the average and calculated the duration and mals, we tested the accuracy of our mobility model’s pre- themaximummagnitudeoftheevent.Wedefinedaclus- dictions by fitting a linear mixed effects model to GPS ter to be two or more successive high acceleration move- data using the lme4 package [36]. Specifically, we tested ments separated by no more than three minutes between whether our mobility model predictions were positively consecutive behaviors. We expected that if high acceler- correlated with the distance traveled by pumas between ation movements were associated with predation events, GPS points. We used the distance between successive wewouldseeaclusterof highaccelerationmovementsin 15-minute GPS points as our response variable and the first quartile of each predation event. Compared to treated our model-predicted percentage of time spent non-predation clusters of high acceleration movements, movingasafixedeffectwithpumaIDasarandomeffect. wealsoexpectedthesizeandmaximummagnitudeofthe We expected that longer-distanceGPS movementswould cluster representing a potential predation event to be in becorrelatedwithahigherpercentageofmodel-predicted thetop10%ofthosemeasurementsacrossallclusters. movement activity, and that this relationship might vary by individual pumas. For example, during a 15-minute Additional file gap between successive GPS points, we used the mobility modeltopredictthepercentageoftimethepumawasac- Additionalfile1:FigureS1.Plotsofpredationeventsbypumas5M, tively moving. If we mostly predicted movement, we 7F,and16M,analogoustoFigure2.Thetoppanelforeachplot illustratesthenumber(N)ofhighaccelerationmovementsperminute would expect that the distance between the two GPS
Description: