ebook img

Life-Span Developmental Psychology. Methodological Issues PDF

365 Pages·1973·6.366 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Life-Span Developmental Psychology. Methodological Issues

CONTRIBUTORS DONALD M. BAER JUM C. NUNNALLY PAULB. BALTES WILLIS F. OVERTON P. M. BENTLER SLOBODAN B. PETROVICH RAYMOND B. CATTELL HAYNE W. REESE LUTZ H. ECKENSBERGER KLAUS F. RIEGEL L. R. GOULET TODD R. RISLEY ECKHARD H. HESS K. WARNER SCHAIE FRANK H. HOOPER EDWIN P. WILLEMS JOHN R. NESSELROADE MONTROSE M. WOLF LIFE-SPAN DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES Edited by JOHN R. NESSELROADE and HAYNE W. REESE Department of Psychology West Virginia University Morgantown, West Virginia 1973 ACADEMIC PRESS New York and London COPYRIGHT © 1973, BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Ill Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24/28 Oval Road, London NW1 LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOG CARD NUMBER: 72-77362 PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA List of Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. DONALD M. BAER (187), Department of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas PAUL B. BALTES* (219), Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia P. M. BENTLER (145), Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, California RAYMOND B. CATTELL (111), Laboratory of Personality Analysis, Psychology Building, Champaign, Illinois LUTZ H. ECKENSBERGER (43), Psychologisches Institut der Universität des Saarlandes, Germany L. R. GOULET (281), Department of Educational Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana-Champaign, Illinois ECKHARD H. HESS (25), Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois FRANK H. HOOPER (299), Department of Home Management and Family Living, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin *Present address: College of Human Development, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania. ix χ List of Contributors JOHN R. NESSELROADE* (219), Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia JUM C. NUNNALLY (87), Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt Uni- versity, Nashville, Tennessee WILLIS F. OVERTONt (65), Department of Psychology, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York SLOBODAN B. PETROVICHt (25), Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois HAYNE W. REESE (65), Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia KLAUS F. RIEGEL (1), Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan TODD R. RISLEY (177), Department of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas K. WARNER SCHAIE (253), Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia EDWIN P. WILLEMS (197), Department of Psychology, University of Houston, Houston, Texas MONTROSE M. WOLF (177), Department of Human Development, Uni- versity of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas *Present Address: College of Human Development, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania. tPresent address: Department of Psychology, Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. ^Present address: Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Baltimore, Maryland. Preface During the past three years, a series of conferences held at West Virginia University has focused on the general topic of Life-Span Developmental Psychology. The second conference, held in 1971 and reported in this volume, provided a forum for the discussion of a variety of methodological issues related to the study of developmental processes over the life-span. The principal objectives of the Life-Span Conference have been not only to explicate, by successive approximation, the range of empirical phenomena with which a life-span developmental psychology should be concerned, but also to explore issues about theory, measurement, design, and data analysis which bear upon it. Although the situation is changing, a sufficiently large cadre of "true" life-span developmentalists does not exist to permit mounting such a conference wholly from their ranks. Consequently, our strategy was to invite several participants who, by their research activities, showed interest in related aspects of developmental psychology, and to challenge them to identify, formulate, and discuss critical methodological issues as, in their perception, they pertained to a life-span approach. Augmenting the schedule of conference participants by researchers not ostensibly committed to a life-span orientation was not only necessary but may be seen as a virtue to the extent that the various perspectives and interests each offers serve to sharpen and refine the meaning of Life-Span Developmental Psychology. Until time renders possible a judgment based on hindsight you, the reader, must evaluate the success of the venture. XI Acknowledgments The editors gratefully acknowledge the cooperation and assistance of all those who helped to make the second Life-Span Conference a reality. Ray Koppelman, Provost for Research and Graduate Affairs, and K. Warner Schaie, Chairman of the Department of Psychology, arranged the necessary financial support. Graduate students in our program, whom the conference was designed to benefit directly, assumed many of the responsibilities related to playing host and discharged them admirably. They include William Arnold, Thomas Bartsch, William Birkhill, Barbara Buech, John Burke, Dana Cable, Nicholas Cavoti, Joseph Fitzgerald, Edward Forbes, Wanda Franz, John Friel, Carol Furry, Frances Hoyer, William Hoyer, Erich Labouvie, Gisela Labouvie, Anne Nardi, Diane Papalia, Leighton Stamps, and Richard Swanson. Xlll 1 CHAPTER Developmental Psychology and Society: Some Historical and Ethical Considerations KLAUS F. RIEGEL UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN ANN ARBOR, MICHIGAN Ethical issues, as commonly conceived, concern the individual's actions toward other individuals. Thus, the guidelines prepared by the American Psychological Association (1963) provide standards on professional interactions. By and large, they disregard the scientist's responsibility toward the society within which his discipline has emerged. The second ethical consideration concerns this connec- tion and impels the scientist to be engaged in or, at least, to be conscious of social actions. In the following paper, attention will be directed exclusively toward the latter issue. I. Social, Political, and Economic Bases of the Developmental Sciences A. Historical Node Points in the Conception of Man . . . for what everyone wanted was denied by everyone else, and what resulted was what no one had wanted —FRIEDRICH ENGELS 1. Psychological Interpretations Our concern with the history of civilization and, especially, our atempts to answer the question of whether a non-Western developmental psychology, for 1 2 Klaus F. Riegel instance, a black psychology is possible, might be compared with the attempts of psychoanalysts. Inquiries into the distant past of our society are similar to their attempts at discovering points of choice and/or catastrophe in the life of an individual (Wyatt, 1963). The psychoanalyst essentially restricts his efforts to the discovery of these points; it is left to the patient to reconstruct his life in a new and "healthier" fashion. Similarly, our historical inquiries aim at uncovering points in time at which crucial choices were made. We can trace the history back to these points, but any distinct groups, such as minority groups, would have to construct their own new interpretation of life, a new philosophy of man and his development. A great many writers have expressed similar viewpoints. In his theory of human development, Erikson (1968) distinguishes various binary choice points which lead to either trust or distrust, initiative or guilt, identity or identity confusion, etc. Erikson extended his interpretations to cultural developments, for instance, by relating the identity choice to the historical period of the Reforma- tion and by demonstrating this interpretation through an analysis of the Young Man Luther (1958). It is tempting to suggest that minority groups within the Western civilization are faced with similar identity problems. Thus, like an individual, perhaps they need to go back to such points in history in order to find their identity and aim at its conceptual description. In contrast to Erikson, Jung's work (Jacobi, 1962) has focused more strongly upon universal features of civilization. By looking at art, symbolism, customs, and social conventions across developmental and historical stages, he searched for those forms and expressions common to all men. Jung (unlike his teacher, Freud) emphasized the constructive aspects in the therapeutic process. It is not enough, he maintained, to rediscover and identify the choice points of the past, but it is necessary to construct new perspectives, a new philosophy of life. This should not be left to the patient; the therapist should be of active assistance. In his Comparative Psychology of Mental Development, Werner (1926) drew parallels between the development of children and that of so-called primitive societies. Thus, he revived the recapitulation hypothesis of evolutionary theory and extended it to psychological and social conditions, claiming that children undergo developmental sequences similar to those that characterize historical progression. Psychopathological conditions are regarded as regressions to or fixations upon early developmental levels. With much support from cultural anthropologists, Werner restricted his interpretations to noncognitive behavior. In comparison, Piaget in his studies of Genetic Epistemology (1950) claimed that in the development of intellectual and logical operations as well, children recapitulate the steps through which societies had to pass in order to attain the knowledge of modern civilization. Developmental Psychology and Society 3 We do not intend to draw upon these recapitulation models in order to specify different conceptualizations of man and his development. However, historical studies make us aware of critical node points at which alternative models of interpretation were still existent but, afterward, were repressed for the sake and success of the dominant civilization. 2. Pre-Grecian Civilizations In a recent presentation, Smith (1970) delineated modes of psychological conceptualization which characterize the civilizations preceding the Greco- Roman period. While the latter has been described by an ontological dualism between being and becoming and modern Western civilization by the epistemolog- ical dualism between mind and body, the ancient civilizations of Sumer, Babylon, Assyria, or Egypt emphasized distinctly different dimensions and dichotomies (Moscati, 1957). The corresponding philosophies of the universe, life, and man need not concern us in detail, but it is of interest to analyze how these conceptions were overcome or repressed through the emergence of the Greco-Roman civiliza- tion. During the early periods of this civilization, we find an array of different world views with which the Greeks became acquainted. This confrontation has been emphasized by several scholars. Nestle (1940) considered the entire develop- ment of Greek civilization as a struggle between mythos and logos, between the forces of darkness and light and—as elaborated by Nietzsche (1872) and Benedict (1934)—between the Dionysian and Appollonian ideals. During the history of the Greek civilization, the latter and thus rational thought attained dominance and the "alien" ecstatic forces of emotionality were successfully controlled and repressed. Early in Greek history, the term "barbarian" denoted nothing else than "the other," i.e., a person of non-Greek origin. However, it soon came to indicate an uncivilized, alien, inferior person. This change characterizes the effort among ancient Greeks to facilitate their own cultural identity by downgrading persons and groups with different languages, habits, customs, and conceptions of man. Since many of these so-called barbarians had attained a much higher level of civilization than the Greeks of the Homeric period, their attempts appear highly artificial. The Greeks at that period were the "barbarians," indeed. For similar reasons, romantic and idealized interpretations of Greek civilization have dominated modern historical views since the Renaissance and, especially, since the classicism of Winkelmann, Herder, and Goethe. Consequently, the Greek civilization appears as an elevated historical period initiated by a distinct historical break. Only recent research and interpretations have revealed the herit- age from pre-Greek civilizations, especially from Persia, Mycenae, and Egypt.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.