ebook img

Is Anybody Listening Evidence That Voters Do Not Respond to European Parties Policy ... PDF

13 Pages·2011·0.18 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Is Anybody Listening Evidence That Voters Do Not Respond to European Parties Policy ...

Is Anybody Listening? Evidence That Voters Do Not Respond to European Parties’ Policy Statements During Elections James Adams University of California at Davis Lawrence Ezrow University of Essex Zeynep Somer-Topcu Vanderbilt University AlthoughextensiveresearchanalyzesthefactorsthatmotivateEuropeanpartiestoshifttheirpolicypositions,thereislittle cross-nationalresearchthatanalyzeshowvotersrespondtoparties’policyshifts.Wereportpooled,time-seriesanalysesof electionsurveydatafromseveralEuropeanpolities,whichsuggestthatvotersdonotsystematicallyadjusttheirperceptions ofparties’positionsinresponsetoshiftsinparties’policystatementsduringelectioncampaigns.Wealsofindnoevidence thatvotersadjusttheirLeft-Rightpositionsortheirpartisanloyaltiesinresponsetoshiftsinparties’campaign-basedpolicy statements.Bycontrast,wefindthatvotersdorespondtotheirsubjectiveperceptionsoftheparties’positions.Ourfindings haveimportantimplicationsforpartypolicystrategiesandforpoliticalrepresentation. R esearch on political representation in Europe policy debates over Left-Right social welfare issues (see, emphasizes the linkages between parties’ policy e.g.,Dalton1985). positionsandtheirsupporters’policybeliefs.Ac- In an evolving political environment wherein both cording to this responsible party model of political rep- partiesandvotersshifttheirpolicypositions,policycorre- resentation, it is normatively desirable that parties’ pol- spondencebetweenpoliticalpartiesandtheirsupporters icy programs—and governing parties’ policy outputs— can be maintained through some combination of party match the views of the party’s supporters, a desidera- elitesrespondingtotheirsupportersandthesesupport- tum that reflects Sartori’s observation that “citizens in ersrespondingtopartyelites,i.e.,elitesmaydynamically Westerndemocraciesarerepresentedbyandthroughpar- adjusttheirpolicypositionsinresponsetoshiftsintheir ties.Thisisinevitable”(1968,471,emphasisinoriginal). supporters’beliefs,aprocesswelabelpartyresponsiveness, Over the past 30 years, dozens of studies have analyzed andpartysupportersmaydynamicallyadjusttheirbeliefs the mass-elite policy linkages that the responsible party inresponsetoshiftsintheirpreferredparty’spolicypo- model highlights (see, e.g., Dalton 1985; Iversen 1994; sitions,aprocesswelabelpartypersuasion.Alternatively, Powell 1989).1 These studies typically report reasonably rank-and-filevotersmayswitchtheirpartisanloyaltiesin close matches between parties’ positions and their sup- responsetoparties’policyshifts,i.e.,votersmayengagein porters’ policy preferences, particularly with respect to policy-basedpartisanswitching.Thelattertwoprocesses JamesAdamsisProfessorofPoliticalScience,UniversityofCaliforniaatDavis,Davis,CA95616([email protected]).LawrenceEzrow isaReaderinEuropeanPolitics,UniversityofEssex,WivenhoePark,ColchesterCO43SQ,UnitedKingdom([email protected]).Zeynep Somer-TopcuisAssistantProfessorofPoliticalScience,VanderbiltUniversity,Nashville,TN37235([email protected]). Earlier versions of this article were presented at the 2008 “Politics of Change” Workshop at the Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, at the 2008annualmeetingoftheMidwestPoliticalScienceAssociation,andatthe2010“PoliticalPartiesandComparativePolicyAgendas’ Workshop”attheUniversityofManchester.WethankJohnBartle,ThomasBra¨uninger,GaryMarks,SlavaMikhaylov,RandySteven- son,HeatherStoll,GuyWhitten,andthreeanonymousreviewersforvaluablecomments.Allthreeauthorscontributedequallytothe article.TheauthorsacknowledgetheUKEconomicandSocialResearchCouncil(ESRC)forfundingsupport(RES000–22-2895,“Sub- constituency Representation across Western Europe”). Data and supporting information for this project can be found on the website http://privatewww.essex.ac.uk/∼ezrow/. 1Analternativerepresentationcriterionemphasizesthelinkbetweengovernmentpoliciesandthemedianvoterpositionintheelectorate (see,e.g.,McDonaldandBudge2005;Powell2000). AmericanJournalofPoliticalScience,Vol.55,No.2,April2011,Pp.370–382 (cid:3)C2011,MidwestPoliticalScienceAssociation DOI:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2010.00489.x 370 371 ISANYBODYLISTENING? bothinvolvevotersrespondingtoparties,andwecollec- actualstatementsintheparties’policymanifestos,which tivelylabeltheseasprocessesofpartisanadjustments. formthebasisfortheparties’electioncampaigns. Weanalyzethedynamicsofvoters’responsestoshifts inEuropeanparties’Left-Rightpositions.Weaskthefol- lowingquestions:WhenpartiesshifttheirLeft-Rightpol- ShouldWeExpectVoterstoReact icy statements, as reflected in their election manifestos, toParties’PolicyStatements? do citizens update their perceptions of the parties’ Left- TheoreticalArguments Rightpositions?And,doweobservepartisanadjustments inresponsetoparties’shiftingpolicystatements,i.e.,do citizens respond to parties’ statements by shifting their Wefirstconsiderthequestion:shouldweactuallyexpect own Left-Right positions (a persuasion process) and/or citizenstoupdatetheirperceptionsofparties’Left-Right theirpartisanloyalties(partisanswitching)? positionsinresponsetopoliticalelites’policystatements? Thesurprisinganswerweprovidetoeachoftheabove More specifically, given that our empirical measure of questions is no. We find no substantively or statistically the Left-Right tone of party policy statements is based significant evidence that voters adjust their perceptions oncontentanalysesoftheirelectionmanifestos,4weask: ofparties’Left-Rightpositionsinresponsetothepolicy shouldweexpectvoters’perceptionsofparties’ideologies statementsinparties’electionmanifestos—aconclusion tovarywiththeLeft-Righttoneofthestatementsinthese that is striking given that interviews with European po- manifestos? We believe this is an interesting question, liticalelitesthatweconducted(discussedbelow)suggest because there are considerations that point in conflict- thatpartiescampaignonthebasisofthesemanifestos.We ing directions. The two considerations that suggest that alsofindnoevidenceofcitizens’partisanadjustmentsin voters will indeed react to parties’ election manifestos response to parties’ policy statements, i.e., we find no areasfollows.First,partiestypicallypublishtheirman- evidence that voters adjust their Left-Right positions or ifestos in the run-up to the national election campaign, their partisan loyalties in response to these policy state- andpreviousstudiesfindthatvotersdisplayheightened ments. This latter conclusion holds both for analyses of interest in (and awareness of) political information that national election surveys from five European countries2 appearsaroundthetimeofanationalelection.Inpartic- andforseparateanalysesofEurobarometerdatafrom12 ular,Andersen,Tilley,andHeath(2005)reportanalyses countries over the period 1973–2002.3 Simply put, we of the British electorate over the period 1992–2002 that findthatwhenpartiesshiftthestatementsintheirpolicy demonstratethatcitizens’informationabouttheparties’ programs—statements that form the basis for the par- policypositionspeakedaroundthetimesofthenational ties’electioncampaigns,accordingtothepartyeliteswe elections held during this period (1992, 1997, 2002).5 interviewed—thereisnoevidencethatvotersrespondby Andersen,Tilley,andHeath’sfindingssupporttheargu- adjustingtheirperceptionsoftheparties’Left-Rightpo- mentspresentedbyGelmanandKing(1993),thatcitizens sitions, their own Left-Right positions, or their partisan becomemoreenlightenedduringthecourseofnational loyalties. electioncampaignsduetotheintensityofpoliticalmedia Bycontrast,wefindthatEuropeancitizensdoreactto coverageduringtheseperiods,andalsobecausecitizens theirperceptions ofparties’Left-Rightpositions,i.e.,cit- areespeciallymotivatedtoseekoutpoliticalinformation izensadjusttheirLeft-Rightpositionsandtheirpartisan during election campaigns. Thus, while only a fraction loyaltiesinresponsetotheparties’policyimages.Overall, ofrank-and-filevoterspresumablyreadtheparties’elec- ourfindingstherebysuggestthatLeft-Rightideologydoes tionmanifestos,wemightexpectcitizenstobeespecially matter tovoters,andthattheyreacttoparties’perceived attentive to media coverage of these manifestos because ideologicalshifts.But,becausevoterperceptionsdonot theyarepublishednearthetimeofnationalelections(see tracktheparties’actualpolicystatements,thereisadis- Topf1994). connectbetweenshiftsinelitepolicydiscourseandvoter reactions. Voters react to what they perceive the parties 4Aswediscussbelow,theComparativeManifestoProject(CMP) standfor,buttheseperceptionsdonotmatchupwiththe codingsofpartypolicymanifestosprovidetheonlyavailableover- timeestimatesoftheLeft-Righttoneofparties’policystatements, 2ThefivecountriesareBritain,Germany,theNetherlands,Sweden, whichgeneralizeacrosstheEuropeanpartysystemsandthetime andNorway,thefiveEuropeanpolitiesforwhichnationalelection periodsincludedinourstudy. surveydataareavailableoveralengthytimeperiod. 5We note that the national election survey data that we analyze 3The12countriesthatareincludedintheEurobarometersurveys belowwerecollectedshortlyafterthenationalelectioncampaign are Britain, France, Germany, the Netherlands, Portugal, Spain, tookplace,sothatrespondents’politicalinformationlevelsshould Denmark,Luxembourg,Greece,Ireland,Belgium,andItaly. peakaroundthetimeofthesesurveys. 372 JAMESADAMS,LAWRENCEEZROW,ANDZEYNEPSOMER-TOPCU Second, although there is no extant study that an- than 100,000 copies of the manifesto that were alyzes whether the policy statements in parties’ election distributed....Andofcoursethepartyactivists manifestosmatchtheirstatementsinothercampaignfo- read it. The manifesto basically is an outline of rums(i.e.,politicaladvertisements,candidates’speeches, the kinds of arguments that we do share so it etc.), we conductedinterviews with 26 party elites from basicallyharmonizesthemessageoftheparty.9 Germany,theNetherlands,andAustriaduringthesum- mer of 2008, in which these elites stated that their par- Whiletheconsiderationsdiscussedabovesuggestrea- tiesmakedeterminedeffortstocampaignbasedontheir sons why citizens’ perceptions of party ideologies may policymanifestos.6 Thisisimportantbecauseitsuggests trackthestatementsinparties’electionmanifestos,other that parties’ manifesto-based policy statements roughly considerations cast doubt on parties’ abilities to shape reflect the policies they communicate to the public via theirpolicyimagesviatheircampaign-basedpolicystate- other avenues, such as campaign advertisements, party ments. First, there is extensive empirical research that elites’campaignspeeches,andmediainterviews.Typical argues that large segments of the mass public are inat- werethefollowingexchanges: tentive to and uninformed about political issues (e.g., Converse 1964; Zaller 1992), so that even if the elec- Q: Does the election campaign usually follow torate is relatively more politically informed around the whattheelectionmanifestosays? timeofnationalelections—asthestudiesdiscussedearlier A:Itisveryimportanttohave,withintheparty, suggest—significantsegmentsoftheelectoratemaynev- and also with the local members...to have a erthelessbeinattentivetocampaign-basedmediacover- view and an awareness of issues which are im- ageofparties’policystatements.Second,aseparatestrand portantforusandforourvoters.Andtheyhave of research suggests reasons why even those voters who to be in everything we do. So, they have to be registertheparties’shiftingpolicystatementsmayresist central points in our election programs and in updating their perceptions of parties’ positions: specifi- ourmaterialsandinspeechesontelevisionand cally,thisresearchdocumentsassimilation/contrasteffects thingslikethat.7 wherebyvotersaligntheircurrentperceptionsofthepar- Q: And how much do you think the election ties’positionswiththeirpreexistingpoliticalloyalties(see, manifestoandtheissuesintheelectionmanifesto e.g.,AchenandBartels2006;GerberandGreen1999). stay the same for the CDA during the electoral Thereareadditionalreasonswhypoliticalpartiesmay campaign?Isitthebasisforthecampaignordoes face difficulties in shaping their policy images via their itchangealot? campaign-based policy statements. Parties’ new policy A:No,no,itis[thebasisforthecampaign].We pronouncementsoftenprovokerivalpartyelitestopub- havesomeessential,veryimportantpartsofthe liclyderidethefocalparty’snewinitiativesasbeing“op- party program which are fundamental for the portunistic,”“pandering,”“insincere,”“unrealistic,”and imageoftheCDA....So,theissuesintheparty so on, and these characterizations may confuse voters programareimage-buildingissues.So,thereisa aboutthefocalparty’sactualpolicyintentions.Further- lot in the manifestos and there are some things more, members of the news media may weigh in with whicharefundamentalforourimage.8 commentary about whether the focal party’s new pol- Q:So,howmuchdoyouthinktheelectionpro- icy initiatives represent a “tactical” change in strategy gramisreflectedintheelectoralcampaign?Be- as opposed to a fundamental change in policy direc- cause,nobodyinthepublicreallyreadstheelec- tion; whether the policy shift is permanent or whether tion program, so do you think that the party it merely represents a temporary victory for one of the closelyfollowstheelectionprogram? party’sfactions;andwhether,intheeventthepartyenters A: It’s not true that nobody reads the man- thegovernment,theparty’sleaderswillhavetheresolveto ifesto. We’ve had, sometimes we’ve had more actuallypushthroughthepolicyreformstheyhaveadvo- catedwhileinopposition(see,e.g.,Stokes2001).Inaddi- tion,duringnationalelectioncampaignsboththemedia 6These elite interviews encompassed party leaders, members of andopposingpartiesmayemphasizeaspectsofthefocal parliament, campaign directors, and directors of think tank or- party’s policy program that differ from the planks that ganizations.Aprotocoloftheseinterviewswasthatwekeepthe subjects’identitiesconfidential. the party itself prefers to emphasize, i.e., parties cannot 7InterviewwithapoliticianfromtheDutchGreenParty(June26, unilaterallysetthe“termsofthedebate”fortheelection 2008). campaignviatheirpolicystatements. 8InterviewwithapoliticianfromtheDutchChristianDemocratic Party(June26,2008). 9InterviewwithapoliticianfromtheGermanGreens(July3,2008). 373 ISANYBODYLISTENING? TheLinkbetweenParties’Statements periods.12 In each of these national election studies, re- andTheirPolicyImages:Empirical spondentswereaskedtolocateallofthemajorpartieson anideologicalscaletypicallyrangingfrom1(extremeleft) Analyses to 10 (extreme right).13 We used these survey responses tocomputetheperceivedpartypositionforeachpartyin WerequirelongitudinalmeasurementsoftheLeft-Right eachelection. toneofpartypolicystatementsalongwithvoters’percep- tionsoftheparties’positionsinordertoanalyzewhether ModelSpecification parties’policyimagesshiftalongwiththeirpolicystate- ments. Our measures of the Left-Right tone of parties’ We specify a multivariate regression model in order to policy statements are based on the Comparative Mani- evaluate whether parties’ policy images shift in tandem festo Project’s (CMP) Left-Right codings of the parties’ withtheCMPcodingsoftheLeft-Righttoneofthestate- electionmanifestos(Budgeetal.2001).Thesemanifestos mentsintheparties’electionmanifestos.Thedependent aretypicallypublishedintherun-uptothenationalelec- variable is the change in the party’s policy image at the tion,and,asdiscussedabove,oureliteinterviewssuggest current election compared to its image at the previous thatpartiesusethesemanifestosasthebasisfortheirsub- election, as measured by NES respondents’ mean Left- sequentelectioncampaigns.Theproceduresusedtomap Rightplacementsoftheparty.Welabelthisvariable[Party parties’policypositionsfromtheirelectionprogramsare j’s perceived Left-Right shift (t)], to denote the shift in described in detail in several of the CMP-related publi- thefocalpartyj’sperceivedpositionbetweenthecurrent cations,sothatweonlybrieflyreviewtheprocesshere.10 election t and the previous election t−1. Our key inde- The coders match up quasi-sentences in the policy pro- pendentvariablesare[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift (t)], gramwithacategoryofpolicy(e.g.,welfare,defense,law which denotes the shift in the CMP’s Left-Right coding and order, etc.) and take the percentages of each cate- of party j’s election manifesto between the current and gory as a measureof the party’s priorities. Based on the thepreviouselection,and[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift mixture of policy priorities, the authors develop an in- (t−1)], which denotes the shift in the CMP’s coding of dex that measures the overall ideology for the program partyj’smanifestobetweenthepreviouselectionandthe ofeachpartyineachelectionyear.Theideologicalscores electionbeforethat.Weincludethislattervariabletocon- range from −100 to +100, with higher scores denoting trolforthepossibilitythatsignificanttimelagsintervene amoreright-wingemphasis.WehaverescaledtheCMP beforecitizensprocessparties’policystatements(see,e.g., partyscorestoa1–10Left-Rightscale,sothattherangeof Adams and Somer-Topcu 2009; Erikson, MacKuen, and thesescoresmatchesthatofthevoters’perceptionsofthe Stimpson2002).Wealsoincludealaggedversionofthe parties’Left-Rightpositions(describedbelow).Welabel dependentvariableinordertocontrolforautocorrelation theCMP-basedcodingsofpartymanifestosastheparty’s issues.14 statedposition.11 Ourspecificationfortherelationshipbetweenvoters’ Our measures of parties’ policy images, i.e., of vot- perceptions of the parties’ Left-Right positions and the ers’ subjective perceptions of the parties’ Left-Right po- Left-Right tone of the party’s election manifesto is as sitions,arederivedfromNationalElectionStudy(NES) follows: datafromGermany,GreatBritain,theNetherlands,Nor- Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift(t) way, and Sweden—the five Western European democ- racies for which NES data are available for protracted =b +b [Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t)] 0 1 +b [Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t−1)] 2 +b [Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift(t −1)] 3 10Foramorethoroughdescriptionofthecodingprocess,seeAp- (1) pendix2inBudgeetal.(2001). 11Wenotethatthereisawide-ranging,oftenheateddebateover thevalidityoftheCMPLeft-Rightcodingsofpartyprograms,with 12Thelistofelectionsincludedinthesurveysisgivenintheap- particularemphasisonwhetherthesecodingscaptureparties’Left- pendix. Rightpolicypositionsasopposedtoparties’issuepriorities(see,e.g., 13IncaseswheretheLeft-Rightscalewasnot1–10,werecalibrated Jandaetal.1995;McDonaldandMendes2001).Whileitisoutside respondents’partyplacementstoa1–10scale. the scope of this article to review this debate, we note that the CMPcodingshavebeenwidelyusedinresearchthatanalyzesthe 14Wenotethatwealsoestimatedtheparametersofthemodelwhile relationshipbetweenparties’Left-Rightpositionsandsurvey-based omitting the lagged dependent variable, and these estimates— measuresofvoters’ideologicalpositions(e.g.,Adams,Haupt,and whichwereportinsupplementarymaterialspostedonourweb- Stoll2009;Ezrow2007),whichisoneofthekeyrelationshipsthat site(http://privatewww.essex.ac.uk/∼ezrow)—supportedsubstan- weanalyzeinthisarticle. tiveconclusionsthatwereidenticaltothosewereportbelow. 374 JAMESADAMS,LAWRENCEEZROW,ANDZEYNEPSOMER-TOPCU TABLE1 DescriptiveStatistics:Dependentand TABLE2 TheRelationshipbetweenChangesin IndependentVariables theLeft-RightOrientationsofParty ManifestosandShiftsinVoters’ MeanAbsolute PerceptionsofPartyPositions(N =97) MeanValue Value Country- Partyjsupporters’ −0.04(0.44) 0.30(0.33) Basic Specific Left-Rightshift (t) Model(1) Intercepts(2) Partyjshift (t)– −0.03(0.42) 0.33(0.25) voterperceptions Partyj’sstated .039(.089) .047(.089) Partyjshift (t)– 0.01(0.58) 0.44(0.38) Left-Rightshift (t) CMPcodings Partyj’sstated .100(.074) .103(.074) Left-Rightshift(t–1) Notes:Thenumbersinparenthesesarethestandarddeviationsof Partyj’sperceived .046(.144) .032(.136) thereportedvalues.Allofthevariablesarecalibratedalonga10- pointscalerunningfrom1(extremeleft)to10(extremeright). Left-Rightshift(t–1) Themeasuresofpartysupporters’Left-Rightshifts,andofvoters’ Britain .157(.344) perceptionsofparties’Left-Rightshifts,aredrawnfromNational Germany .107(.288) ElectionStudydatafromGermany,GreatBritain,theNetherlands, Norway, and Sweden. The complete list of elections and parties Norway −.031(.132) includedinthedatasetisgivenintheappendix. Sweden .068(.125) Intercept −.017(.046) −.051(.119) AdjustedR2 .02 .04 where ∗∗p≤.01;∗p≤.05,two-tailedtests. Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift(t)=thechangeinparty Notes:Standarderrorsareinparentheses.Fortheseanalysesthe j’sperceivedLeft-Rightpositionatthecurrentelectiont dependent variable was the change in voters’ perceptions of the comparedwithitspositionatthepreviouselectiont−1, party’sLeft-Rightpositionatthecurrentelectioncomparedtothe previous election, basedonrespondents’partyplacements from based on NES respondents’ party placements. The vari- National Election Study data from Germany, Great Britain, the able[Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift (t−1)]issimilarly Netherlands,Norway,andSweden(seetheappendixforthecom- defined. plete list of elections and parties included in the data set). The independent variables are defined in the text. In column 2 the Party j’s stated Left-Right shift (t) = the change in party baselinecountryistheNetherlands. j’sLeft-Rightpositionatthecurrentelectiont compared with its position at the previous election t−1, based on concern for the model we estimate. However, there are the CMP’s Left-Right codings of the policy statements other methodological concerns to address. The lagged intheparty’selectionmanifestos.Thevariable[Partyj’s statedLeft-Rightshift (t−1)]issimilarlydefined. dependentvariableincludedinourspecificationhelpsto addresstheconcernofseriallycorrelatederrors(Beckand Katz 1995), and a Lagrange multiplier test fails to reject thenullhypothesisofnoserialcorrelation.Anothercon- Results cern is that there may be unobserved, election-specific Our analysis encompassed 97 stated Left-Right shifts factors that influence the positions of parties’ partisan by parties and their supporters in five European party constituencies in a particular election. We address these systems—Germany, Britain, the Netherlands, Norway, concernsthroughtheuseofrobuststandarderrorsclus- and Sweden—for which NES data were available over teredbyelection(Rogers1993). lengthy time periods. The complete set of parties and The parameter estimates for the alternative models electionsincludedintheanalysesarereportedintheap- thatweinvestigatedarereportedinTable2.Column1in pendix.Table1reportsthemeansandstandarddeviations thetablereportsparameterestimatesforamodelwhere oftheobservedvaluesofthevariablesinourdataset. thedependentvariable,[Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift Ouranalysesencompassed25parties,eachobserved (t)], represents the change in the mean Left-Right po- over an average of six elections, and should thus be re- sition ascribed to the focal party along the 1–10 Left- garded as time-series cross-sectional data. Estimating a Right scale by NES respondents at the current election, simple regression on the pooled data can lead to erro- comparedtothemeanpositionthatallNESrespondents neousconclusionsifthereareunobserveddifferencesbe- ascribedtothepartyatthepreviouselection.Totheex- tweenpartisanconstituencies(Hsiao2003);fortunately, tent that voters’ perceptions of parties’ Left-Right posi- tests for party-specific effects indicate that this is not a tionsrespondtotheactualpolicystatementstheparties 375 ISANYBODYLISTENING? publishintheirpolicymanifestos,wewouldexpectpos- Voters’PartisanAdjustmentsto itive and statistically significant coefficient estimates on Parties’PolicyStatements:Empirical the [Party j’s stated Left-Right shift (t)] variable—which Analyses would indicate that voters adjust their perceptions con- temporaneously with parties’ stated Left-Right shifts— andpossiblyonthe[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t−1)] Theempiricalanalysesreportedabovecastseriousdoubts on the proposition that European voters engage in sys- variable,whichwoulddenotetimelagsbeforevotersup- tematic partisan adjustments in response to parties’ date their perceptions of party positions. However, the campaign-basedpolicystatements.Forifcitizensdonot coefficientestimatesreportedincolumn1ofTable2are evenupdatetheirperceptionsofparties’Left-Rightposi- smallandstatisticallyinsignificant,andthustheydonot tionswhenparties’policystatementsshift,thenitseems support these expectations. These estimates imply that, unlikelythatcitizensadjusttheirpartisanloyaltiesortheir ceterisparibus,aone-unitshiftintheCMPcodingsofa own Left-Right positions in response to parties’ policy party’sLeft-Rightpositionatthecurrentelection(com- statements. However, this logic is not necessarily sound paredtothepreviouselection)isassociatedwithaLeft- because parties may move citizens indirectly via their Rightshiftinvoters’perceptionsoftheparty’sLeft-Right policystatements:specifically,partiesmaycommunicate position of less than one-twentieth of one unit, where their Left-Right shifts to organizations and political in- boththeCMPcodingsandvoters’partyplacementsare terest groups (i.e., trade unions, business organizations, calibratedalongthesame1–10scale.And,thecoefficient etc.), and these organizations may in turn provide cues estimateonthelaggedvariable,[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Right shift (t−1)], is also near zero and statistically insignifi- thatpromptrank-and-filevoterstomakepartisanadjust- ments.Suppose,forinstance,thattradeunionmembers cant.Furthermore,whenwereestimatetheparametersof holdgenerallyleft-wingpolicyviews,andthatthesemem- themodelwhileincludingcountry-specificinterceptsto bersalsotakepoliticalcuesfromtheirunion’sleadership. controlforunobserved,country-specificfactorsthatin- Thentotheextentthatunionleadersprovidemoreenthu- fluencevoters’perceptions,theparameterestimates(re- siastic endorsements to socialist, labor, and communist ported in column 2 of Table 2) support the same sub- stantive conclusions.15 Thus, we find no evidence that partieswhentheseparties’policystatementsshiftfarther totheleft,wemightexpectthatcue-takingunionmem- voterssystematicallyupdatetheirperceptionsofparties’ bers will more reliably support these parties when the Left-Rightpositionsinresponsetotheactualpolicystate- partiesadvocatemoreleftistpolicies—eveniftheunion mentsthatpartiespresentintheirmanifestos.Giventhat members themselves do not directly perceive the shift previousresearchdocumentsthatcitizensacquiredispro- in the focal party’s policy statements. The possibility of portionate amounts of political information during na- thistypeofcue-basedpartisanswitchingissupportedby tionalelectioncampaigns,andourinterviewsthatsuggest extensiveresearchdocumentingtheinformationalshort- thatpartiescampaignbasedontheirmanifestos,thesere- cutsthatcitizensemploytomakepoliticaldecisions(e.g., sultscastdoubtonparties’abilitiestoshapetheirpolicy Sniderman,Brody,andTetlock1991). imagesviatheircampaign-basedpolicystatements. 15Weconductedadditionalrobustnesschecksusingalternativever- SearchingforEvidenceofVoters’Partisan sionsofthedependentandindependentvariablesinequation(1). Inoneversion,wecalibratedshiftsinparties’statedLeft-Rightpo- AdjustmentswithCross-SectionalSurvey sitions(ascodedbytheCMP)againsttheshiftsofotherparties Data inthepartysystem,i.e.wecreatedavariablelabeled[Partyj’srel- ativestatedLeft-Rightshift (t)]thatwasdefinedasthedifference Inanalysesbasedonpaneldata,wemightparseoutthe between the focal party j’s stated Left-Right shift at the current electionandthemeanstatedLeft-Rightshiftofallotherpartiesin extent to which voters’ partisan adjustments to parties’ thesystematthecurrentelection.Ourlogicforthisspecification stated policy positions reflect partisan switching as op- wasthatwhenvotersassesswhetherafocalpartyhasshiftedtothe posedtopartypersuasion.However,becausethedatawe rightorleft,theymaycalibratetheparty’sshiftagainsttheshifts ofotherpartiesinthesystem.Wealsoestimatedamodelwhere analyze are time-series cross-sectional, we instead assay thedependentvariable,theparty’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift,was thesimplertaskofestimatingtheextenttowhicheither calibratedagainsttheperceivedshiftsofotherpartiesintheparty of these processes is taking place, i.e., we will search for system,i.e.,wecreatedavariablelabeled[Partyj’srelativeperceived anyevidenceofvoters’partisanadjustmentstoEuropean Left-Right shift (t)]. Our parameter estimates for these alterna- tive specifications—which we report in supplementary materials parties’ stated Left-Right shifts. Our logic is simple: to postedonourwebsite(http://privatewww.essex.ac.uk/∼ezrow)— the extent that parties’ campaign-based policy commu- supportedsubstantiveconclusionsthatwereidenticaltothosere- nicationsmotivatepartisanadjustmentsintheelectorate ported above. We thank an anonymous reviewer for suggesting theseanalyses. duetopartisanswitchingand/orpartypersuasion(and, 376 JAMESADAMS,LAWRENCEEZROW,ANDZEYNEPSOMER-TOPCU regardless of whether these effects are direct or occur variablebecauseofthepossibilityoflaggedpartisanad- indirectly via cues from political organizations/interest justmentstoparties’Left-Rightshifts.Wealsoincludea groups), we expect that when a party shifts its policy laggedversionofthedependentvariableinordertocon- statements, the mean Left-Right position of the party’s trolforpossibleautocorrelationissues.18Welabelthisthe supporters will shift in the same direction. This test is partisanadjustmentspecification—CMPcodings: verysimple,andasdiscussedbelowitisincompleteinthat Partyjsupporters’Left-Rightshift(t) even if we find evidence that party shifts correlate with =(cid:2) +(cid:2) [Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t)] shiftsbytheirsupporters,wecannotnecessarilyconclude 0 1 thattheseassociationsreflectacausalprocessofpartisan +(cid:2) [Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t −1)] 2 adjustment.16 However, if we find that the positions of +(cid:2) [Partyjsupporters’Left-Rightshift(t −1)] 3 parties’ supporters do not shift in the same direction as (2) theirpreferredparties’policyshifts,thenwehavestrong wherePartyjsupporters’Left-Rightshift (t)=thechange reasons to doubt that partisan adjustments of any kind inthemeanLeft-Rightpositionofpartyj’ssupportersat havetakenplace.Onthisbasisweproceed. the current election t compared to the mean supporter positionatthepreviouselection(t−1),basedonNESre- ModelSpecification spondents’self-placementsandtheirpartysupport.The variable[Partyjsupporters’Left-Rightshift (t−1)]issim- Ourlongitudinalmeasureofvoters’Left-Rightpositions ilarlydefined. and their partisan loyalties is derived from the national Column 1 in Table 3, which reports the parameter election studies described above, from Germany, Great estimatesforthepartisanadjustmentspecification—CMP Britain, the Netherlands, Norway, and Sweden. In these codings(equation2),doesnotsupportthehypothesisthat surveys the respondents were asked to place themselves votersengageinpartisanadjustmentsinresponsetoshifts onthe1–10Left-Rightscales,andtheywerealsoaskedto inparties’campaign-basedpolicystatements.19Theesti- indicate which party (if any) they supported. Our mea- mateonthe[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift (t)]variable sure of the Left-Right position of each party’s partisan impliesthat,ceterisparibus,aone-unitshiftintheCMP constituencyinthefocalelectionisthemeanLeft-Right codings of a party’s Left-Right position at the current self-placement of all respondents in that election who reportedsupportfortheparty.17 election(comparedtothepreviouselection)isassociated withaLeft-Rightshiftinthemeanpositionoftheparty’s Wespecifyamultivariateregressionmodeltosearch supportersofjust0.07units,alongthesame1–10scale. for evidence of voters’ partisan adjustments to parties’ Thus,weobservenoevidenceofstatisticallyorsubstan- Left-Right shifts. As we are interested in how the mean tivelysignificantcontemporaneouspartisanadjustments positions of the parties’ supporters shift in response to by voters in response to shifts in the Left-Right tone of parties’ stated Left-Right shifts, we specify a model in parties’campaign-basedpolicystatements.Withrespect which the dependent variable is the change in the party to lagged effects, the coefficient estimate +.140 on the supporters’meanLeft-Rightpositioninthecurrentelec- [Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift (t −1)]variableindicates tioncomparedtopartysupporters’positionsintheprevi- thatwhenpartiesshiftedtheirstatedLeft-Rightpositions ouselection,asmeasuredbyNESrespondents’Left-Right byoneunitatthepreviousinterelectionperiod,then,ce- self-placementsandreportedpartysupport.Welabelthis terisparibus,themeanpositionsoftheirsupportersshift variable[Partyjsupporters’Left-Rightshift (t)].Ourkey inthesamedirectionbylessthanone-seventhofoneunit independentvariablesareagainthe[Partyj’sstatedLeft- atthecurrent interelectionperiod.However,thislagged Rightshift (t)]variable,whichdenotestheshiftinparty estimateisnotstatisticallysignificant.20 j’s stated Left-Right position between the current and the previous election, based on the CMP codings, and thelaggedversionofthisvariable.Weincludethislatter 18Wealsoestimatedtheparametersofthemodelwhileomittingthe laggedindependentvariable,andtheseestimatessupportedsub- stantiveconclusionsthatwereidenticaltothosewereportbelow. 16Asdiscussedbelow,ifweobservethatpartysupportersshiftinthe samedirectionastheirpreferredpartyduringthesametimeperiod, 19Weestimatedtheparametersofthismodelusingrobuststandard thismayreflectpartisanadjustments,butalternativelyitmayreflect errorsclusteredbyparty(Rogers1993). partiesshiftingtheirpositionsinresponsetotheirsupporters. 20Weconductedadditionalrobustnesschecksusingalternativever- 17The text of the survey questions relating to party support dif- sionsofthedependentandindependentvariablesinequation(2). feredacrosscountries,withsomequestionsinvitingrespondents Specifically,weincorporatedthe[Partyj’srelativestatedLeft-Right toidentifytheparty(ifany)they“feelcloseto,”othersasking,“Of shift(t)]variableandthe[Partyj’srelativeperceivedLeft-Rightshift whichpartyareyouanadherent?”andsoon.Informationabout (t)]variablesdescribedabove(seenote15)inordertoaddressthe thesesurveyquestionsisavailableontheEuropeanvotertrend-file possibilitythatwhenvotersassesswhetherafocalpartyhasshifted athttp://www.gesis.org/en/research/EUROLAB/evoter/. totherightorleft,theycalibratetheparty’sshiftagainsttheshiftsof 377 ISANYBODYLISTENING? TABLE3 RelationshipbetweenShiftsinPartySupporters’Left-RightPositionsandParties’ Left-RightShifts Partisan Adjustments CMPCodings+ Specification— Voter Economic Eurobarometer CMPCodings(1) Perceptions(2) Conditions1(3) data2(4) Partyj’sstated .070(.119) .037(.063) .027(.066) .027(.037) Left-Rightshift (t) Partyj’sstated .140(.141) .067(.088) .040(.109) −.002(.028) Left-Rightshift (t –1) Partyj’sperceived .635∗∗(.075) .645∗∗(.062) Left-Rightshift (t) Partyj’sperceived .326∗∗(.066) .374∗∗(.084) Left-Rightshift (t –1) Inflationrate −.021(.013) change(t) Unemployment −.010(.012) ratechange(t) Partyjsupporters’shift (t –1) −.056(.137) −.277(.152) −.332(.173) −.381∗∗(.079) Intercept −.057(.046) −.046(.030) −.045(.044) −.038(.024) Numberofcases 97 97 97 296 AdjustedR2 .033 .471 .496 .121 ∗∗p≤.01;∗p≤.05,two-tailedtests. Notes:Standarderrorsareinparentheses.Theanalysesreportedincolumn1arebasedonthespecificationgivenbyequation(2)inthe text,wherethedependentvariableisthechangeinthemeanLeft-Rightself-placementsoftheparty’ssupportersintheyearofthecurrent electioncomparedtothepreviouselection,ascomputedfromNESdatafromGermany,GreatBritain,theNetherlands,Norway,and Sweden(seetheappendixforthecompletelistofelectionsandpartiesincludedintheanalysis).Theindependentvariablesaredescribed inthetext. 1Forthismodelweestimatedcountry-specificintercepts,whichwereasfollows(standarderrorsinparentheses):Germany−.011(.010); Britain−.175(.123);Norway.034(.071);Sweden.001(.094).TheNetherlandsisthebaselinecountry.Wealsoestimatedcountry-specific intercepts(availableuponrequest)forthemodelsreportedincolumns3–4. 2Theanalysesreportedinthiscolumnarebasedonthespecificationgivenbyequation(2)inthetextandarecomputedfromEurobarometer datafrom12Europeancountries. As discussed above, the absence of strongly positive organizationsandinterestgroupsthatmonitorthesepol- statistical associations between shifts in the Left-Right icystatements.Finally,wenotethattotheextentthatour toneofparties’policystatementsandthemeanLeft-Right analysesdonotcontrolforthereciprocalcausalprocess positions of their supporters provides strong evidence whereby parties react to voters’ policy shifts, this factor that voters do not react to shifts in parties’ campaign- shouldinflatethepositivestatisticalassociationsbetween based policy statements, to any substantively significant ourdependentandindependentvariables.Thus,thefact degree:fortotheextentthatvotersshifteithertheirLeft- thatwedonotobservepositiveandstatisticallysignificant Right positions (the party persuasion process) or their associations between party supporters’ Left-Right shifts partysupport(thepartisanswitchingprocess)inresponse andshiftsinparties’policystatementsisstriking. to parties’ Left-Right shifts, this should create positive statistical associations between Left-Right shifts by par- ties and shifts by their partisan constituencies. Further- DoCitizensRespondtoParties’Policy more,thesepositivestatisticalassociationsshouldobtain Images? regardless of whether voters respond directly to parties’ Ourfindingthatcitizensdonotsystematicallyadjusttheir policystatements,orindirectlyviacuesfromthepolitical Left-Rightpositionsortheirpartisanshipinresponseto otherpartiesinthesystem.Ourparameterestimatesforthesealter- parties’ policy statements raises the question: to what nativespecifications—whichwereportinsupplementarymaterials policy-based attributes of parties, if any, do voters re- postedonourwebsite(http://privatewww.essex.ac.uk/∼ezrow)— spond? An obvious candidate is parties’ policy images, supportedsubstantiveconclusionsthatwereidenticaltothosewe reporthere. i.e., citizens’ perceptions of the parties’ positions. Given 378 JAMESADAMS,LAWRENCEEZROW,ANDZEYNEPSOMER-TOPCU ourfindingthatparties’policyimagesdonottracktheir mateonthe[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift (t)]variable, campaign-basedpolicystatements,itseemsplausiblethat andonthelaggedversionofthisvariable,arebothnear citizensreacttotheseimagesinlieuofreactingtothepar- zero and are statistically insignificant, which again sup- ties’actualpolicystatements.Toevaluatethishypothesis, portstheconclusionthatparties’campaign-basedpolicy weestimatedtheparametersofamodelthatwasidentical statementsexertlittleeffectonvoters. toequation(2)exceptthatweincludedtwoadditionalin- Next, we estimated the parameters of a model that dependentvariablesthatweintroducedabove,[Partyj’s was identical to the CMP Codings + Voter Perceptions perceivedLeft-Rightshift (t)]and[Partyj’sperceivedLeft- specification,exceptthatwecontrolledforadditionalfac- Rightshift(t−1)],whichrepresentthecurrentandlagged torsthatplausiblyinfluencecitizens’Left-Rightshifts.To shiftsintheparties’policyimages,i.e.,theshiftsinvot- control for national economic conditions (which may ers’perceptionsoftheparties’positions.Byincorporating motivatecitizenstoshifttheirLeft-Rightpolicyorienta- theseperceptualvariablesintoamodelthatalsoincludes tions independently of the parties’ policy positions), we the parties’ shifts as coded from their actual campaign includedthevariables[inflationratechange(t)],defined statements,wecanevaluatetheextenttowhichcitizens’ as the change in the inflation rate since the last elec- partisan adjustments reflect their reactions to their per- tion,and[unemploymentratechange (t)],definedasthe ceptions of the parties’ positions, as opposed to voters’ changeintheunemploymentratesincethelastelection. reactionstoobjectivemeasuresbasedontheactualstate- And,tocontrolforunmeasured,country-specificfactors mentsintheparties’electionmanifestos.21 thatinfluencecitizens’Left-Rightpositions,weincluded The parameter estimates for this CMP Codings + country-specificintercepts.Theparameterestimatesfor Voter Perceptions specification, which we report in col- thismodel,whichwereportincolumn3ofTable3,again umn 2 of Table 3, strongly support the hypothesis that supportthehypothesisthatcitizensreacttotheirpercep- citizens react to their perceptions of the parties’ Left- tionsoftheparties’Left-Rightshifts,butnottoshiftsin Rightshifts,butnottoshiftsintheLeft-Righttoneofthe theLeft-Righttoneoftheparties’actualcampaignstate- parties’actualcampaignstatements.Theseestimatesre- ments.22 flectstrongandpositiverelationshipsbetweenLeft-Right shiftsinthemeanpositionsofpartysupportersandshifts invoters’perceptionsoftheparties’left-Rightpositions: RobustnessChecksBasedonanAlternative thecoefficientestimate+0.635onthe[Partyj’sperceived DataSet:TheEurobarometerSurveys Left-Rightshift(t)]variableindicatesthat,ceterisparibus, While we have so far uncovered no evidence that voters aone-unitshiftinaparty’sperceivedpositiononthe1–10 adjusttheirpartisanshiportheirLeft-Rightpositionsin Left-Rightscaleisassociatedwitha0.635-unitshiftinthe response to parties’ campaign-based policy statements, mean position of the party’s partisan constituency on our analyses encompass only five European electorates, the same 1–10 scale, during the same interelection pe- riod.And,theestimate+0.326onthe[Partyj’sperceived namelyBritain,Germany,theNetherlands,Sweden,and Left-Rightshift (t−1)]variableindicatesthataone-unit Norway (the five European countries for which NES data are available over long time periods). In order to shift in a party’s perceived Left-Right position is asso- ciated with an approximate 0.3-unit shift in the mean 22We estimated the parameters of two additional specifications position of the party’s partisan constituency during the in an effort to control for statistical issues that might affect our subsequent interelection period. Both of these estimates substantiveconclusions.First,whilewefindnoevidenceofagen- arestatisticallysignificant(p<.01),andtheysupportthe eral relationship between parties’ stated policy shifts and shifts in their supporters’ positions, it seems possible that there are hypothesis that voters adjust their Left-Right positions “threshold effects,” whereby voters react to party policy shifts and/or their party support in response to their percep- if these shifts are sufficiently large. Second, we estimated a sec- tionsofparties’Left-Rightshifts—particularlythelagged ond alternative version of our CMP Codings + Voter Percep- relationshipsweidentify,whichcannotbeascribedtothe tionsspecification,inwhichshiftsinparties’perceivedpositions weredefinedasthedifferenceintheparty’spositionasperceived reciprocal causal process whereby parties react to their by all respondents except for the party’s supporters in the fo- supporters’Left-Rightshifts.Finally,theparameteresti- cal election. We estimated the parameters of this Voter Projec- tion specification in order to evaluate the hypothesis that the positive relationship that we estimate between parties’ perceived 21Wenotethatourmodeldoesnotsufferfrommulticollinearity Left-Right shifts and shifts in their supporters’ Left-Right self- problemsbecausethecorrelationbetweenourmeasuresofparties’ placements is due to party supporters projecting their own po- shifts based upon their campaign statements versus voters’ per- sitionsontotheirpreferredparty.Ouranalysesforthesealternative ceptionsofparties’shiftsisverylow,i.e.,thecorrelationbetween specifications—whichwereportinsupplementarymaterialsposted theobservedvaluesofthe[Partyj’sperceivedLeft-Rightshift (t)] onourwebsite(http://privatewww.essex.ac.uk/∼ezrow)—support variableandthe[Partyj’sstatedLeft-Rightshift(t)]variableisonly substantive conclusions that are identical to those we reported .05. above. 379 ISANYBODYLISTENING? evaluatewhetherourconclusionsgeneralizetootherEu- Insummary,wefindnoevidencethatcitizensreactto ropeanelectorates,wereestimatedtheparametersofthe shiftsinEuropeanparties’campaign-basedpolicystate- partisan adjustment specification (equation 2) using al- mentsbysignificantlyshiftingeithertheirownLeft-Right ternativemeasuresofpartisanconstituencies’Left-Right positionsortheirpartisanloyalties.Thisconclusionholds shifts that were derived from Eurobarometer surveys. regardlessofwhetherwemeasurepartysupporters’Left- These surveys have been administered in the following Right shifts using national election study data from five Western European democracies beginning in the early countries (Britain, Germany, the Netherlands, Sweden, 1970s:Britain,Italy,Denmark,France,Portugal,Greece, and Norway) or Eurobarometer data from 12 European Spain, Luxembourg, the Netherlands, Belgium, Ireland, democracies. Simply put, we find no evidence that the andGermany.23 Thesesurveyscontainthesameitemin mean Left-Right positions of party supporters move in each year in each country, asking approximately 1,000 tandemwiththeLeft-Rightpolicystatementsthatparties respondents per country to place themselves on a scale publishintheirelectionmanifestos.Bycontrast,wefind runningfrom1(extremeleft)to10(extremeright),and a strong and positive relationship between citizens’ per- alsotoindicatetheparty(ifany)thattheysupport.24We ceptions of parties’ Left-Right shifts, and the Left-Right used these Eurobarometer items to construct the vari- shiftsoftheseparties’supporters. ables [Party j supporters’ Left-Right shift (t)] and [Party j supporters’ Left-Right shift (t−1)] that we used as our dependentvariablesintheNES-basedanalysesdescribed DiscussionandConclusion above.(Wenotethatwecannotusethesedatatoconstruct measuresofvoters’perceptionsofpartyshiftsbecausethe Eurobarometersurveysdonotaskrespondentstolocate Ourfinding,thatvotersreactstronglytotheirperceptions theparties’Left-Rightpositions.)Wethenreestimatedthe ofparties’Left-Rightshiftsbutnottoparties’actualshifts parameters of equation (2) on all partisan constituency as coded by the Comparative Manifesto Project (CMP), Left-Right shifts that are covered in the Eurobarometer raisestroublingquestionsaboutthenatureofmass-elite surveys—296 party constituency shifts in 12 European policylinkages.InsituationswherepartiesshifttheLeft- democracies,roughlytriplethenumberofpartyshiftsin Rightorientationsofthepolicystatementsthattheypub- ourNES-basedanalyses.25 Column4inTable3presents lishintheirelectionmanifestos,wefindnoevidencethat theparameterestimates,whichdisplaythesamepatterns voters respond by adjusting their own Left-Right posi- astheestimatesbasedontheNESdata:thecoefficientes- tions,theirpartisanloyalties,oreventheirperceptionsof timatesonthekeyindependentvariables,[Partyj’sstated theparties’Left-Rightpositions.Thesefindingsarestrik- Left-Right shift (t)] and [Party j’s stated Left-Right shift ing given that previous research concludes that citizens (t−1)], are almost exactly zero, so that there is no evi- displayheightenedlevelsofpoliticalattentionaroundthe denceofstatisticallyorsubstantivelysignificantpartisan timeofelectioncampaigns,andthatoureliteinterviews adjustmentsbyvotersinresponsetoshiftsinparties’pol- suggestthatpartiescampaignonthebasisoftheirmani- icystatements.26 festos. While our findings may appear surprising, we have notedconsiderationsthatcastdoubtonparties’abilities 23WenotethatSpainandPortugalwerenotincludedintheEuro- toshapetheirpolicyimagesduringpoliticalcampaigns, barometersurveysuntil1981and1986,respectively. includingthefactthatmanycitizensareinattentivetopol- 24IneachEurobarometersurvey,respondentsareaskedtoindicate itics;thatrivalpartiesandmediaelitesmaychallengethe their party support by answering the question: “If there were a sincerityofthefocalparty’spolicypromises;thatcitizens ‘generalelection’tomorrow,whichpartywouldyousupport?” tend to assimilate new political information into their 25Theseanalysescovered82partiescompetingin69elections.We preexisting schemas; and that citizens must balance the estimatedtheparametersofthismodelusingrobuststandarderrors clusteredbyparty(Rogers1993),andaLagrangemultipliertestfails policystatementsthatpartiesissueduringelectioncam- torejectthenullhypothesisofnoserialcorrelation. paignsagainstparties’statements(andbehavior)during 26Wereestimatedtheparametersofequation(2)onseveraldiffer- the precampaign period. It is this balance of conflicting ent subsets of parties using the Eurobarometer data, in order to considerations that prompted us to undertake this re- explorewhethervoters’partisanadjustmentstoparties’campaign- search project. We have diligently searched for evidence basedpolicystatementsaremediatedbythetypeofpartyorbythe characteristicsofthepartysystem.Thesesupplementaryanalyses that citizens react to the policy statements that parties included separate estimates on all governing parties, and on all oppositionparties;onallleft-wingparties,andonallright-wing (2006,chapter5)identifyasrevolvingprimarilyaroundLeft-Right parties;andontheBritish,Dutch,andItalianparties,i.e.,thethree economic issues. All of these analyses continued to support our partysystemsintheEurobarometersurveysthatBenoitandLaver substantiveconclusions.

Description:
Somer-Topcu is Assistant Professor of Political Science, Vanderbilt . ties make determined efforts to campaign based on their things like that.7.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.