Francesco Arcidiacono Antonio Bova Editors Interpersonal Argumentation in Educational and Professional Contexts Interpersonal Argumentation in Educational and Professional Contexts Francesco Arcidiacono Antonio Bova (cid:129) Editors Interpersonal Argumentation in Educational and Professional Contexts 123 Editors Francesco Arcidiacono AntonioBova HEP-BEJUNE Universitàdella Svizzera italiana University of Teacher Education Lugano Biel/Bienne Switzerland Switzerland ISBN978-3-319-59083-7 ISBN978-3-319-59084-4 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-59084-4 LibraryofCongressControlNumber:2017941464 ©SpringerInternationalPublishingAG2017 Thisworkissubjecttocopyright.AllrightsarereservedbythePublisher,whetherthewholeorpart of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission orinformationstorageandretrieval,electronicadaptation,computersoftware,orbysimilarordissimilar methodologynowknownorhereafterdeveloped. The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publicationdoesnotimply,evenintheabsenceofaspecificstatement,thatsuchnamesareexemptfrom therelevantprotectivelawsandregulationsandthereforefreeforgeneraluse. The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authorsortheeditorsgiveawarranty,expressorimplied,withrespecttothematerialcontainedhereinor for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictionalclaimsinpublishedmapsandinstitutionalaffiliations. Printedonacid-freepaper ThisSpringerimprintispublishedbySpringerNature TheregisteredcompanyisSpringerInternationalPublishingAG Theregisteredcompanyaddressis:Gewerbestrasse11,6330Cham,Switzerland Foreword When I was asked by one of my best former students, Francesco Arcidiacono, currentlyprofessorattheUniversityofTeacherEducationBEJUNEinBiel/Bienne, and by his collaborator, Dr. Antonio Bova, to write a foreword for their relevant bookon“InterpersonalArgumentationinEducationalandProfessionalContexts,”I was looking at the impressive collection of scientific contributions, within a large set of national realities, that are included in the present volume. These papers strongly contribute to the further development of research on argumentative dimensions connected to educational and professional contexts, because of their natureofdistinctiveandnewcontributionsinthefieldofargumentation.Thistopic andthecurrentstateofthefieldarewellrepresentedinthisvolumeandofferedme the possibility to look back at my past experiences in studying argumentation in Italian schools over the last 40 years. In fact, in the 1980s, through studying classroomdiscussionsoncurriculainbiology,physicsandsocialhistory,chosenby ourselves in fifth grade classrooms, we observed, through the careful and repeated reading of the transcripts of didactic interactions, how children of primary school were solicited to develop their reasoning and explanation in order to answer to the pertinentobjectionsofaclassmate(thatwehaveindicatedas“sceptic”byvirtueof adissatisfactionbytheexplanationsadvancedbyothers).Whenteacherswereopen to giving children the freedom to express their ideas (as was the case of two excellent teachers, Delia Castiglia and Ornella Formentini, participating in our research at the Montessori school “S.M. Goretti” in Rome), the great educational relevance of verbal interactions among children of different social status was immediately visible. My idea at that time (Pontecorvo 1987) was firstly based, at a theoretical level, on the pivotal works of Toulmin (1958) and of Perelman and Olbrechts-Tyteca (1971) that had the merit to value the classical rhetoric tradition. However, I was learning something very interesting by listening to a sentence produced by a child of 2.5 years, Marianna, the daughter of two friends who were guests in my countryside home. The child was eating vanilla and chocolate ice cream offered to herand,whenthemother(anexcellentscientistofchilddevelopmentandexpertin health)enteredtheroom,said:“Butthevanillaishealthy!”avoidinganyreference v vi Foreword to chocolate and showing the need to account for her action by a valid argument, preventing possible comments against the mother’s objection to commercial ice cream. During that period of research, as one of the main interest was the scientific thoughtofchildren(especiallywithintheresearchgroupUniversità-Scuola,G.U.S. in 1979), I was regularly invited to the “Journées internationales sur l’éducation scientifique”promotedbyAndréGiordaninChamonixduringtheperiodofJanuary andFebruaryof1983and1984.Duringthesemeetings,Iwasdiscoveringtheworks of Ducrot (1980) and Caron (1983) that were showing to what extent, even in the course of ordinary conversation (written and oral), there are different degrees and scales of argumentation, referred to various knowledge values. In addition, by developingtheworkonthescientificcurriculum,webecomemoreandmoreaware oftheconnectionbetweenopposing,arguingandexplaining(cf.Pontecorvo1983, 1990). Always in Chamonix (Giordan and Martinand 1984) I was in charge, with JeanMathieu,ofaseminarofdiscussionabouttheargumentativestrategies(withthe participation,amongothers,ofcolleaguessuchasCaravita,Caron,Coty,Gagliardi, Lammé, Vermès and Wermus). During the very intense verbal interactions of the group we weredebatingdifferent interestingtopics, suchasthe needtogo beyond the opposition between natural and scientific thoughts, scientific explanation and argumentativethinking.Weweremainlyorientedtowardsaneducationalapproach valuingclassroomdiscussionasapowerfultooltopromoteeffectiveargumentation andtoconstructthescientificthoughtanddiscourseofchildren.Lateron,duringthe biennial EARLI congresses, I was confronted with the studies on argumentation done by Baruch Schwarz and colleagues (Schwarz and Glassner 2003). In his original research (Schwarz 2011), he was studying the discussions on Talmud amongstudentsofYeshivà(Israeliinstitutestotraintherabbis).Thesecontributions wereopeningnewavenuesinthefieldofargumentationthatisstillofactualinterest, as it isthecase in thepresentvolume. In the further works of my research group, we discovered the relevance of the child’s position as a sign of “assuming a point of view” within a small group (see thevolumepresentedin1991“Discutendosiimpara”editedbyPontecorvo,Ajello and Zucchermaglio; and Orsolini and Pontecorvo 1992), and not only during classroom discussion, when the teacher is able to uptake a valuable child’s inter- vention which is not considered by other interlocutors. The cited book has been frequentlyusedintheItalianschoolsystemandhasbeentranslatedintoPortuguese andpublishedinBrazil(Pontecorvoetal.2005).Inaddition,wewereexperiencing other designs involving small groups of kindergarten and primary school children (Pontecorvo 1993). These studies have indicated interesting key points in the field ofeducational argumentationandhaveinspirednewdirections,asitisthecasefor some of theworks presentedinthis volume.Thesecontributions havethe merit of expanding and improving our knowledge about the interpersonal dimensions connected to argumentation. A few years beforehand, we were discovering the relevant work of Michael Billig(1987).Iwasimpressedbytheextendeduseofancientsources,includingthe Foreword vii studies of Greek rhetoric, as well as the use of the famous story of Rav Eliezer (presentedintheBabylonianTalmud,theTractatusBabaMetzia):indiscussingthe ritual cleaning of an oven with students, Rav Eliezer defends a thesis that is not accepted by others; after having recalled two miraculous events which really happened (the moving of a carob tree and the removing of an external wall of the house) to sustain the thesis, he appeals to God’s intervention and got it. Consequently, thegroup ofRabbis opposes Godandcitesfrom theBible that“the law is not in the sky (…) and a legal decision does not depend on the age of the teacherbutonthepowerofhis/herargumentation.”Asitwasexpected,thevolume of Billig produced a revolutionary effect within the field of contemporary social psychology,evenasproductoftheresearchgroup“DiscourseandRhetoric”atthe University of Loughborough, also involving Jonathan Potter and Margaret Wetherell, as well as Derek Edwards and David Middleton, from whom I read variousresearchconcerningdifferentsocialcontextsandwhomImetseveraltimes during EARLI congresses. Concerning Billig’s conception, he affirms that all essential cognitive activities are produced by a discourse of several participants, even within memory. Thus, what is considered in the psychology field as an individualmentalactivityisacollectiveproduct(asitemergesfromabookwritten in 1990 by Edwards and Middleton on remembering). Thanks to Billig we have discovered the role of Bakhtin, who, translated into English few times before, underlined, by citing Plato, the function of dialogue, in which truth “shoots like a spark” in the relationship between two speakers (p. 29). The interactionist and discursive conception of thought was connected to our constant re-reading of “Thought and Language” written by Vygotskij (1990), and also to a new attention for the work of Hannah Arendt for whom “the individual thought is a form of internal dialogue” (p. 42). Different contributions in this volume are excellent examplesofhowtheseconceptionsarestillrelevantineducationalandprofessional contexts in which interpersonal argumentation plays a central role. Research on childrenoffourthgradeinaschoolofRome,startingfromawrittendocumentofa Roman historian, led us to discover the presence of a counterfactual reasoning in autonomous discussions of small groups: through analysing this reasoning, I had the privilege to be accompanied by Merrilee Salmon, excellent professor of informal logics at the University of Pittsburgh, and really interested by the col- lectiveargumentationofourItalianfourthgradechildren(GirardetandPontecorvo 1993). Having those conceptual and methodological tools at our disposal, in 1991 (duringtheISCRATCongressinLahti,Finland,duringaseminaronliteracyledby Elinor Ochs) we discovered the Conversation Analysis (CA) as a powerful tool to read and interpret human interaction in different contexts (Ochs 1986). Consequently, we started a national research project (in an analogous way to the research that Elinor Ochs was conducting at the same time in Los Angeles) on dinnertime conversations of Italian middle-class families. This topic is still con- stitutingarelevantfieldofresearchwithineducationalargumentation,asshowedin oneofthecontributionsinthisvolume.Inmyexperience,theCAapproachhadthe merit to solicit a collaborative process of work and analysis: in fact, theresearcher Alessandra Fasulo and (at that time) the advanced Ph.D. student Laura Sterponi, viii Foreword bothexpertsofCA,weremembersoftheresearchgroupandfullyimmersedinthe context of the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA). The group included four new Ph.D. students (Francesco Arcidiacono, Marilena Fatigante, Vivian Liberati and Barbara Maroni), as well as a variable number of master students in developmental and educational psychology, under my supervision. During that period,wereceivedavisitfromDr.TaniaZittoun(currentlyprofessorofeducation at the University of Neuchâtel). Tania Zittoun was immediately interested in the cooperative atmosphere of the group, in which the master students were in primis referring to one of the Ph.D. students and only later discussing their works within the whole research group. This modality of reciprocal and collaborative teaching and learning could have been an input for the promotion of a further international seminar organized by Tania Zittoun with her colleagues Baucal, Cornish and Gillespie, and sponsored by the European Science Foundation, on the issue of “collaboration in psychological research,” in Veysonnaz (Switzerland) in June 2006, which included five collaborative case studies. At the meeting were inter- national well-known researchers, among which Engeström, proposing his applicative research in different social contexts (based on the Activity Theory of Leont’ev), Plichova and Marková, studying prospective ideological variables in Eastern European countries at the end of the Soviet period. However, the research that was probably among the most interesting for me was the study “DUNES” coordinated by Baruch Schwarz in collaboration with a Swiss group headed by Michèle Grossen about the argumentative education and the relevant informatics implications. The connection between educational aspects and technological innovations in the field of argumentation is well highlighted by some of the con- tributions of the present volume. Iapologize tomyfriends andeditorsofthebookif their request for a foreword has activated such an autobiographical personal discovery of the relevance of argumentation,inconnectiontothevolume’scontributionsofmanyotherexcellent colleagues. The international dimension of the various papers is a relevant aspect and a merit of the present book: the study of interpersonal argumentation in dif- ferentcontextsandrealitiesstronglycontributestounderstandingthecomplexityof socio-cultural dimensions connected to argumentative processes. More recently, I have been thinking about the fact that, starting from the doctoral study on Italian familiesconductedbyLauraSterponi,wehaveunderstoodtherelevanceofparental requests towards children, of the need to account for a violation (action) or for incorrect verbal expressions. These requests are almost absent within the middle-classfamilyinteractionsstudied byOchsinLosAngeles(Ochs andTaylor 1992),inwhichchildrenobeyveryquicklytoparentaldirectives.Itisuniquethata requestofaccountinfamilyplaystheroleofpromotingthechildrenargumentation (Pontecorvo and Arcidiacono 2016), as well as that the opposition solicited in classroom discussions promotes argumentation and counter-argumentation in kindergarten and primary children. It is possible that the role of verbal opposition, at least in Italian families, is played by an attempt of not complying to disobey parental requests. However, by recognizing in this important volume the multiple settings that concern the use of argumentative strategies that are analogous yet Foreword ix varied,Imustobservetowhatextentargumentationcanbeusedalsobeyondschool and family contexts. It is possible that the educational context keeps a type of ontogenetic priority, while the application to contexts of professional training or different organizations opens new relevant perspectives. I believe that this volume will contribute to the further development of inter- personal argumentation based on a common socio-cultural ground and varied applications to different social contexts. Clotilde Pontecorvo “Sapienza” University of Rome, Italy References Billig,M.(1987).Arguingandthinking:Arhetoricalapproachtosocialpsychology.Cambridge: CambridgeUniversityPress. Caron,J.(1983).Lesrégulationsdudiscours.Paris:PUF. Ducrot,O.(1980).Leséchellesargumentatives.Paris:LesEditionsdeMinuit. Edwards,D.,&Middleton,D.(1990).Collectiveremembering.London:Sage. Giordan, A., & Martinand, J. L. (1984). Journées internationales sur l’éducation scientifique, Chamonix:D.C.R.I. Girardet,H.,&Pontecorvo,C.(1993).Arguingandreasoninginunderstandinghistoricaltopics. CognitionandInstruction,11,365–395. G.U.S.(1979).L’educazionescientificadibase.Florence:LaNuovaItalia. Ochs,E.(1986).Introduction.InB.Schieffelin&E.Ochs(Eds.),Languagesocializationacross cultures(pp.1–13).Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. Ochs, E., & Taylor,C. (1992).Family narrative as political activity. Discourse & Society, 3(3), 301–340. Orsolini, M., & Pontecorvo,C. (1992). Children’stalk in classroom discussions. Cognition and Instruction,9(2),113–136. Perelman, C., & Olbrechts-Tyteca, L. (1971). The new rhetoric: A treatise on argumentation. NotreDame,IN:UniversityofNotreDamePress. Pontecorvo,C.(1983).Children’sscience,children’sthinking,whatisitabout?InD.E.Hadary & M. Vicentini (Eds.), Proceedings of US—Italy Joint Seminar on Science Education for ElementarySchoolChildren.CEDE,TheAmericanUniversity. Pontecorvo,C.(1987),Discussingforreasoning:Theroleofargumentinknowledgeconstruction. In E. De Corte, J. G. L. C. Lodewijks, R. Parmentier & P. Span (Eds.), Learning and instruction(71–82).Leuven:LeuvenUniversityPress. Pontecorvo, C. (1990). Opposition, explication et invocation des règles dans la discussion en classe entre enfants de cinq ans. In Actes du Colloque International “Le Jeune Enfant et l’Explication”.Paris:CALAP. Pontecorvo, C. (1993). Forms of discourse and shared thinking. Cognition and Instruction, 3, 189–196. Pontecorvo,C.,Ajello,A.M.,&Zucchermaglio,C.(1991).Discutendosiimpara.Rome:Carocci. Pontecorvo,C.,Ajello,A.M.,&Zucchermaglio,C.(2005).Discutindoseaprende.PortoAlegre: Artmed. Pontecorvo, C., & Arcidiacono, F. (2016). The dialogic construction of justifications and arguments in a seven-year-old child within a “democratic” family. Language and Dialogue, 6(2),306–328. x Foreword Schwarz, B. B. (2011). “Hevruta” learning in Lithuanian Yeshivas: Recurrent learning of Talmudic issues. In I. Etkes, T. El’or, M. Heyd & B. B. Schwarz (Eds.), Education and religion:Authorityandautonomy(pp.279–308).Jerusalem:MagnessPublishingHouse. Schwarz, B. B., & Glassner, A. (2003). Construction of collective and individual knowledge in argumentative activity: An empirical study. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 12(2), 221–258. Toulmin,S.E.(1958).Theusesofargument.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. Vygotskij, L. S. (1990). Pensiero e linguaggio [Thought and Language, orig. ed. 1934]. Bari: Laterza.
Description: