ebook img

Limbu nous autres and First Person morphology PDF

10 Pages·0.167 MB·Nepali-English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Limbu nous autres and First Person morphology

preprint, Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 24.1:145-156 (spring 2001) special issue: Person and evidence in Himalayan languages, part 2 corrected in accordance with errata: LTBA 24:2:255 (autumn 2001) LIMBU NOUS AUTRES AND 1ST PERSON MORPHOLOGY1 Boyd Michailovsky Lacito/CNRS, Paris In a number of Eastern Kiranti languages, verb forms for 1st person patient scenarios have been replaced, in some cases optionally, by a construction in which a word for “man” represents the 1st person patient, and the verb form lacks the usual 1st person markers to index the object. This micro-areal phenomenon has been described by Ebert (1991:86-88; 1994:28-29) and named “impersonal 1st person patient marking”. The precise extent of such marking, its integration into the verbal morphology, and the particular morphemes used vary from language to language. I will discuss this phenomenon in the Maiwa-Mewa Khola dialect, with reference to Limbu 1st person marking in general, including some evidence from the notes of Brian Hodgson (see accompanying article). In Limbu, two morphemes are used in this construction: yapmi and napmi. It is not entirely clear from the sources if these form a doublet in all dialects, but this appears to be the case in the dialect of the Maiwa and Mewa Khola valleys (MM) on which I did fieldwork in 1987-88; they will not be distinguished here. Their uses fall into two categories: (1) as an independent quasi-pronoun meaning ‘a person, someone, someone else’, and (2) as a 1st person patient marker. I will begin with the first use, of which the second appears to be a grammaticalized extension. Independent yapmi Table 1 shows the definitions of yapmi and napmi in the available Limbu dictionaries and vocabularies. The definitions center around Nepali mìnche and English ‘man, person’, but Subba and van Driem both cite the meaning ‘other, someone else’. In fact, there is another Limbu word (possibly of Indo-Aryan origin), mÁna, glossed ‘man, human being’ by Subba, which is used when definite reference (‘that man’), or reference to the quality of a person as a human being (‘what kind of man’, example (1) below) is intended. This word appears also in the examples and texts of Weidert and Subba (e.g. 1985:121), van Driem, and the LSI, sources in which I have found only one example (see note 2 below) of independent yapmi~napmi, apart from van Driem’s vocabulary entry. Example (1) is the only example in my materials in which yapmi functions as transitive A. It is marked in the ergative case. In this respect it does not look like a pronoun, since pronouns in Limbu do not take case markers: 1 Abbreviations: CTR counter-expectancy; DF definite; ERGative; EMPHatic; INFinitive; INSTru- mental; NEGative; NOMinalizer; PAst; PLural; PResent; PV preverb; Question; REFLexive; TOPic. 1/10 H. A. R.. Senior (1908): Man: mané; yapmí; generic yapmísí [i.e. ‘person.PL, man.PL’]. I. S. Chemjong (n.d. ?1960): yìpmó man; yìpmó khumì to kidnap [i.e. ‘man to.steal’], yìpmó phoÙmì, to agitate [i.e. ‘man to.rouse’], yìpmó keseppì, murderer [i.e. ‘man killer’], etc. nìpmó man. A. Weidert and B. Subba (1985): [neither form is in the vocabulary; yapmi appears in verb conjugations]. G. van Driem (1987:477,545): yaÊpmi see naÊpmi. naÊpmi n. man. naÊpmi pro. 1) someone else, other; 2) first person patient in 2→1 forms. B. B. Subba (1989): yapmi man, person; napmi other person. [in Nepali, both are glossed mìnis ‘person, man’.] Khel Raj Yonghang (1995): yapmi mìnis [Nep. ‘person, man’, p. 58]. napmi mìnis [p. 127]. Table 1: yapmi in dictionaries of Limbu (1) “abhedaÑba mÁna-si” yapmi-llÀ ammÀt [J4:10] what.kind person-3PL.PNother-ERG say.PR.3p→1pi Others will wonder what kind of people we are. In my other examples, yapmi is used as a possessive modifier, ‘someone else’s’, without genitive case marking2. The possessed noun does not bear the usual 3rd person possessive prefix (ku-, etc.); apparently yapmi has this function: (2) yo yapmi him-thikk-Àtmu tyÀaÑ… [Hb5:50] below other house-one-in arrive.PA.1s I arrived at someone’s house down below. (3) “lo! theaÑ i~mÀkheksu” pha “kha-n ÀmphÀllÀ ni?” hey why PV~tie.3p→3 COMP that-DF like.this EMPH – “napmi himdaÑm'-Àn mu taËru khunÀ” [A59] – other wife-DF CTR bring.3s→3s he “Hey! Why did they tie him up like this?” – “He took another man’s wife!” The possessive construction in (2) and (3) is that of a pronoun. Compare, for example, khunchi him ‘theird house’ with the full nominal construction: (4) kuÑ-gwa-rÀ ku-him-mu ‘at his uncle’s house’ 3s-uncle-GEN 3s-house-LOC There is one curious use of yapmi as a relational noun: ku-yapmi ‘his/her spouse’, reminiscent of English other or (especially) significant other. I did not record yapmi as representing an indefinite in expressions like Chemjong’s ‘murderer’, etc. (table 1), except in one compound, napmi-sokma ‘index finger’ 2 But cf. van Driem's example (1987:45) naÊpmi-rÀ-n (other-GEN-DF) ‘someone else’s’. 2/10 [“other-pointer”]. Often, as has been remarked in other Kiranti languages, the indefinite person is represented by the 1st person inclusive, for example in a-mu- mu-ba [1.incl-poison-poison-NOM] ‘poisonous’ [lit. ‘which poisons uspi’] or a-tuk [1.incl-be.ill] ‘one gets ill, you get ill’. The above are essentially the only examples I recorded of independent yapmi. In all it functions as a kind of indefinite quasi-pronoun ‘someone, someone else’, while mÁna serves as the full noun ‘person, man’. (In Nepali, the noun mìnche can be used in both senses.) I suspect this is the case in other Limbu dialects as well. Note, however, that the same word yapmi in Athpare and in Yamphu, languages closely related to Limbu, is clearly a full noun ‘person, man’ (Ebert 1997, Rutgers 1998:95). 1st person object yapmi The other use of yapmi is as a 1st person non-singular object pronoun. In this construction, the transitive verb has what looks like an intransitive form because it shows no object agreement (unless one takes yapmi itself as an agreement marker): it bears neither of the 1st person markers a- or -igÀ nor the 3rd person person object-marking suffix -u. This is the only condition under which a transitive verb has the indicative form PA-À, the regular 3rd person past for intransitives (table 2). Only past stem forms have been recorded in the Maiwa- Mewa dialect. In the Maiwa-Mewa and Phedappe dialects, this construction coexists with the more common regular finite transitive indicative forms, which have personal agreement affixes showing agreement with the 1st person non-singular object, often in the same sentence: (5) anigÀ nurik mÀmÀttigÀ-aÑ cißßhó yapmi mÀhaktÀ [K14] wepe well do.3p→1pe-and letter us send.3p They treated us well and they sent us letters. [cf. (anigÀ) mÀhaktigÀ ‘they sent to uspe’]. In the following, the second object phudoÑ intervenes between yapmi and the verb, something which would be impossible with the prefixed pronominal agreement markers a-, kÀ-, mÀ-. Note the impersonal form mÀbÀrÀ, past in form, in parallel with the non-past personal form amÀmbinÀn. (6) anigÀ ammuËt mÀËnn-i?ammuËttÀ-aÑ khunchi ni uspe call.PR.3p→1pi no-Q call.PA.3p→1pi-and they EMPH yapmi phudoÑ mÀbÀrÀ phÁgÁrÁ allÁ ani-aÑ kÁ us gift give.3p if now wepi-also TOP egaÑ uËtmasiÑ uhi khan purai pimasi poksÀ… after call.INF.PL that.same that fully give.INF.PL become.PA.3s khunchi khann-Àn amÀmbinÀn phÁgÁrÁ… [K151,154] they that-DF give.PR.3p→1pi.NEG if They call us, right? If they call uspi and give us gifts, then later wepi too must call them and give them fully the same… But if they don’t give uspi gifts … 3/10 Transitive: O → ↓A 1s 1di 1de 1pi 1pe 1s KEY: 1di In each cell, the non-past form appears over the past, where they are distinct. PR = present stem 1de PA = past stem 1pi N represents a nasal morphophoneme, homorganic with the stem-final; realized as a glottal stop or hiatus after a vowel. 1pe kÀ-PR-Na 2s kÀ-PA-aÑ akÀ-PR 2d akÀ-PA-À 2p PR-Na a-PR-si PR-sigÀ a-PR 3s PA-igÀ PA-aÑ a-PA-Àsi PA-ÀsigÀ a-PA-À 3d mÀ-PR-Na am-PR-si mÀ-PR-sigÀ am-PR mÀ-PA-igÀ mÀ-PA-aÑ am-PA-Àsi mÀ-PA-ÀsigÀ am-PA-À 3p Intransitive: S → PR-Na a-PR-si PR-sigÀ a-PR PA-igÀ PA-aÑ a-PA-Àsi PA-ÀsigÀ a-PA-À Reflexive: S → PR-NasiÑÑa a-PR-nÀsi PR-nÀsigÀ a-PR-Nasi PR-NasigÀ Table 2: Maiwa-Mewa Khola affirmative indicative verb paradigm. Transitive Intrans. Reflexive ↓A/S O →1S 1D/P 3S 3D/P — — 2S PA-aÑÑÀ PA-À PA-À PR-siÑÑÀ 2D a-PA-À PA-ÀsÀ PA-ÀsÀ PR-nÀsÀ 2P a-PA-innÀ PA-ammÀ PA-ams(imm)À PA-innÀ PR-NasinnÀ Table 3: Mewa Khola affirmative imperatives (7) him-mu pa-ma-rÀ-aÑ napmi mÀndÁrÀn kusiÑ house-infather-mother-ERG-alsous scold.3p.NEG PV mÀnniËttun. ÀmbhÀllÀ kÁ khÀni kÁ kÀipsi kÀdÀi-aÑ kÁ. [K243] know.3p→3.NEG so TOP youp TOP sleep.2parrive.2p-and TOP 4/10 Transitive: O → ↓A 2s 2d 2p 3s 3d 3p 1s PR-nÀ PR-nÀsiÑ PR-(nÀ)niÑ PA-uÑ PA-uÑsiÑ a-PR-su a-PR-susi 1di a-PA-usi PR-nÀsigÀ PR-sugÀ PR-susigÀ 1de PA-usigÀ 1pi a-PA-um PA-umsim PR-NasigÀ (PR-nÀsigÀ) 1pe PA-umbÀ PA-umsigÀ 2s kÀ-PA-u kÀ-PA-usi kÀ-PR-su kÀ-PR-susi 2d kÀ-PA-usi 2p kÀ-PA-um kÀ-PA-umsi kÀ-PR kÀ-PR-si 3s kÀ-PA-i PA-u PA-usi kÀ-PA-À kÀ-PA-Àsi PR-su PR-susi 3d kÀm-PR kÀm-PR-si PA-usi kÀm-PA-i kÀm-PA-À kÀm-PA-Àsi 3p mÀ-PA-u mÀ-PA-usi Intransitive: S → kÀ-PR kÀ-PR-si PR PR-si mÀ-PR kÀ-PA-i kÀ-PA-À kÀ-PA-si PA-À PA-si mÀ-PA-À Reflexive S: → kÀ-PR-siÑ kÀ-PR-nÀsi kÀ-PR-Nasi PR-siÑ PR-nÀsi mÀ-PR-siÑ Table 2 (cont.) [We used to sneak back into the house after dancing and go to work normally, so] at home our parents didn’t scold us – they didn’t know. But you guys just sleep after getting home. [cf. (anigÀ) mÀndÁrigÀn ‘they do/did not scold uspe’]. In the following, yapmi bears the definite singular/topic marker -n, again impossible for a pronominal prefix: (8) khÁmbhÀaÑ ni kha simal-khe-llÀ kÁ yapmi mÀdÁktÀn then EMPH that kapok-yam-ERG TOP us support.3s.NEG ekdam muyaÑ – yapmi-n muyÀ simal-khe-llÀ very intoxicate.PA.3→1s – us-DF intoxicate.PA.3skapok-yam-ERG 5/10 yapmi-n muyÀ-aÑ kÁ ekdam pÀÌasigÀ [Hb2:00] us-DF intoxicate.PA.3s-and TOP very vomit.REFL.1pe But afterward that manioc wasn’t good for us. I was badly intoxicated – we were poisoned by the manioc. It poisoned us and wepe vomited a lot. (9) na-nu sardìr-Àn phÀrÀ. phÀrÀ-aÑ kÁ yapmi teÌrÀ there-from contractor-DF come.PA.3scome.PA.3s-andTOP us take.3s khÁmbha bhìÛì yapmi thÀktÀ-aÑ kÁ khÁmbhÀaÑ Siliguri yammu then pay us pay.3s-and TOP then S. again pegigÀ [Hb2:20] go.1pe The contractor came from over there [in Assam] and took us away from there. He paid us our wages and wepe went back to Siliguri. [cf. teÌrigÀ, thÀktigÀ ‘he took uspe, he paid uspe’] In the following, napmi acts like a pronominal prefix in separating the pre- verb sen from the root: (10) lo khÀni akkhe poksÀ-aÑ kÀnuËksi phÀaÑ kÁ well youp how become.3s-and return.2p COMP TOP yammu sen napmi mÀdosÀ-llÀ ni [G10:10/132] again PV us ask.3p-when EMPH When they asked us, “Well! Why did you come back?” … (11) paisì rokwì mÀdzogu-aÑ kÁ yapmi mÀmbÀrÀn [Ia4:00] moneystop do.3p→3s-and TOP us give.3p.NEG They stopped the money and and they didn’t give it to us. [cf. (anigÀ) mÀmbÀrigÀn ‘they did not give to uspe’] (12) khÁn-haÌ-rÀ galla napmi mÀdzogÀ napmi mÀteÌrÀ that-PL-ERG recruitment us do.3p us take.3p bhartó poÑsÀ khÁmbhÀllÀ belì [O44 (Tembe)] enlistment effect.3p then time At that time they [soldiers on leave] used to recruit us and take us off and get us enlisted. In the above examples from the MM dialect, yapmi~napmi is seen to occur sporadically for a 1st non-singular object with a 3rd person agent. Where forms in the surrounding context distinguish exclusive from inclusive, these are exclusive, except in 6, which is rather anomalous: after the first word (‘wepe’) the speaker switches from exclusive to an apparently empathetic inclusive. The context is notionally exclusive — a younger man explaining to a older one how the new generation functions. yapmi never represents a 1st singular argument, and it does not appear in 2→1 scenarios or imperatives, although there are many of these in the corpus. Other dialects Descriptions of other Limbu dialects present somewhat different conditions for the use of 1st person yapmi. For the dialect of Phedap, Van Driem writes: 6/10 “The first person morpheme <a-> is often dropped from 2→1 forms and replaced by the word <naÊpmi> which immediately precedes the verb. The word naÊpmi also occurs as a non-bound morph in the meaning ‘someone else’, whence the restricted usage to signal a first person actant probably derives.” (1987:78). There are two major differences with the MM dialect: use for 2→1 (including imperatives) rather than 3→1 forms, and use for 1st singular object, even for 2s→1s (where, incidentally, it cannot be said to replace the prefix a-, because this prefix does not occur in the regular finite form). The impersonal verb form may be either past or non-past. Weidert and Subba (Panchthar dialect) present forms with yapmi as the only ones for most 1st person object scenarios except those with specifically INCLUSIVE 1st person objects (necessarily with 3rd person agent). As in Phedappe, the impersonal verb form may be either past or non-past. Without going into a fully detailed analysis, yapmi forms are used in the following parts of the paradigm (1985:60 and paradigms 43, 68, 69; paradigm 42 differs in minor details): (1) 2→1 except 2s→1s; imperative 2→1dp. (2) 3→1dpe. (3) 3d→1s. In a few cases, non-yapmi forms are listed as alternatives: 2p→1s present kÀ-PA-igyaÌ (resembling Myanglung Phedappe — see below), 2p→1p present akÀ-PR-Ì, and 3p→1pe present mÀ-PA-igyaÌ (Weidert and Subba 1985:60, 183). It is possible that a complete personal paradigm without yapmi forms exists in Panchthar as in the other Limbu dialects, but the impersonal form appears to be more frequent there than elsewhere. In older sources for Limbu morphology — the LSI (1908) and Hodgson’s notes (1857), both of which contain extensive paradigms — yapmi does not occur. This is a clear indication that yapmi forms had not replaced the regular paradigm. It is possible, however, that such forms did exist but were either not recorded or not judged to be part of the verbal morphology proper, since yapmi may have been taken as an independent word in an idiomatic construction. We may resume the use of yapmi-forms in Limbu dialects as follows: Mewa/Maiwa Phedap (van Driem 1987) Panchthar (Weidert and Subba, 1985) 3→1dpe 3→1dpe, 3d→1s (not 3s→1s) 2→1 2→1 (except 2s→1s) Impersonal yapmi-forms and 1st person morphology A number of factors may have contributed to the replacement of first person morphology by yapmi. It has often been noted that the non-singular parts of the you-and-me paradigms are the most difficult to elicit in Kiranti languages, with informants frequently resorting to non-finite forms even in spontaneous speech (Allen 1975:49, Michailovsky 1988:105, Ebert 1994:25-26). The development of 7/10 impersonal yapmi forms may reflect this. But why should this part of the paradigm be more problematic than others? The reason may be, as Ebert suggests, that in these forms the finite paradigm does not allow marking of the number of both speech act participants. The yapmi forms at least sidestep this problem, if they do not solve it. For Limbu, it is possible that the presence of a-, the inclusive3 marker, is felt to be anomalous in 2→1dp forms, in which the 1st person argument is notionally exclusive, even if it is not structurally in opposition with an inclusive category. (Note that 1dp→2 forms all bear the exclusive marker -igÀ.) This would explain the replacement of 2→1dp finite forms, marked by the prefix agÀ-, by impersonal-object forms (yapmi kÀ-) forms in Panchthar and (optionally) Phedap. It also explains the divergent evolution in the Myanglung Phedappe dialect mentioned by van Driem (1987:78n), where 2→1dp forms bear the 2nd person prefix kÀ- and the EXCLUSIVE suffix -igÀ4. If a- is felt to be anomalous in 2→1dp forms, it may be wondered why it is there in the first place. One possibility is that it was originally a 1st person (?non- singular) marker, unmarked for “clusivity”. Another is that it was originally an 1st person (non-singular) object marker of some kind. This hypothesis could find support in Hodgson’s paradigms (see accompanying article), where a- occurs only in transitive 1st person object forms. 3 Van Driem and I have been carrying on a low-intensity debate on the significance of a-, which he considers to mean ‘1st person' (Michailovsky 1989:472, van Driem 1994:159). My reasons for considering a- to be a 1st person (non-singular) inclusive — and not general 1st person — marker are the following (valid for the MM, Panchthar, and Phedap dialects at least): a- does not occur in ANY 1st person form with only singular arguments, that is 1s intransitive, 1s reflexive, 1s→2s, 1s→3s, 2s→1s, or 3s→1s, i.e. in any basic 1st person form. a- occurs in ALL 1st person non-singular forms which are distinctively inclusive, and in NO form that is distinctively exclusive. These include all non-singular 1st person intransitive forms, and all transitive 1dp→3 and 3→1dp forms. Van Driem replies, “I cannot concur with this view, as the prefix clearly functions as a marker of first person, not only in 2→1 forms, but also in non-finite forms such as the supine” (1994:159). The first assertion does not contradict my analysis, while the new argument, concerning the supine, is apparently irrelevant, because what is prefixed to the supine, as van Driem explains elsewhere (1987:212), is the oblique (or possessive) pronoun, a different paradigm of 12 forms. Of course, the possibility that the 1st person singular possessive pronoun a- (and the element a- in other 1st person pronouns) has the same etymological origin as the verbal prefix a- cannot be excluded. 4 For van Driem, a- “has been reanalysed as the inclusive morpheme” in this dialect. 8/10 REFERENCES Allen, N. 1975. Sketch of Thulung Grammar. Ithaca. Cornell University. Bradley, D., ed. 1994. Tibeto-Burman Languages of the Himalayas. Papers in Southeast Asian Linguistics, No. 14. Pacific Linguistics. Canberra. Chemjong, I. S. 1965. Limbu-Nepali-English Dictionary. Kathmandu, Royal Nepal Academy. [Limbu in devanagari script.] Ebert, K. 1991. “Inverse and pseudo-inverse prefixes in Kiranti lanugages: evidence from Belhare, Athpare, and Dungmali”. LTBA 14.1:73-92. —. 1994. The structure of Kiranti languages. Arbeiten des Seminars für allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft 13. Universität Zürich. —. 1997. A Grammar of Athpare. München, Newcastle. Lingcom Europa. Hodgson, Brian Houghton. 1857. Papers. vol. 89. Conserved in the India Office Library, London. Michailovsky, B. 1988. La langue hayu. Paris. Editions du CNRS. —. 1989. review of van Driem 1987. BSLP 84.2:470-473. Rutgers, Roland. 1998. Yamphu. Research School CNWS. Leiden. Senior, H. W. R. 1908. A Vocabulary of the Limbu Language. [English-Limbu. Roman script. Reprint 1977, Kathmandu, Ratna Pustak Bhandar.] Subba, B. B. 1979. Limbu Nepali English Dictionary. Government of Sikkim. Gangtok. [Limbu in Limbu and Devanagari scripts.] Van Dreim, George. 1987. A Grammar of Limbu. Berlin. Mouton de Gruyter. —. 1994. “A new analysis of the Limbu verb”. Bradley (1994) 153-173. Weidert, Alfons, et B. Subba. 1985. Concise Limbu Grammar and Dictionary. Amsterdam. Lobster Publications. Yonghang, Khel Raj. 2052 B.S. [1995]. limbú-nepìló ŠabdakoŠ. [Limbu-Nepali Dictionary] ?Lalitpur. [Limbu-Nepali, by semantic categories. Limbu in Limbu script.] 9/10 preprint LTBA 24:2:255 (autumn 2001) ERRATA, LTBA 24.1 "Limbu nous autres and 1st person morphology". I would like to correct errors in Tables 2 (p. 149) and 3 (p. 150). In Table 2, the forms in the 1st dual exclusive ("1de") column are incorrect, as the intransitive and 3d person agent forms should not have the inclusive prefix a-. The corrected forms: Transitive : O → ↓A 1de PR-sigä 3s PA-äsigä 3d mä-PR-sigä mä-PA-äsigä 3p intransitive: PR-sigä PA-äsigä Table 2: Maiwa-Mewa Khola affirmative indicative verb paradigm (partial) In Table 3, "Mewa Khola affirmative imperatives", the plural intransitive and first person object imperatives, which in the Mewa Khola dialect have the suffix - innä, were omitted. These forms are interesting because in Tembe, Phedap (van Driem 187) and Panchthar (Weidert and Subba) the plural intransitive imperative has the same suffix as the transitive. The corrected table: Transitive Intrans. Reflexive ↓A/S O →1s 1dp 3s 3dp — — 2s PA-a³³ä PA-ä PA-ä PR-si³³ä 2d a-PA-ä PA-äsä PA-äsä PR-näsä 2p a-PA-innä PA-ammä PA-ams(imm)ä PA-innä PR-Nasinnä Table 3: Mewa Khola affirmative imperatives — Boyd Michailovsky

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.