ebook img

MONOAMINES IN THE BRAIN OF TARANTULAS (APHONOPELMA HENTZI) (ARANEAE, THERAPHOSIDAE): DIFFERENCES ASSOCIATED WITH MALE AGONISTIC INTERACTIONS PDF

8 Pages·2001·9.5 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview MONOAMINES IN THE BRAIN OF TARANTULAS (APHONOPELMA HENTZI) (ARANEAE, THERAPHOSIDAE): DIFFERENCES ASSOCIATED WITH MALE AGONISTIC INTERACTIONS

2001. The Journal of Arachnology 29:388-395 MONOAMINES IN THE BRAIN OF TARANTULAS (APHONOPELMA HENTZI) (ARANEAE, THERAPHOSIDAE): DIFFERENCES ASSOCIATED WITH MALE AGONISTIC INTERACTIONS Fred Punzo: Dept, of Biology, Box 5F, University of Tampa, 401 W. Kennedy Blvd., Tampa, Florida 33606 USA Thomas Punzo: 2782 Johnson Dr., Albuquerque, New Mexico 50052 USA ABSTRACT. Experiments were conducted to determine the effects of male-male agonistic encounters on changes in monoamine neurotransmitter concentrations in the supraesophageal ganglion (brain) ofthe tarantula, Aphonopelma hentzi. Serotonin levels were significantly reduced 30 min after fighting in both dominant (66.5 ±9.1 SE nmol/mg protein) and subordinate (42.8 ± 7.6) animals as compared to isolated controls (89,7 ± 13,2), and these differences persisted for up to 24 h. A similar decrease was found for octopamine concentrations in dominant (43.7 ± 7.7) and subordinate (31.2 ± 4.9) spiders when compared to controls (56.9 ± 5.8). In addition, serotonin and octopamine levels were significantly lower in subor- dinate vs. dominant spiders. Agonistic interactions had no effect on the concentrations of dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine. In isolated control spiders, serotonin (89.7 ± 13.2 SE nmol/mg protein) was present in highestconcentration in thebrain, followedby octopamine (56.9 ±5.8 nmol/mg), dopamine (22.4 ± 3.8 pmol/mg), norepinephrine (15.3 ± 4.7 pmol/mg), and epinephrine (0.57 ± 0.2 pmol/mg). The results indicate that following agonistic encounters, monoamine concentrations in the brain decrease to different levels in winners and losers. This is the first demonstration that the establishment ofsocial status causes changes in brain monoamines in spiders. Keywords: Agonistic interactions, Aphonopelma, CNS monoamines, males Activation of monoaminergic systems in ganglion (SEG) of arthropods lies above the the central nervous system (CNS) has been esophagus and consists of three major brain implicated in the mediation of short-term and regions: the protocerebrum, which contains chronic physiological stress responses as well the ‘higher brain centers' including the optic as aggressive and social dominance relation- lobes and nerves; the central body, corpora ships in numerous taxa (Eichelman 1987; pedunculata and a variable number of ganglia Bicker & Menzel 1989; Haney et al. 1990; and associated neuropils; the deuterocere- Summers et al. 1995). Most of this work has brum, which innervates the antennae (reduced focused on social interactions in vertebrates in arachnids); and the tritocerebrum, which in- exposed to staged encounters between con- nervates the mouthparts (Horridge 1965; Gup- specifics under laboratory conditions. For ex- ta 1987). In spiders, the CNS is highly con- ample, the social status ofsubordinate animals densed anteriorly, and the SEG consists is associated with an increase in the utilization primarily of the cheliceral and optic nerves, of the indolalkylamine neurotransmitter (NT) optic neuropils (masses of axonal fiber tracts) serotonin (5-HT, 5-hydroxytryptamine) in var- for primary and secondary eyes (‘corpora pe- ious brain regions from fish to mammals (Ha- dunculata’), and a loosely organized central & ney et al. 1990; Winberg et al. 1997; Punzo body (Babu 1985; Wegerhoff Breidbach 2000a). In addition, activation of brain cate- 1995).There have been few studies on neuro- cholaminergic systems including dopamine chemical parameters associated with behavior (DA), norepinephrine (NE), and epinephrine in general, and agonistic interactions specifi- (Epi) in vertebrates is associated with in- cally, in arthropods and other invertebrates. RNA creased levels of aggression and dominance For example, increases in protocerebral (Eichelman 1987; Matter et al. 1998). and protein synthesis have been shown to ac- In general, the brain or supraesophageal company learning in molluscs (Kerkut et al. 388 — PUNZO & PUNZO—TARANTULA BRAIN MONOAMINES 389 1970; Adamo & Chase 1991), decapod crus- trations of acetylcholine (ACh), norepineph- taceans (Punzo 1985), insects (Lin & Roelofs rine (NE), and acetylcholinesterase (AChE) as 1992; Punzo 1996), and spiders (Punzo compared to younger instars (Punzo 1993). 1988a). Cycloheximide-induced inhibition of Theraphosid spiders exhibit a variety ofag- brain protein synthesis impaired learning and gressive behaviors. Some of these involve memory in insects (Jaffe 1980) and spiders male-male agonistic interactions (Baerg 1958; (Punzo 1988a), as well as innate phototactic Minch 1977; Punzo & Henderson 1999; Pun- behavior in tenebrionid and passalid beetles zo 2000b), while others involve males and fe- & (Punzo Jellies 1980). Changes in levels of males, especially during courtship and mating brain monoamines have been implicated in a (Costa & Perez-Miles 1992; Shillington & variety of ontogenetic shifts in behavior in Verrell 1997), and sometimes end in sexual honeybee workers including the onset ofnest- cannibalism (Punzo & Henderson 1999). A guarding behavior (Moore et al. 1987) and previous study showed that agonistic interac- discrimination between olfactory cues (Mac- tions between paired conspecific males of millan & Mercer 1987). Aphonopelma hentzi (Girard 1854) were ob- With respect to aggression and agonistic in- served in 24 out of 27 (88.9%) staged en- teractions, octopamine turnover rates in- counters in the laboratory (Punzo & Hender- creased significantly in crickets after fighting son 1999). These encounters were initiated by with conspecifics (Adamo et al. 1995). In- vigorous leg-fencing with each protagonist creased foraging activities and nest defense pushing forcefully against its opponent. These were correlated with higher concentrations of fencing bouts were interrupted from time to octopamine (OA), dopamine (DA), and sero- time with at least one of the males exhibiting tonin (5-HT) in the SEG ofworker honeybees a threat display (elevation of the anterior end (Harris & Woodring 1992). Indeed, ithas been of the body, first pair of legs and pedipalps, OA suggested that is part of a general arousal and opening of the fangs). At least one male system which prepares insects for a variety of exhibited a strike response toward his oppo- vigorous skeletal-muscular activities, territo- nent in 11 out of 24 cases (45.8%). In eight rial defense, and helps the animal deal with of these instances (33.3%), one of the males stressful conditions (Corbet 1991; Orchard et was killed. al. 1993). Increased 5-HT levels in the brain The purpose ofthis study was to investigate (SEG) have been implicated in the onset of neurochemical parameters associated with ag- flight behavior in weevils (Guerra et al. 1991). onistic interactions between males ofthe ther- Lobsters exhibiting dominance over conspe- aphosid spider, Aphonopelma hentzi. Neuro- cifics exhibited higher levels of CNS 5-HT transmitters and comodulators are important when compared with subordinate animals regulatory molecules required for the trans- (Kravitz 1988). mission ofinformation (nerve impulses) along Changes in SEG amine concentrations as neural pathways involved in the control of well as otherNTs were shown to be associated motor movements as well as 'mood’ and mo- & with ontogenetic shifts in behavior in solifug- tivational states (Ansell Bradley 1973). ids (Punzo 1993, 1994). First nymphal instars Specifically, we were interested in whether or (Nl) typically have poorly developed chelic- not there were any differences in the concen- erae, and are gregarious, do nothuntprey, and trations of monoamines (OA, DA, NE, Epi, remain in the nest with their siblings and ma- 5-HT) in the brains (SEG) ofdominant (‘win- ternal parent. However, after molting, second- ners’) vs. subordinate (‘losers’) males follow- instar nymphs (N2) possess functional chelic- ing agonistic encounters. To our knowledge, erae and become aggressive (Punzo 1998a). this is the first study to address neurochemical They will cannabilize one another if they do correlates of aggression in spiders. not disperse from the nest. This pronounced METHODS increase in aggression is associated with sig- nificant changes in brain 5-HT and DA levels, Animals. Males were collected during although OA levels remained relatively con- July and August of 1997 at a site 3.5 km S of stant throughout postembryonic development Elgin, Texas (30°32^N, 97°29'W; Bastrop (Punzo 1—994). In addition, later nymphal in- County). This site consisted ofa dry wash and stars (N5 N8) exhibited higherbrainconcen- surrounding flood plain consisting of sand. 390 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOGY gravel and adobe soils, with numerous rocks, attempt to flee after making initial contact rock crevices, and burrows. The dominant with its opponent. These trials were not used vegetation included prickly pear cactus in data analysis. In all other cases {n = 60 (Opuntia), catclaw {Mimosa), mesquite {Pro- trials), following initial contact, one ofthe spi- sopis), broom weed {Xauthocephalum), and ders would begin to push with its front pair mesquite grass {Bouteloua), Adult males rang- of legs against its opponent. The other spider ing in size from 4.2-6.7 g were abundant dur- rapidly responded in a similar fashion (leg- ing this period and were easily found moving fencing). In other cases, after initial contact, about the surface between 2000-0300 h (Cen- one or both spiders would exhibit the threat tral Standard Time). Spiders were collected display, followed by another bout of leg-fenc- and weighed to the nearest 0.1 g using a ing. In a few instances the fighting escalated Ohaus Model 87 portable electronic balance. until one or both spiders attempted to bite the Spiders were transported to the laboratory other. An encounter trial was terminated if at and housed individually in plastic cages (20 any time during the encounter one of the spi- X 16 X 8 cm). They were provided with water ders backed away and rapidly fled from the ad libitum and fed three times per week to vicinity of the other spider and attempted to satiation on a mixed diet of crickets {Gryllus crawl out ofthe chamber. The spider that held sp,), mealworms {Tenebrio molitor), and its ground was recorded as the 'winner’ (dom- grasshoppers {Schistocerca sp.). They were inant animal), and the spider that fled, the maintained at 22 °C ± 1°, 65% RH, and a Toser’ (subordinate). Each pair of contestants photoperid regime of 12L:12D in a Percival were subjected to only one encounter trial as Model 805 environmental chamber (Boone, described by Summers & Greenberg (1995) in Iowa). Adult males were kept in these condi- their study ofmale-male aggression in lizards. tions for two weeks and then re-weighed on We conducted a total of 60 encounter trials the day before the initiation of encounter tri- comprising 60 pairs of contestants {n = 120). als. Since previous studies on arachnids have Ne—urochemical analyses of brain tis- indicated that differences in body size can in- sues* Immediately following their designa- fluence the outcome of aggressive encounters tion as dominant or subordinate (based on the (Faber & Bayliss 1993; Punzo 1998c, 2000b), outcome of encounter bouts), paired contes- only males ofapproximately similar size (6.2- tants were randomly assigned to one of three 6.7 g) were used for encounter trials and sub- groups; each group consisted of40 spiders (20 sequent neurochemical analyses. Voucher pairs). Spiders in group 1 (Gl) were anaes- specimens have been deposited in the Inver- thetized with CO thirty min after encounter 2 tebrate Collection at —the University ofTampa. trials, and their brains (SEG) removed in a Encounter trials. We used a rectilinear cold room, weighed to the nearest 0.1 g on an glass arena (26 X 16 X 12 cm) divided into electronic analytical balance, and frozen at halves by an opaque divider to stage conspe- -80°C as described by Punzo (1988b) for cific male encounters as described by Punzo subsequent neurochemical analyses. Group 2 (1998c). To summarize, the floor of the arena (G2) and group 3 (G3) spiders were anaesthe- was provided with a layer of loose sand to a tized and their brains frozen at 24 hr and 48 depth of 2 cm. All observations were con- h, respectively, after encounter trials. In this ducted under Black lighting (BioQuip One., way, we were not only able to determine what Model 2804, Gardena, California). We used a neurochemical changes, if any, followed Panasonic PS 150 tape recorder to record ver- male-male aggression, but also how rapid the bal descriptions of each encounter. response might be, and how long these chang- Before each encounter trial, a male spider es might persist. The brains from another (chosen at random) was placed at each end of group of 20 spiders (G4) maintained in iso- the arena, separated by the opaque divider. A lation and notexposed to encountertrials were trial was initiated by removing the divider and used as controls. allowing the animals to interact. Within a pe- After thawing, all glandular and peripheral riod of time ranging from 0.5-8.5 min over fatty tissue was carefully removed from the all trials, the contestants made contact with surface of the SEG (Murdock & Omar 1981). one another (usually with a front leg). In afew The SEG were then weighed to the nearest trials, one of the spiders would immediately 0,01 g on a Sartorius Model 54C electronic PUNZO & PUNZO—TARANTULA BRAIN MONOAMINES 391 analytical balance. Brain protein determina- weights and brain protein values between tions were conducted using the standard pro- dominant, subordinate or isolated control spi- cedure described by Lowry et aL (1951) and ders {P < 0.5), Serotonin was the monoamine expressed as percent (brain protein/brain found in the highest concentration in the weight) (Meyer et al. 1984). The SEG from brains of isolated control of A. hentzi (Table the dominant and subordinate spiders were an- 1), This was followed in decreasing order by alyzed to determine the concentrations of the OA, DA, NE, and Epi. monoamine eeurotransmitters, 5-HT, OA, DA, The effects of agonistic interactions be- and NE, using high performance liquid chro- tween coespecific males on SEG monoamine matography with electrical detection (HPLC- concentrations at various time intervals fol- ED, Beckman Model 47A) as described by lowing encounter trials are shown in Table 1. Brandes et al. (1990). To summarize, each Serotonin (5-HT) levels were significantly re- brain tissue sample was placed in a 750 pi duced in spiders losing aggressive encounters glass vial and homogenized in 50 pi of a 200 (subordinates) for up to 24 h as compared to mM perchloric acid (PA) solution. Following dominant animals (t = 9.4, P < 0,01). This homogenization, an additional 50 pi of PA difference persisted for at least 24 h, with lev- were added to each vial. Samples were then els returning to normal after 48 h (F = 3.36, centrifuged at 10,000 g and 4 °C for 3 min in P < 0.05). In addition, the brains ofdominant a Sorvall Model 100A high speed refrigerated spiders contained significantly lower levels of centrifuge. Twenty pi of supernatant were in- 5-HT than those of the isolated controls (t — jected directly into the HPLC column (40 cm 6.8, P < 0.05) for up to 24 h. These changes in length, with a 0.2 p pore diameter) packed in 5-HT and OA levels associated with fight- with Hypersil and provided with a Hewlett- ing occurred quite rapidly since changes were Packard 760E detector (0.40 V). The mobile detected after only 30 min following an en- phase (flow rate, 3000 psi) used to elute the counter. monoamines consisted of 12% acetonitrile, 20 A similar pattern was found for OA levels mM mM sodium acetate, 100 sodium dihy- which were also significantly reduced in sub- drogen orthophosphate, 2.5 mM octane sul- ordinate vs. dominant animals (t = 6,2, P < fonic acid, and 0.3 mM EDTA disodium salt 0.02). However, the reduced levels of OA as- adjuested to pH 4.2 and filtered through a 0.45 sociated with agoeisitic interactions returned pm filter. Each sample was compared to 5-HT to control levels within 24 hr. With respect to and DA standards tested at the beginning of DA, NE, and Epi, no differences were found each assay run and retested at 30 min inter- between spiders exposed to agonistic encoun- vals. Monoamine concentrations were ex- ters and controls at any time interval {P < pressed as nmol or pmol/mg protein as des- 0.5). ribed by Meyer et al. (1984). DISCUSSION All statistical procedures followed those de- scribed by Sokal & Rohlf (1995). Compari- Although the profile for monoamine con- sons between mean concentrations of mono- centrations in the SEG ofA. hentzi (5-HT > amine NTs for the various groups were OA > DA > NE < Epi) is in general agree- conducted using an analysis of variance (AN- ment with what little information is available OVA), followed post-hoc by a Duncan’s mul- on the neurochemistry of spiders, differences tiple range test at a significance level of0.05, in the NT profiles for spiders from different Significant differences between dominant and families have been reported (Florey 1967; subordinate males following aggressive en- Meyer et al. 1984; Meyer 1991) Similar con- counters were determined using an indepen- centrations were reported for NE and DA dent-samples t test {P < 0.05), from the brain ofthe theraphosid, Aphonopeh ma eutylenum Chamberlin 1918, although no RESULTS data were presented for 5-HT and OA (Meyer Brain weights for all spiders ranged from et al. 1984). In contrast, the brain ofPardosa 8.98-9.32 mg (mean: 9.11 ± 0.56 SE). Brain amentata (Clerck 1932) contained much high- protein/brain weight (%) ranged from 7.2-7.6. er concentrations of NE (174.9 pmol/mg ± An analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated 5.0 SE), a condition most likely associated that there were no differences in mean brain with the noradrenergic system of the optical 392 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOGY — Table 1. Concentrations of various monoamines (in nmol or pmol/mg protein) in the supraesophageal ganglia (SEG) ofAphonopelma hentzi following agonistic interactions between conspecific males. Brain analyses were conducted from tissue extracted from isolated control spiders, as well as from the brains of dominant and subordinant spiders removed 5 min (20 pairs; N = 40) 24 h {n = 20 pairs), and 48 h {n — 20 pairs) after an encounter trial. Data expressed as means; values in parentheses represent (± SE). Values followed by asterisks are significantly different than controls: ** (P < 0.01); * (P < 0.05). See text for details. Time after encounter Neurotransmitter Controls 5 min 24 h 48 h Serotonin (5-HT) (nmol/mg) 89.7 (13.2) Subordinate 42.8** (7.6) 51.3** (8.3) 92.2 (12.6) Dominant 66.5* (9.1) 73.9* (10.4) 86.3 (9.5) Octopamine (nmoi/mg) 56.9 (5.8) Subordinate 31.2** (4.9) 54.8 (8.1) 60.1 (10.6) Dominant 43.7* (7.7) 55.3 (5.2) 57.8 (8.2) Dopamine (pmol/mg) 22.4 (3.8) Subordinate 19.6 (5.1) 23.6 (7.1) 21.9 (5.5) Dominant 22.2 (7.8) 20.6 (3.5) 24.4 (6.2) Norepinephrine (pmol/mg) 15.3 (4.7) Subordinate 18.1 (5.8) 14.3 (2.9) 17.3 (6.6) Dominant 16.6 (4.4) 18.5 (3.1) 14.9 (4.1) Epinephrine (pmol/mg) 0.57 (0.2) Subordinate 0.61 (0.1) 0.55 (0.2) 0.58 (0.1) Dominant 0.52 (0.2) 0.63 (0.3) 0.56 (0.2) brain centers which are more highly devel- activate endocrine stress mechanisms in ver- oped in salticids (Meyer & Jehnen 1980). Ly- tebrates, which are thought to be mediated by cosids and agelenids also contained higher changes in CNS neurotransmitters brought levels of DA and NE as compared to thera- about primarily via activation of monoamin- & phosids, although not as high as salticids ergic systems (Ansell Bradley 1973; Ei- (Meyer 1991). chelman 1987). Eor example. Summers & The most pronounced changes in mono- Greenberg (1995) showed that 5-HT levels amine levels involved 5-HT. They occurred decreased significantly after one h and one among subordinate males 30 min after fight- day in the brains (diencephalon and non-optic ing, although 5-HT levels were reduced in lobe midbrain) oflizards (Anolis carolinensis) dominant males as well. Thus, fighting be- losing aggressive interactions. Similarly, no tween male spiders resulted in a decrease in changes were detected for NE and DA levels SEG serotonin levels. To our knowledge, this over this time interval. However, subordinate DA is the first demonstration ofan association be- males exhibited significantly lower levels tween monoaminergic activity and aggression after one week than did subordinates after one in spiders. Serotonin has been identified with h. Changes in the serotonergic content and aggressive behavior in other arthropods as turnover between individuals of different so- well. Injection of5-HT into lobsters and cray- cial status were found in the telencephalon fish caused them to elevate and flex their tails, and diencephalon of territorial vs. satellite which represent behavioral acts associated males in the lizard, Sceloporusjarrovi (Matter with the expression of dominance (Yeh et al. et al. 1998). In addition, the levels of5-HT in 1996). the telencephalon and diencephalon were These observations are interesting since a found to decrease significantly following similar reduction in brain serotonin levels ac- male-male aggression in rodents (Haney et al. companying fighting and territorial defense 1990; White et al. 1991) and fish (Winberg et has been reported for a number ofvertebrates. al. 1997). Indeed, it has been well established that a va- Immediately following aggressive defense riety of stimuli, including social interactions, of territories, territorial male lizards {S. jar- PUNZO & PUNZO—TARANTULA BRAIN MONOAMINES 393 rovi) exhibited higher Epi levels as compared with vertebrates is not possible since OA has to males that did not experience aggressive not been identified as a NT in this group. encounters (Matter et ah 1998). In contrast, no In conclusion, significant changes in brain comparable changes in Epi levels in the SEG concentrations of 5-HT and OA result from ofA. hentzi were observed after fighting. male-male agonistic encounters in tarantulas. Octopamine levels also decreased signifi- It has been well established that 5-HT is a cantly in males of A. hentzi exposed to ag- CNS monoamine involved in the expression gressive interactions. This suggests that an ac- of dominance and aggression in vertebrates, tivation of the octopaminergic system, in and the results of this study suggest that the addition to serotonergic activation, follows establishment ofsocial status in spiders causes aggression in spiders. This is not surprising changes in brain monoamine levels and may since OA appears to be central in eliciting the play a role in the elicitation ofcommunicative overall arousal response of arthropods (Krav- displays as well. Future studies should focus itz 1988; Corbet 1991), and elevated OA ac- on other species of spiders as well as other tivity has been shown to accompany stress arachnids in order to determine if similar (Downer & Hiripi 1993; Harris & Woodring changes in monoamine profiles are associated 1992), increased locomotor activity (Orchard with aggression in these groups. et al. 1993; Adamo et al. 1995), courtship ACKNOWLEDGMENTS (Downer & Hiripi 1993), and a wide range of systemic physiological responses including We are indebted to C. Bradford, R. Khatibi, respiration, gastrointestinal peristalsis, cardio- M. Harvey, and R. B. Suter for commenting acceleration, Malpighian tubule filtration, gly- on an earlier draft of the manuscript, B. Gar- cogenolysis, and pheromone production (Cor- man for consultation on statistical analyses, bet 1991) in insects. It has been further and J. Bottrell for assistance in the collection suggested that certain behaviorpatterns canbe of specimens. A University of Tampa Faculty triggered by the activation of specific octo- Development Grant and a Delo Foundation paminergic pathways in arthropods, an idea Research Grant (DFRG-10-204) to FP made known as the ‘orchestration hypothesis’ (Som- much of this work possible. & bati Hoyle 1984). For example, administra- LITERATURE CITED tion ofexogenous OA has been shown to trig- ger diurnal hyperactivity in nocturnal moths Adamo, S.A., C.E. Linn & R.R. Hoy. 1995. The (Shimizu & Fukamii (1981). Changes in OA role of neurohormonal octopamine during ‘fight levels in the CNS have been associated with or flight’ behavior in the field cricket Gryllus bi- maculatus. Journal ofExperimental Biology 198: ontogenetic shifts in specific behavioral acts 1691-1700. in social insects (Bicker & Menzel 1989; Adamo, S.A. & R. Chase. 1991. Central arousaland OA Brandes et al. 1990), and an increase in sexual responsiveness in the snail, Helixaspersa. activity was found in the brains of crickets Behavioral & Neural Biology 55:194-213. following aggressive interactions between Ansell, G.B. & P. Bradley. 1973. Macromolecules conspecifics (Adamo et al. 1995). and Behavior. Univ. Park Press, Maryland. Although most of the research on OA has Babu, K. S. 1985. Patterns ofarrangement andcon- focused on insects, some previous studies, in- nectivity in the central nervous system ofarach- nids, Pp. 3-19, In Neurobiology of Arachnids. cluding the present one, suggest that this (F. G. Barth, ed.). Springer-Verlag, Berlin. monoamine plays an important role in regu- Baerg, W.J. 1958. The Tarantula. University of lating the behavior of other arthropods as Kansas Press, Lawrence, Kansas. well. For example, the tail flip response, an Bicker, G. & R. Menzel. 1989. Chemical codes for integral behavioral component of the escape the control of behavior in arthropods. Nature response of crayfish, is enhanced by OA 337:33-39. (Bicker & Menzel 1989). The injection ofOA Brandes, C., M. Sugawa & R. Menzel. 1990. High into freely moving lobsters elicited submissive performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) measurement ofmonoamines in single honeybee body postures toward conspecifics (Kravitz 1988). The application of OA caused en- bwroariknesrrbeeveeaslsancdasqtue-esepnesciifnicApdiifsfemreelnUcfeesrab.etCwoeme-n gorged ticks to detach from theirhosts (Mason parativeBiochemestry & Physiology97C:53-57. 1986). With respect to OA, direct comparison Corbet, S.A. 1991. A fresh look at the arousal syn- 394 THE JOURNAL OF ARACHNOLOGY drome of insects. Advances in Invertertebrate Randall. 1951. Protein measurementwiththefol- Physiology 23:81-116. in reagent. Journal ofBiological Chemistry 192: Costa, EG. & F. Perez-Miles. 1992. Notes on mat- 265-275. ing and reproductive success of Ceropelma lon- Macmillan, C.S. & A.R. Mercer. 1987. An inves- gisternalis (Araneae,Theraphosidae) incaptivity. tigation of the role of dopamine in the antennal DowJnouerrn,alRo.fGA.rHa.ch&nolLo.gyH2ir0i:p1i2.9-1193933.. Biogenic CloobmepaorfatthievehoPnheyysbieoel,ogAypiAs m1e6l0l:i3fe5r9a-.36J6o.urnal of amines in insects. Pp. 23-38, In Insect Neuro- Mason, S.T. 1986. Catecholamines and Behaviour. chemistry and Neurophysiology. (A.B. Borkovek Cambridge Univ. Press, London. & M.J. Loeb, eds.). CRC Press, Boca Raton, Matter, J.M., P.J. Ronan & C.H. Summers. 1998. Florida. Central monoamines in free-ranging lizards: dif- Eichelman, B. 1987. Neurochemical and psycho- ferences associated with social roles and territo- pharmacological aspects of aggressive behavior. riality. Brain Behavior & Evolution 51:23-32. Pp. 697-704, In Psychopharmacology: Third Meyer, W. 1991. The biology of excitatory neuro- Generation ofProgress. (H. Y. Meltzer, ed.). Ra- transmitters in the central nervous system ofspi- ven Press, New York. ders. Histochemistry 59:177-189. Faber, D.B. & J.R. Bayliss. 1993. Effects of body Meyer, W. & R. Jehnen. 1980. The distribution of size on agonistic encounters between malejump- monoamine oxidase andbiogenic monoamines in ing spiders (Araneae: Salticidae). Animal Behav- the central nervous system of spiders (Arachni- iour 45:289-299. da: Araneida), Journal of Morphology 164:69- Florey, E. 1967. Neurotransmitters and modulators 81. in the animal kingdom. American Society ofEx- Meyer, W., C. Schlesinger, & H.M. Poehling. 1984. perimental Biology 26:1 164-1178. Comparative quantitative aspects of putative Guerra, A.A. & R.W. Steger & J.C. Guerra. 1991. neurotransmitters in the central nervous system The possible role of brain serotonin in seasonal of spiders (Arachnida: Araneida). Comparative dispersal behavior of the boll weevil. South- Biochemistry & Physiology 78C:357-362. western Entomologist 16:92-98. Minch, E.W. 1977. The Behavioral Biology of the Gupta, A.P. 1987. Arthropod Brain: Its Evolution, Tarantula, Aphonopelma chalcodes. Ph.D. Dis- Development, Structure, and Functions. Wiley sert., Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona. Interscience Pubis., New York. Moore, A.J.M., M.D. Breed & M.J. Moore, 1987. Haney, M., K. Noda, K. Kream & K.A. Miczek. The guard honeybee: ontogeny and behavioral 1990. Regional serotonin and dopamine activity: variability of workers performing a specialized sensitivity to amphetamines and aggressive be- task. Animal Behaviour 35:1159-1167. havior in mice. Aggessive Behavior 16:259-270. Murdock, L.L. & D. Oman. 1981. N-acetyldopa- Harris, J.W. & J, Woodring. 1992. Effects ofstress, mine in insect nervous tissue. Insect Biochem- age, season, and source colony on levels of oc- istry 1 1:161-166. topamine, dopamine, and serotonin in the hon- Orchard, L, J. Ramirez & A.B. Lange. 1993. Amul- eybee {Apis mellifera) brain. Journal of Insect tifunctional role for octopamine in locust flight. Physiology 38:29-35. Annual Review of Entomology 38:227-249. Honidge, G.A. 1965. Arthropoda: general anatomy, Punzo, F. 1985. Recent advances in behavioral plas- Pp. 801-964, In Structure and Function in the ticity in insects and decapod crustaceans. Florida Nervous System of Invertebrates. Vol. 2 (T. H. Entomologist 68:89-104. Bullock & G. A. Horridge, eds.). W. H. Freeman, Punzo, F. 1988a. Learning and localization ofbrain San Francisco, California. function in the tarantula spider, Aphonopelma Jaffe, K. 1980. Effects ofcycloheximide on protein chalcodes (Orthognatha, Theraphosidae). Com- synthesis and memory in preying mantids. Phys- parative Biochemistry & Physiology 89A:465- iology & Behavior 25:367-371. 470. Kerkut, G.A., M. French & R. Walker. 1970. The Punzo, F. 1988b. Physiological amino acids in the location of axonal pathways of identifiable neu- central nervous system ofthe tarantulas, Aphon- rons of Helix aspersa using procion yellow M- opelma chalcodes and Dugesiella echina (Or- 4R. Comparative Biochemistry & Physiology 32: thognatha: Theraphosidae). Comparative Bio- 681-690. chemistry & Physiology 90C:381-383. Kravitz, E.A. 1988. Hormonal control ofbehavior: Punzo, F. 1993. An analysis of free amino acids, amines and the biasing of behavioral output in neurotransmitters and enzymes in the nervous lobsters. Science 241:1775-1780. system of Solpugida (Arachnida). Comparative Lin, G. & C. Roelofs. 1992, The effects ofbiogenic Biochemistry & Physiology 106C:699-703. amines on learning in arthropods. Archives of Punzo, F. 1994. Changes in brain amine concentra- Insect Biochemistry & Physiology 20:359-366. tions associated with postembryonic develop- Lowry, O.H., N.J. Rosebrough, A. Farr & R.J. ment in the solpugid, Eremobatespalpisetidosus L PUNZO & PUNZO—TARANTULA BRAIN MONOAMINES 395 & Fichter (Solpugida, Eremobatidae). Journal of Shillington, C. P. Verrell. 1997. Sexual strategies Arachnology 22:1-4, of a North American ‘tarantula’ (Araneae: Ther- Punzo, F. 1996. Localization of brain function and aphosidae). Ethology 103:588-598. neurochemical events associated with learning in Shimizu, T. & J. Fukamii. 1981. Role of octopa- insects. Recent Trends in Comparative Biochem- mine in noctuid activity patterns. International & istry Physiology 2:9-16. Pest Control 23:166-175. Punzo, F. 1998a. The Biology of Camel-Spiders Sokal, B. & R.J. Rohlf. 1995. Biometry. 3rd ed. W. (Arachnida, Solifugae). Kluwer Acad. PubL, H. Freeman, New York. & Norwell, Massachusetts. Sombati, S. G. Hoyle. 1984. Generalizations of Punzo, F 1998b. Theeffects ofmaternal nestguard- specific behaviours in a locust by local release ing behavior by Eremobates marathoni Muma & into neuropil of the natural neuromodulator oc- Brookhart on the survivorship of offspring (So- topamine. Journal of Neurobiology 15:481-506. lifugae, Eremobatidae). Bulletin British Arach- Summers, C.H. & N. Greenberg. 1995. Activation nological Society 11:54-56. of central biogenic amines following aggressive Punzo, F. 1998c. Intraspecific male aggression in interaction in male lizards, Anolis carolinensis. Arenotherus joshuaensis Brookhart & Muma, Brain Behavior & Evolution 45:339-349. and Eremobates marathoni Muma (Solifugae, Wegerhoff, R. & O. Breidbach. 1995. Comparative Eremobatidae). Southwestern Naturalist 43:291- aspects of the chelicerate nervous system, Pp. 295. 159-179, In The Nervous Systems of Inverte- Punzo, E 2001. Neurochemical correlates of ago- brates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Ap- nistic interactions and dominance between males proach. (O. Breidbach & W. Kutsch, eds.). Birk- of the brown anole, Anolis sagrei. Florida Sci- hauser, Basel, Switzerland. entist, 64:131-139. White, S.M., R.F. Kucharik & J.A. Moyer. 1991. Punzo, E 2000b. Desert Arthropods: Life History Effects of serotonergic agents on isolation-in- Variations. Springer-Verlag, Heidelberg, Germa- duced aggression. Pharmacology Biochemistry ny. & Behavior, 39:729-736. Punzo, E & L. Henderson. 1999. Aspects of the Winberg, S., Y. Winberg & R.D. Fernald. 1997. Ef- natural history and behavioral ecology ofthe ta- fect of social rank on brain monoaminergic ac- rantula spiderAphonopelma hentzi (Girard 1854) tivity in cichlid fish. Brain Behavior&Evolution (Orthognatha, Theraphosidae). Bulletin British 49:230-236. Arachnological Society, 11:121-128. Yeh, S., R.A. Fricke & D.H. Edwards. 1996. The Punzo, F & J. Jellies. 1980. Effects of cyclohexi- effect of social experience on serotonergic mod- mide-induced protein synthesis inhibition on the ulation of the escape circuit ofcrayfish. Science phototactic behavior of Tenebrio molitor (Cole- 271:366-369. optera: Tenebrionidae) and Popilius disjunctus (Coleoptera: Passalidae). Journal of the Kansas Manuscript received 6August 2000, revised2 May Entomological Society 53:597-606. 200

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.