Odonatologica34(4):379-385 December 1,2005 Malebehaviourinthemaledimorphic damselflyParaphlebiaquinta Calvert (Zygoptera: Megapodagrionidae) E.González-Soriano¹ andA.Córdoba-Aguilar² 1DepartamentodeZoologia,InstitutodeBiologia,Universidad Nacional Autonoma deMexico, Apdo.Postal70-153,MX-04510 Mexico, D.E,Mexico [email protected] 2Instituto deEcologia,Universidad Nacional Autonoma deMexico,Apdo.Postal70-275, Circuitoexterior,Ciudad Universitaria,MX-04510 Mexico,D.E,Mexico [email protected] ReceivedMarch 19,2004/RevisedandAcceptedMarch8, 2005 P.quintaisatropicalsp.with 23morphs:theblack-winged(BW) 3and thehya- line-winged(HW) 3;here theirsexual behaviour isdescribed.Ingeneral,63seem tospendrelatively little timein flyingactivities.This maybeexplainedeitherby the inabilitytorecogniseconspecificsand,hence,engagein socialinteractions,orbythe reduced energeticreservesthatpreventthem fromengaginginexpensive activities. BW<J3weremoreaggressive and site-faithful than HW <J6.BW defended spaces containingdebris(plantandwood)againstconspecificswhile HWdid not.BW-BW, BW-HWand HW-HWaggressiveencounters werecommon.Despitetheirnon-ag- gressivenaturetowardBW <J<J, HW 33behaved aggressively when facedby HW 3 3.Thedistanceflown byeachmorphfrom 3 graspingofthe ? untilshe started ovipositionwasmeasured:HWflewlongerdistancesthanBW.Thesedifferencesbe- tween3 morphsarecomparedtothose found in Mnaisp.pruinosa,another 3 di- morphiczygopteran.Similartowhathappensinthatsp.,bothtacticsinP. quintaare possiblymaintained duetothe similar reproductiveand energeticcosts accrued by andbenefitspaidtoeachmorph. INTRODUCTION Maleterritoriality representsan ubiquitous phenomenon inmany dragonfly taxa(reviewed by CORBET, 1999).IntheZygoptera, territorialityis particularly prevalent amongseveraltaxaofthesuperfamily Calopterygoidea, such asinthe generaCalopteryx (e.g. WAAGE, 1973), Cora (e.g. GONZALEZ-SORIANO & VERDUGO-GARZA, 1984; FRASER & HERMAN, 1993), Heliocypha 380 E.Gonzalez-Soriano &A.Cordoba-Aguilar (ORR, 1996),Hetaerina(JOHNSON, 1962),Libellago(ORR, 1996),Mnais(e.g. HIGASHI, 1981), Platycypha (ROBERTSON, 1982), and Rhynocypha (ORR, 1996). Inotherzygopteran families, territorialbehaviouris a commonfeature amongsomegeneraofMegapodagrionidae, such asHeteragrion (GONZALEZ- -SORIANO & VERDUGO-GARZA, 1982) and Paraphlebia (GONZALEZ- -SOR1ANO & CORDOBA-AGUILAR, 2003); inProtoneuridae,such as Chlo- roneura (SRIVASTAVA & BABU, 1985) andNososticta(THOMPSON, 1990); in Platystictidae, such as Palaemnema(GONZALEZ-SORIANOet al., 1982); in Pseudostigmatidae, such as Megaloprepus (FINCKE, 1984);in Platycnemidi- dae,such as Platycnemis (HEYMER, 1966);andinsomeCoenagrionidae, such as Megalagrion (MOORE, 1983)andPseudagrion (MESKIN, 1986). Malesofthe orientalgenusMnaisare dimorphic. One morph bearspigment- ed wings and shows territorial behaviour, while the other morph has hyaline wings andbehaves assubordinate, non-territorialindividuals(WATANABE & TAGUCHI, 1990;SIVA-JOTHY & TSUBAKI, 1989a, 1989b). Thisphenome- nonisnotconfinedtothistaxon. Malesinsomespecies oftheNewWorldgenus Paraphlebia, such as P. quintaand P. zoe,arealsodimorphic(GONZALEZ-SO- RIANO, 1997;GONZALEZ-SORIANO &CORDOBA-AGUILAR,2003). In P. quinta,forexample, one morph is theblack-winged (BW)malewhiletheother is thehyaline-winged(HW) male.BWmalesare usually largerindividuals, bear- ing ablacktransverse band onthewing tips, whileHW males are smaller, with entirely clear wings, resembling thoseof females(GONZALEZ-SORIANO & CORDOBA-AGUILAR,2003). How thetwo morphs are maintainedis uncer- tain.InMnais, themaintenanceofbothmorphs ispossibly dueto thebalanced reproductive benefitsandcostsofeachphenotype (SIVA-JOTHY &TSUBAKI, 1989a;PLAISTOW& TSUBAKI,2000). Herewepresentasummaryofthebehaviouralcomponentsofthemalesexual activitiesofP. quinta. Our firstaimis to provide a descriptiveframeworkofthe behaviourofeach morph.Additionally, wepresent apreliminary analysisofthe cost ofaccompanying afemaleby each morph, whichis discussedintheframe- workofthe possible fertilisationsuccess achievedby bothtypesofmales. MATERIALAND METHODS Field work wascarriedoutatasmallseepage, dividedinto36 quadrants(2x2m),attheEstacion deBiologiaTropical Los Tuxtlas in the state ofVeracruz, Mexico (95°04-95°09’Wand 18°34’- 18°36’N).Mostobservations wereperformedon23-26August 1984 and 19 July-15August 1986. In bothyears, malesand females wereindividuallymarked ontheirwings, usinganindelible inkpen, allocatingdistinctive numbers. Inboth years, observations of 30-60 min duration offocal males werecarriedout.In 1986,dailycensusesweremadeandthepositionofeachmarkedindividual was located onascalemap. Bythisprocedure,dailymovementsofeachindividual weremeasured and trackedwith relativeaccuracy. On those datesand alsoon8-11 July 1988 and 19-23 August 1990,werecorded the trajectories followed byeach morphfromthemalegraspingofthefemaleprothorax(attheonset ofcopulation) Malebehaviour inParaphlebiaquinta 381 tothebeginningofthefirstoviposition bout. For theuseofparametric tests,wetransformed thosedata that wereamenable forsuchmanipu- lation. Mann Whitneytestsarereferred toasUtests. Resultsarepresentedasmeans± STDunless indicated otherwise. RESULTS Dailycensuses were carriedout during 18consecutive days(19 July-5 August 1986).Thenumberofindividualsofbothmalemorphs founddailyfluctuatedbe- tween33and41individuals(36.5 ± 2.59). However, malesremainedinthestudy sites for variableperiods (in days;BWmales, 11.5 ± 6.5, N =35; HW males, 4.8 ± 4.8, N = 45).For BW males, theminimumnumberof days anindividualwas foundinthesame site(during amaximumof a 17-day period) was 1,whilethe maximum(butnot necessarily consecutive days)was 17(N =35). Morethan50% ofBWmaleswerefoundin thesamesitefor16-17days,andonly 14%were seen onthe dayofmarking. For HW males,theminimumof dayspresentinthesame sitewas 1,whilethe maximumwas 16(N =45). Only4.6%oftheHWmalesre- mainedbetween 15-16days,65% were concentratedwithina greaterintervalof 1-15days,andalargenumber(almost30%)werepresentonly onthedayofmark- ing. ThesedifferencesindicatethatBWmalesstayedforlongerperiodsthanHW malesintheplaces ofmarking(Utest= 1878.5, P<0.0001). Asforfemales, they werefound fewerdays (0.5 ± 0.8)compared to BW (U test= 3019, P < 0.0001) andHW males(Utest=3653.3,P<0.0001).Theminimumnumberofdaysthey returnedto thegeneral areawas 1 day, whilethemaximumwas 4. Thefourmainflying activities, performed by the twomalemorphs at the re- productive site were;changes ofperching site, aggressive flights(mainly directed atsame-sexconspecifics), investigatory flights(short-duration flightsofafewcm withnoclearexplanation), and patrolling flights(in whichtheanimalcovers the areaarounditsperching site)(Tab. I). Flying activity wasfairlylowincompari- son withtotaltime inactivity(0.87% forBW and0.38%forHW). Theonly sig- nificantdifferenceinflyingactivitiesbetweenmorphs wasintheaggressive flight: Table I Accountofthedifferentflyingactivitiesatthe reproductivesite byP.quintablack-winged(BW)andhyaline-winged(HW)males Perching site Aggressive Investigatory Patrolling change flight flight flight BW HW BW HW BW HW BW HW Occurrence 20 17 15 1 55 20 20 1 Meanduration 2.0 2.2 4.3 5 1.6 1.2 0.8 0.4 %occurrenceinrelation to all flights 18.2 43.5 13.6 2.6 50 51.2 18.2 2.6 %durationin relation toall flights 22.9 25 49.4 56.8 18.3 13.6 9.3 4.5 MannWhitneyUtest (P) 57.5 (>0.05) 69.5 (<0.05) 66.0 (>0.05) 66.0(>0.05) 382 E.Gonzalez-Soriano &A.Cordoba-Aguilar BW males made more of theseflightsthan HW males (U test = 69.5, P < 0.05). No further differenceswere foundinchanges of perch- ing site (U test = 57.5, P > 0.05)orinvestigatoryandpa- trolling flights (both U test = 66.0,P> 0.05).Inperched males,threemainbehaviours wereobserved:wingclapping (BW: 14.2± 12.8;HW; 12.60 ± 6.8), wing warning (BW: 0.2±0.5; HW;0.4±0.9)and abdominalcleaning(BW:6.9 ± 9.2; HW; 4.2± 3.5).Sam- ple size forallcomparisons: Fig. 1.ExamplesofdistancesflownbyeachP. quintamorph whilewith afemale(measuredfromthe onsetofcopulation N =8forBWmalesand5for tothefirstovipositionbout):(a-b)twoBWmales;1(c-d)two HW males. Themorphs dif- F1Wmales; — ®onsetofflight;—xfirstovipositionbout;— feredinnoneofthesebehav- dotsrepresentquadrats(2x2m). iours(wingclapping,Utest= 57.0; wingwarning, Utest=56.0;abdominalcleaning,U test=53.0;allP>0.05). Agonistic interactionsoccurredbothbetween(BW-HW)andwithin(BW-BW and HW-HW) morphs. Most inter-morph interactionsconsisted basically ofa chasing flightofHW malesby BW malesinwhich invariably theHW male left theterritory(25 outof25 cases). Inthefewoccasions thatescalatedflights were observed, they tookplace mainlybetweenneighbouring BW males. Thesecon- testsconsistedofafrontal,slowly-ascending interaction.Inmostcases,theowner remainedin theterritory(3 outof 5 times). As forHW-HW encounters,the in- truderwaschasedandexcluded fromtheterritory(5 outof6 times). Inthis type ofencounters,physical contact was observed. Interactionswerenotonlyintraspecific. P. quintamalesalsochasedotherdam- selflies, especially Palaemnemapaulitaba and Hetaerinasempronia. BW males had9 interactions withP.paulitabaandonewithH.sempronia, whileHWmales had 12withP.paulitaba andone withH. sempronia. P.paulitabahas clearwings withblack tips, H. sempronia clearwings withred bases and smallbrownspots atthe hindwingtips. TRAJECTORIESOFPAIRS. — Therewas adifferenceinrelationto malemorph in thedistance travelledby thepair to thetimeatwhichthe femalefirst ovipos- ited: HWmalesflewalongerdistance(N=4; 10.65±7.1 m)thanBWmalesdid (N =5; 1.28 ± 1.1 m)(t on log transformeddata=-3.36, P= 0.015). Someex- 7 amples ofthis differenceare shownin Figure 1. Malebehaviour inParaphlehiaquinta 383 DISCUSSION P. quinta malemorphs exhibit clearbehaviouraldifferences, whichpresuma- blyrelateto theirdifferentmate-seeking tactics. TheBW morph ismoreaggres- sive (reflected in the numberof aggressive flights andtheir chasing HW males butnot being chased by them)and site-attachedthanthe HW morph. An in- teresting convergence to this dimorphism is thesituation inthe Japanese Mnais (SIVA-JOTHY &TSUBAK1,1989a, 1989b;WATANABE& TAGUCHI,1990; PLAISTOW&TSUBAKI,2000), Wheretheterritorialmorph isalsoaggressive, whilethenon-territorialmorph seems toavoidagonistic encounters. Addition- ally, theterritorialmorph of M. pruinosa requires moremass than non-territo- rialmales, anindicationthatthe territorial tactic is morecostly. Our results in- dicatethattheBW malesalsoseemto incurhighercosts, as they haveto defend aterritory. Thiscost maybebalanced bythe reproductive benefits. In P. quinta, copulations by the non-territorialmorph are fewer, they are more frequently genitalicallyinterrupted and, during these interruptions,they are likely toresult in moresperm-ejection events by females (GONZALEZ-SORIANO & COR- DOBA-AGUILAR,2003). Giventheseresults, someinteresting avenues forre- search in thesespecies are:(a)toelucidatethegeneticbasisofmaledimorphism (to determinetheenvironmentalroleingiving risetoeachmorph); — and(b) to measurethelong-termreproductive success ofeach morph.Theseaimswillshed lightonhowboth morphs are undergoing selection. BEHAVIOURAL REPERTOIREOFP.QUINTA Territorialbehaviourin P. quinta shows two unusual featuresin comparison withotherterritorialzygopterans,viz. malesarerelativelyinactive,whilethenon- territorialmorph isfairlyaggressive towardnon-territorialconspecifics. BothP quintamorphs showedextremely reducedflightactivity: nomorethan 0.9%oftotaltime.Incomparison, malesofthecoenagrionid Pseudagrion hage- niinvest 6.7% oftheir territorialoccupation in flight (MESKIN, 1986), while HIGASHI (1981) pointed out that “territorialmales of M. pruinosa seemed to havealmost notimeperching on theirperch sites for pursuing theintruding males”. Also unusual for otherterritorial zygopterans butconcurrent with the above:P quinta engaged inrelatively fewmale-maleinteractions, withmost of themlasting only afewseconds(foracomparison seeWAAGE, 1988). Atleast two explanations canbeputforwardforthis;(a) thereis notmuch lightintheir habitats, whichpossiblyaffects visualrecognition ofconspecifics; — and/or (b) malesmaypossess areducedamountofenergetic reserves which preventsthem frommovingtoomuch. Itisalsointerestingtonoticethatalthough HW malesalwaysshowedasubmis- sivebehaviourin thepresenceofa BW male, they behavedaggressively against 384 E.Gonzalez-Soriano &A.Cordoba-Aguilar malesoftheirownform.Possibly, thesubmissivebehaviouronlyemergesinthe faceofaBWmale. Actually, according to theunpublished observationsonare- latedspecies, P. zoe, attheendoftheflightseason,whennoBWmalesarepresent, HW malesaggressively defendterritories(A. Romo-Beltran, pers.comm.) This indicatesthatthe submissivebehaviourof HW males occurs only towards BW males. Perhaps the valueofa territoryis “appreciated” by HW males, butthey do notdispute itwith BWmalesbecausethey are notableto undertakeacostly fight (forasimilarrationaleseePLAISTOW& TSUBAKI,2000). Thiscost can beaffordedonly against HW males. Pairs formed by HW males flew longer thanthoseformedby BW malesdur- ing andaftercopulation. Thisexpenditure isnot meaningless. Thedelay incop- ulationand/oroviposition couldaffectfitnessofbothpartners in at leastthree non-exclusiveways:(1) agreaterrisk of predation; — (2) anincreasednumber ofattacks fromconspecific males whichmay cause tandemdisruption; - and (3) areduced dispositionto oviposit byfemales.Wehaveindirectevidenceof the lasttwo. Although not measured, during theincreasedHW male tandemdura- tion, thecouple was morefrequentlyapproached by conspecific maleswhichoc- casionally induced tandembreakage. Theincreased attack may be“perceived” by the femaleandaffects her disposition to oviposit. This mayexplain why fe- malesspend lesstimein oviposition aftermating withan HW malethanwitha BWmale(GONZALEZ-SOR1ANO &CORDOBA-AGUILAR.2003), though thereisalso the possibility that HW malesmaynot be good mate guarders, as showninsomeotherspecies. Thereducedoviposition timemaybeactuallytrans- latedto the numberof eggslaid. In M. pruinosa there is a direct relationship betweenthenumberofeggslaidand timespentin oviposition (WATANABE & TAGUCHI, 1990). Inodonates, the lastmaleinseminatesusually mosteggs (CORDOBA-AGUILAR etal., 2003). Ifthese relationships holdforP. quinta, thenHW malesmay seetheirfertilisation success reduced as a consequenceof reducedoviposition bouts. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS EG-SthanksROLANDOMENDOZA,JOSELUISVILLAREAL,and LIZBETHHERAS for helpduringthefieldwork. REFERENCES CORBET,P.S., 1999. Dragonflies:behavior andecologyofOdonata. Cornell Univ.Press, Ithaca/NY. CORDOBA-AGUILAR,A„ E.UHIACASTRO&A.CORDERORIVERA, 2003. Spermcom- petitionin Odonata (Insecta):theevolution offemalespermstorage and rival’s spermdis- placementability. J Zool. 261: 381-398. FRASER, A.M. &T.B. HERMAN, 1993. Territorial and reproductivebehaviour in a sympatric species complexoftheneotropicaldamselflyCoraSelys(Zygoptera:Polythoridae).Odona- Malebehaviour inParaphlebiaquinta 385 tologica22:411-429. GONZALEZ-SORIANO, E., 1997.Paraphlebiaquinta.In:E.Gonzalez-Soriano,R.Dirzo&R.C. Vogt,[Eds],Hisloria natural deLos Tuxtlas,,pp. 332-334,UNAM,Mexico. GONZAlEZ-SORIANO,E„M.VERDUGO-GARZA&R.NOVELO-GUTIERREZ, 1982.Re- productivebehavior ofPalaemnema desiderataSelys. Adv. Odonatol. 1:55-62. GONZALEZ-SORIANO, E.& M,VERDUGO-GARZA, 1982. Studies onneotropicalOdonata: The adult behavior ofHeteragrionalienum Williamson (Odonata: Megapodagrionidae). Foliaenl.mex.52: 3-15. GONZALEZ-SORIANO, E. & M.VERDUGO-GARZA, 1984. Estudios en odonatos neotropi- cales II:NotassobreelcomportamientoreproductivedeCoramarinaSelys(Odonata:Poly- thoridae).Foliaenl. mex.62: 3-15. GONZAlEZ-SORIANO, E.&A.CORDOBA-AGUILAR, 2003,Sexual behaviourinParaphlebia quintaCalvert(Odonata:Megapodagrionidae):maledimorphismandapossibleexampleof odonatefemalecontrol. Odonatologica32: 345-353. HEYMER,A., 1966. Etudes comparees du comportementinne dePlatycnemisacutipennisSelys 1841 et dePlatycnemis latipes Rambur 1842 (Odon.Zygoptera). Annls Soc ent. Fr. (N.S.) 2:39-73. HIGASHI,K., 1981. A descriptionof territorial and reproductivebehaviours in Mnais pruinosa Selys(Odonata:Calopterygidae).J.Fac. lib.Arts, SagaUniv. 13: 123-140. MESKIN,I., 1986.Territorialbehaviour inPseudagrionhagenitropicanumPinhey(Zygoptera:Coe- nagrionidae).Odonatologica15: 157-167. MOORE,N.W.,1983.Territorialbehaviour inthegenusMegalagrion(McLachlan)(Zygoptera:Coe- nagrionidae).Odonatologica12:87-92. ORR, A.G., 1996. Territorial and courshipdisplaysinBorneanChlorocyphidae(Zygoptera).Odo- natologica25: 119-141. PLAISTOW,S.J.&Y.TSUBAKI, 2000. Aselectivetrade-offfor territorialityandnon-territoriality inthepolymorphicdamselfiyMnaiscostalis.Proc. R. Soc. Land. (B)267:969-975. ROBERTSON,H.M.,1982.MatingbehaviouranditsrelationshiptoterritorialityinPlatycyphaca- ligata(Selys)(Odonata:Chlorocyphidae).Behaviour 79: 11-27. S1VA-JOTHY, M.T. & Y.TSUBAKi, 1989a. Variation in copulationduration in Mnais pruinosa pruinosaSelys (Odonata:Calopterygidae).1.Alternative matesecuringtactics and sperm precedence.Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 24: 39-45. SIVA-JOTHY, M.T. & Y.TSUBAKI, 1989b. Variation in copulationduration in Mnais pruinosa pruinosaSelys(Odonata:Calopterygidae).2.Causal factors.Behav.Ecol. Sociobiol. 25:261- -267. SRIVASTAVA,B.K.&B.S.BABU, 1985.Onsomeaspectsofreproductivebehaviour inChloroneura quadrimaculata(Rambur)(Zygoptera:Protoneuridae).Odonatologica 14:219-226. THOMPSON, D., 1990. OnthebiologyofthedamselflyNososticta kalumburu Watson &Theis- chinger(Zygoptera:Protoneuridae).Biol. J.Linn.Soc. 40: 347-356. WAAGE, J.K.,1973.Reproductivebehavior anditsrelation toterritorialityinCalopteryxmaculata (Beauvois). Behaviour47: 240-256. WAAGE, J.K., 1988. Confusion overresidency and theescalation ofdamselflyterritorial disputes. Anim. Behav. 36:586-595. WATANABE,M.&M.TAGUCHI,1990. Matingtactics andmalewingdimorphisminthedamsel- fly,M.pruinosacostalis Selys(Odonata:Calopterygidae).J. Ethol.8: 129-137.