ebook img

Kindness in a Cruel World: The Evolution of Altruism PDF

421 Pages·2004·3.4 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Kindness in a Cruel World: The Evolution of Altruism

Acknowledgments 7 Introduction 9 PART 1: ALTRUISM IN MAN AND BEAST Chapter 1: Altruism: Birds Do It, Bees Do It, People Do It 19 Chapter 2: Evolution and Ethics 43 Chapter 3: Sterile Castes of Priests and Nuns 69 PART 2: GROWING UP TO BE GOOD Chapter 4: Why Do People Grow Up to Be Altruists? 99 Chapter 5: Altruism among Thieves 133 PART 3: THE SOCIAL IMPACT OF KINDNESS Chapter 6: Kindness and Health 161 Chapter 7: Kindness among Strangers 203 Chapter 8: Conformity as Altruism 233 Chapter 9: When Altruism Fails 267 PART 4: KINDNESS AND POLITICS Chapter 10: Tapping Human Altruism 303 Chapter 11: Saving the World 333 Chapter 12: Where Have All the Villains Gone? 357 Notes 381 Index 407 Acknowledgments am grateful for the help received from many people at various stages in the conception, planning, and writing of this book. My editor at Prometheus Books, Linda Regan, influenced the content of the book and provided many helpful and thoughtful comments on the manuscript. My research was aided by the work of the Interlibrary Loan Department at the Portland Public Library, Maine, including Rita Gorham, Eileen MacAdam, and Anne Ball. Schadijah Camp of the Library of Congress helped with illustrations. Trudy Callaghan provided informal editorial assistance. I am grateful for her counsel and support. David Barber-Callaghan also helped by providing clever analogies. I am thankful to many friends and colleagues in research whose important ideas and findings in the evolutionary study of altruism put evolutionary psychology on the intellectual map. Many are, or were, members of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society. I also owe a debt of gratitude to the online community of evolutionary psychologists whose challenging ideas promote intellectual fitness. We all mourn the passing of Linda Mealey, who contributed generously to my previous book, The Science of Romance, during her final illness. Introduction aking the case that people are naturally helpful to others seems a hard sell in a world preoccupied with global terrorism, corporate swindlers, and pedophile priests. Yet, none of these manifestations of evil minimizes the altruistic motive that springs eternal in the human breast. Kindness exists, but it struggles to stay afloat on an ocean of cruelty that is the default condition for organisms competing for existence on this planet. This book describes actions rather than philosophies. It is also more concerned with concrete examples of helpful behavior than with the computer models many scholars use to decode them. Altruism is defined as actions that help another individual at some cost to the altruist. The main advantage of this approach is that it allows us to discuss human altruism in terms of the evolutionary ideas that account for altruism in other species. The biological definition of altruism is sometimes criticized as defining altruism out of existence by implying that apparent acts of selflessness are really only selfishness in disguise. The argument goes that such acts are undertaken to (1) make the altruist feel better, (2) increase the reproductive success of the altruist, or (3) increase the prevalence of genes for altruistic behavior. Yet, altruism is real, in the sense that it is predicated on evolved moral emotions like empathy and shame. Moreover, it is possible for humans and other species to engage in altruistic actions that have no ulterior motive, except whatever pleasure comes from the action itself, and no delayed benefit of any kind. (The only requirement for altruistic tendencies to evolve is that they should generally increase the biological success of individuals expressing them, not that every altruistic act should have a reproductive payoff.) There is nothing pie-eyed about the case for altruism as an animal adaptation. The simplest, and most prevalent, type is parental care that happens to be well developed in female mammals, some male fishes, and birds of both sexes. Such altruism is easily explained in terms of kin selection, or the promotion of one's genes through reproduction by close relatives. It is predictable, obligatory, and thus not very interesting, except when it fails, as in the case of parents who abuse or abandon their children. The farther one moves from nuclear families, the more interesting, and more fragile, altruism becomes. Solitary altruists cannot exist in a group of nonaltruists. Altruism is thus a property of groups, or social contexts, as much as it is a property of individuals. Our ancestors survived the uncertainty of hunting large animals by pooling their kill in a prehistoric insurance system that was to the benefit of all. Relying on meat alone, the average solitary hunter might be successful in bringing home a large game animal once a month, which is far too infrequent to enable survival. It makes more sense to share the meat while it is fresh, rather than leaving it to rot in a few days, because sharing entitles the donor to a steady supply of fresh food from other kills. With some excitement, animal behaviorists discovered a similar form of cooperation among vampire bats. These bats can survive for only a few days if deprived of a blood meal. Sated animals regurgitate food to sustain their famished friends. Such altruism is remarkable among animals that are not close relatives, but it is not automatic, or unconditional. If a bat refuses to share with her hungry friend, she is liable to be kicked out of their shared roost site and may starve. Our human ancestors were similarly protected against cheats by emotions such as anger and indignation that helped keep other members of their community in line. Without such checks and balances, any altruistic relationship among nonrelatives would inevitably collapse as cheats prospered at the expense of cooperators. Food-sharing altruism promoted survival among our huntergatherer ancestors, but the sense of community of interest clearly did not stop at the cooking fire: it extended to cooperative childcare, medicine, entertainment, and so on. Cooperative behavior and the many psychological adaptations necessary to maintain it did not go away when our ancestors shifted to agricultural food production. In settled communities, people helped each other to survive the uncertainties of agricultural production, with its droughts, fires, damaging storms, and pests. In the earliest urban civilizations, people cooperated to build colossal irrigation systems and cereal storage facilities that finally took much of the uncertainty out of agriculture. In the modern world of highly centralized governments, the initial impulse for centralization came from military threat, but large government is maintained by altruistic social programs, including welfare, education, and security for seniors. Adaptations for altruism that arose from a huntergatherer way of life can thus be expressed in very different ecologies. Even so, some aspects of urban living, particularly anonymity, decrease the level of interpersonal altruism and increase crime. There is a qualitative difference between everyday food sharing, for example, and the more costly forms of heroic self-sacrifice, such as death while fighting in a war. Sharing lunch is one thing, giving up one's life is another. Yet, this book makes the case that both types of altruism are founded on the same adaptations for mutual support. Many of the combatants in wars have not chosen to be present, of course. They were pushed into service by their governments, their tribes, their families, or, even, by the desire to impress their girlfriends. In many cases, there has been wholesale manipulation based on social class and ethnicity, with those on the bottom bearing the brunt of military service while the elite either bribe their way out of combat situations entirely, or purchase military rank that keeps them out of the most dangerous engagements. In the Vietnam War, for example, more African Americans from southern states served than would be predicted from their proportion of the population. Despite such evidence of manipulation, military service is altruistic, in the sense that the combatants sacrifice their personal welfare for the good of others. Throughout recorded history, families have been willing to send their young men into battle to preserve a power structure that favored the survivors. In return for their risky service, surviving soldiers used to be rewarded with high social status that made them more attractive to women. There was thus some potential for personal gain, although hardly enough to account for the great risks undertaken. One of the toughest conclusions of this book is that in-group altruism can translate into out-group aggression. A soldier supports his own nation by killing the sons of an enemy nation. This irony is all the more remarkable given that group allegiances can be so easily, and so arbitrarily, formed. Just about any distinguishing feature of a group, such as wearing red buttons rather than blue,

Description:
Beginning with Darwin's theory, Barber shows how the original notion of a dog-eat-dog world where survival of the fittest is the only rule must now be modified by new findings on altruism. In bees, for example, the workers evolve without reproductive ability and exist only for the good of the hive a
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.