ebook img

Hyper-active gap filling PDF

26 Pages·2015·0.62 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Hyper-active gap filling

ORIGINALRESEARCH published:10April2015 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00384 Hyper-active gap filling AkiraOmaki1*,EllenF.Lau2,ImogenDavidsonWhite2,MylesL.Dakan2,AaronApple1 andColinPhillips2 1DepartmentofCognitiveScience,JohnsHopkinsUniversity,Baltimore,MD,USA,2DepartmentofLinguistics,Universityof Maryland,CollegePark,MD,USA Much work has demonstrated that speakers of verb-final languages are able to construct rich syntactic representations in advance of verb information. This may reflect general architectural properties of the language processor, or it may only reflect a language-specific adaptation to the demands of verb-finality. The present study addressesthisissuebyexaminingwhetherspeakersofaverb-mediallanguage(English) wait toconsult verb transitivity information before constructingfiller-gap dependencies, where internal arguments are fronted and hence precede the verb. This configuration makes it possible to investigate whether the parser actively makes representational commitmentsonthegappositionbeforeverbtransitivityinformationbecomesavailable. A key prediction of the view that rich pre-verbal structure building is a general architectural property is that speakers of verb-medial languages should predictively construct dependencies in advance of verb transitivity information, and therefore that disruptionshould beobservedwhentheverbhasintransitivesubcategorization frames Editedby: that are incompatible with the predicted structure. In three reading experiments (self- ClaudiaFelser, pacedandeye-tracking)thatmanipulatedverbtransitivity,wefoundevidenceforreading UniversityofPotsdam,Germany Reviewedby: disruption when the verb was intransitive, although no such reading difficulty was ArildHestvik, observed when the critical verb was embedded inside a syntactic island structure, UniversityofDelaware,USA which blocks filler-gap dependency completion. These results are consistent with the OliverBoxell, UniversityofPotsdam,Germany hypothesis that in English, as in verb-final languages, information from preverbal noun *Correspondence: phrases is sufficient to trigger active dependency completion without having access to AkiraOmaki, verbtransitivityinformation. DepartmentofCognitiveScience, JohnsHopkinsUniversity,3400North Keywords: filler-gap dependency, active gap filling, prediction, verb transitivity, island, plausibility mismatch CharlesStreet,237Krieger, effects,eye-tracking Baltimore,MD21218,USA [email protected] Specialtysection: Introduction Thisarticlewassubmittedto LanguageSciences,asectionofthe Aleadinggoalofsentenceprocessingresearchistounderstandhowtheparseradaptstoamulti- journalFrontiersinPsychology tudeoflinguisticdifferencesacrosslanguagestoenablesuccessfulcomprehension.Inthisregard, Received:22December2014 comparisonsofverb-medialandverb-finallanguageshaveprovidedavaluablesourceofevidence Accepted:18March2015 (MazukaandLust,1990;InoueandFodor,1995).Themainverbcontainsrichinformationsuch Published:10April2015 assubcategorizationandthematicroleinformationthatiscriticalforconstructingstructuralanal- Citation: ysesandinterpretations(e.g.,Chomsky,1965;Grimshaw,1990;PollardandSag,1994;Levinand OmakiA,LauEF,DavidsonWhiteI, Rappaport Hovav,1995). Much experimental evidence shows that the verb is a valuable source DakanML,AppleAandPhillipsC of information for parsing (e.g., Ford et al., 1982; Tanenhaus and Carlson, 1989; Boland et al., (2015)Hyper-activegapfilling. 1990; MacDonald et al., 1994; Spivey-Knowlton and Sedivy,1995; Garnsey et al., 1997; Mauner Front.Psychol.6:384. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00384 andKoenig,2000;Traxleretal.,2002;BlodgettandBoland,2004;SnedekerandTrueswell,2004). FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 1 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling Theimportanceoftheinformationfromtheverbheadhasengen- associatesthefillerwiththeverbinadvanceoftheverbacrosslan- dered theoretical claims that structure building processes do guages,regardlessofdifferencesinverbpositions. Theseresults not even start until the parser encounters the head of a phrase suggestthattheprocedureforfiller-gapdependencycompletion (e.g.,verbalhead)tobeconstructed,eveninverb-finallanguages may be uniform across languages, and are consistent with the wherethiswouldbesignificantlydelayed(Abney,1989;Pritchett, viewthattheparserpredictively constructs richrepresentations 1992). attheearliestpossiblemomentinadvanceofcriticalbottom–up However, subsequent empirical research on verb-final lan- evidence. guageslike Japanese or Germanhasgenerated evidence against such head-driven parsing theories in their strongest form, Background on ActiveFiller-Gap demonstrating that the parser uses various morphological and Dependency Processing syntactic cues to incrementally build structures and interpre- Pastresearchonfiller-gapdependencyprocessinghasestablished tations in verb-final languages (Bader and Lasser, 1994; Koh, thattheparserpostulatesagapbeforethereissufficientbottom– 1997;ClahsenandFeatherston,1999;KamideandMitchell,1999; up evidence to confirm that analysis (Active gap filling: Fodor, Konieczny, 2000; Bornkessel et al., 2002; Felser et al., 2003; 1978; Crain and Fodor, 1985; Stowe, 1986; Frazier and Flores Kamideetal.,2003;Aoshimaetal.,2009;Yoshida,unpublished d’Arcais,1989).Forexample,Stowe(1986)observedtheso-called doctoraldissertation).Thus,althoughverbinformationstrongly Filledgapeffectin(2),i.e.,slowerreadingtimesatthedirectobject influencesparsingdecisionswhenavailable,speakersofverb-final position us in the wh-fronting condition (2a) than in a control languagesoftenbeginbuildingsyntactic andsemanticstructure conditionthatdidnotinvolvewh-fronting(2b).Thispatternof inadvanceoftheverb. readingtimessuggeststhattheparserhadalreadypositedagap Thesefindingsraisethequestionofwhetherpre-verbalstruc- followingthetransitiveverb,beforecheckingwhetherthedirect ture building reflects a language-specific adaptation to the pro- objectpositionwasoccupied. cessingdemandsofverb-finality,orratherapropertyofageneral parsingarchitecturethatspeakersofalllanguagesuse.Forexam- (2) a. MybrotherwantedtoknowwhoRuthwillbringushome ple,considerlessfrequentcasesinverb-mediallanguageswhere to____atChristmas. multiple arguments precede the verb. A classic example of this b. MybrotherwantedtoknowifRuthwillbringushometo comesfromprocessingof‘filler-gap’dependenciesasillustrated MomatChristmas. bytherelativeclauseconstructionshownin(1),wheretheobject nounphrase(NP)thecity(calledthefiller)isdislocatedfromthe Convergingevidencecomesfromaneye-trackingexperimentby post-verbal thematic position (called the gap1), and the parser TraxlerandPickering(1996),whomanipulatedthethematicfit needs to associate the filler and the gap in order to assign a betweenthefillerandthepotentialverbhost,asin(3). thematicinterpretation. (3) We like the city/book that the author wrote unceasingly (1) The city that the author visited ____ was named for an and with great dedication about _____ while waiting for a explorer. contract. Ithasbeenreportedthatspeakersofverb-finallanguagescom- TraxlerandPickeringfoundaplausibilitymismatcheffectat plete filler-gap dependencies in advance of verb information, thecriticalverbin(3),i.e.,thefirstfixationtimeattheoptionally associatingthefillerwiththeearlieststructuralpositionwherea transitive verbwrote increased when the fillerwasanimplausi- thematicrole could beassigned(pre-verbalobject gapcreation: bleobjectoftheverb(i.e.,thecity),comparedtowhenthefiller Nakano et al., 2002; Aoshima et al., 2004). The current study was a plausible object of the verb (i.e., the book). This suggests examines whether this may also be the case in a verb-medial thatatleastasearlyastheverbposition,theparserpostulatesa language like English, and whether pre-verbal gap creation is a gap and analyzes the filler as the object of the verb,even when language-general parsing procedure rather than an adaptation the filler is a poor semantic fit to that role. In fact, there is specifictoverb-finallanguages.Underthishypothesis,wepredict ampletimecourseevidenceforactiveobjectgapcreation,using thatEnglishspeakersshouldpositagapirrespective ofwhether a variety of dependentmeasuressuch asreading time and gaze theverbultimatelylicensesadirectobjectgapposition,andthat durationmeasures(CrainandFodor,1985;Frazier,1987;Frazier signsofreadingdisruptionshouldbeobservedincaseswherethe andClifton,1989;deVincenzi,1991;PickeringandTraxler,2001, verbdoesnotaccommodateadirectobject. 2003; Aoshima et al., 2004; Phillips, 2006; Wagers and Phillips, We report the results of three on-line reading experiments 2009), cross-modal priming (Nicol and Swinney, 1989; Nicol, in Englishthattestedthis prediction byexamining the effect of 1993; Nakano et al., 2002), visual world eye-tracking (Sussman verbtransitivityonreadingtimesinfiller-gapconfigurations.The and Sedivy, 2003) as well as event-related potentials (Garnsey resultsareconsistentwiththehypothesisthattheparseractively etal.,1989;Featherstonetal.,2000;Kaanetal.,2000;Felseretal., 2003;Phillipsetal.,2005;Gouveaetal.,2010). 1Inthispaperweusethe‘gap-filling’or‘gap-creation’terminologyinatheory- The work summarized above may suggest that filler-gap neutralway,asistypicalinthepsycholinguisticliterature.Thisterminologyshould dependency completion is triggered only after the parser gains notbetakenasindicatingacommitmenttorepresentationsthatincludegapsor accesstotheverbandconfirmsthattheverbistransitiveandis traces;alloftheprocessingtheorieswediscussherecouldbespecifiedintermsof representationsthatdonotincludeemptycategories. abletosyntacticallyaccommodateanobject.However,evidence FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 2 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling that active dependency completion does not depend on verb Inverb-mediallanguageslikeEnglish,verbsbecomeavailable information has been presented by studies that investigated (i) relatively earlier in the sentence, such that the average work- subjectgapcreationinEnglish,aswellas(ii)objectgapcreation ing memory cost of waiting for the verb would be less than in inverb-finallanguages.Forexample,Lee(2004)usedsentences verb-finallanguages.Theadvantageofwaitingfortheverbinfor- like(4)torevealafilledgapeffectinthesubjectNPposition. mation is that the parser can reduce the likelihood of making riskycommitments,becausetheverbmayturnouttobeintran- (4) a. Thatisthe laboratory which, ontwo differentoccasions, sitiveanddisallowanobjectNPanalysisforthefiller.InEnglish, Ireneusedacouriertodeliverthesamplesto___. therefore,theparsermaycreateanobjectgappositiononlyafter b. Thatisthelaboratorytowhich,ontwodifferentoccasions, theverbisconfirmedtobetransitive.Thisstillconstitutesactive Ireneusedacouriertodeliverthesamples___. gapfilling,intherespectthattheultimategappositionmayturn out to be somewhere later than the object position [e.g.,after a Here,thecontentofthewh-fillerismanipulatedinsuchaway late-arriving preposition gap, as in (2) and (3)]. Let us call this thatthewh-fillercanplausiblybeasubject(4a)ornot(4b).The aconservativeactivegapfillingmechanism,sincethebottom–up resultsshowed a longerreading time atthe subject NPIrenein subcategorizationinformationfromtheverbstillplaysacritical (4a)thanin(4b),suggestingthattheparserhadpostulatedasub- role in the parser’s decision on whether to postulate an object jectgapbeforeencounteringtheactualsubjectNP.Althoughthis gapor not. Thisviewof active gapfilling isratherstandard for interpretationhasbeenchallenged(Staub,2010),itwouldinany explainingfiller-gapdependencycompletioninverbmediallan- case not be surprising that the parser actively creates a subject guageslikeEnglish.Forexample,McElreeandGriffith(1998)and gapwithouthavingaccesstoverbinformation,giventhatasub- McElreeetal.(2003)havearguedthatthe dependencycomple- ject is present in any sentence,regardlessof verb properties. In tion process is triggered when the parser accesses information thissense,ifverbinformationweretoplayaroleintheparser’s fromtheverbandinitiatestheretrievalprocessforthefillerthat attempttopositagap,thecriticalempiricalevidenceshouldcome isstoredinworkingmemory(seealsoPickeringandBarry,1991; from dependency completion at the object position, where the LewisandVasishth,2005). presence or absence of anobject gap reliesonproperties of the On the other hand, pre-verbal object gap creation in verb- verb. final languages may reflect a language-general property of the Evidenceforpre-verbalobjectgapcreationhasbeenreported processingarchitecture,althoughevidenceforsuchmechanisms for verb-final languages like Japanese in which the object gap maybesimplymoredifficulttoobtaininverb-mediallanguages. position linearlyprecedestheverb.Forexample,Aoshimaetal. In the English filler-gap case, for example, in any parser that (2004) examined processing of scrambling sentences in which adoptssomeform ofleft-corner strategy(Kimball,1975;Abney a dative object NP was dislocated to the sentence initial posi- and Johnson, 1991; Resnik, 1992; Shieber and Johnson, 1993; tion, and found a filled gap effect at a pre-verbal dative object Stabler,1994;Crocker, 1996;LewisandVasishth,2005;Gibson, position for the first verb phrase (VP) in the sentence (see unpublished doctoral dissertation), the presence of the subject also Omaki et al., 2014). Using similar sentences, Nakano et al. NPallowstheparsertopredictthepresenceofaVP.Giventhata (2002) reported evidence for an antecedent priming effect for VPcancontainanobjectNPposition,theparsercouldprojecta the scrambled NP at a pre-verbal gap position, although the VPwithanobjectNPslotandassignthefillertothisobjectposi- priming effect was only found in the high working memory tionbeforeconfirmingwhethertheupcomingverbisatransitive span group. These data indicate that the parser can in prin- verbornot.Letuscallthisahyper-activegapfillingmechanism, ciple complete filler-gap dependencies before accessing verb because this involvesa more risky predictive structure building information. processthanisstandardlyassumedforactiveobjectgapcreation In verb-medial languages, no such evidence for pre-verbal inEnglish.Fillerretrievalandstructuralintegrationisstillinte- object gap creation has been reported to date. This may reflect gral to the hyper-active gap filling mechanism, but the crucial a real difference between languages in processing strategy, and difference is in what information triggers retrievaland integra- pre-verbalobjectgapcreationinverb-finallanguagesmayreflect tion,andconsequently,atwhatpointinthesentencethisprocess the parser’s adaptation to the demandsof processing these lan- isexecuted. guages.Maintainingastructurallyunintegratedfillerinmemory Itisimportanttonotethateitherofthesetwoactivegapfill- hasbeenarguedtoimposeaburdenonworkingmemory(King ing mechanisms is compatible with the existing data on active and Just, 1991; Gibson, 1998; Gordon et al., 2002; Haarmann objectgapcreationreviewedabove.Afilledgapeffectonlyindi- and Cameron,2005). Alternatively, the parser may be architec- catesthatthegaphadbeencreatedbeforetheactualobjectNPis turally constrained to assign a thematic interpretation to the processed,andthisresultiscompatiblewithbothaccounts,given filler as soon as possible (Pickering and Barry, 1991; Aoshima thatbothhyper-activegapfillingandconservativeactivegapfill- etal.,2004).Onthisview,the parser shouldprioritize integrat- ing mechanisms assume that object NP gap creation happens ing the filler into the first grammatically permissible structural before or on the verb. A plausibility mismatch effect indicates position that canpotentially receive a thematicrole. Giventhat thatwhentheverbispotentiallytransitive,thenthesemanticfit filler-gap dependencies are potentially unbounded, waiting for between the filler and the verb is immediately assessed.This is the verb before constructing the ultimate object gap position also predicted by both accounts. The assessmentof the seman- couldimposealargeprocessingburdenonspeakersofverb-final tic relation between the filler and the verb requires the parser languages. toaccessthe contentofthe verb,bywhich pointthe object gap FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 3 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling position shouldhavebeencreatedoneitheraccount.Thus,nei- Staub(2007)foundlongerfirst-fixationdurationsinthetran- ther paradigm allows us to tease apart the two hypotheses on sitivegapcondition(5a)thaninthetransitiveno-gapcondition what kind of information is sufficient for triggering object gap (5b), but no such difference was observed between the intran- creation. sitive gap and no-gap conditions (5c) and (5d). This pattern of Inthe currentstudyweaimtoteaseapartthepredictions of data supports the prediction of the conservative active gap fill- twohypothesizedmechanismsforactiveobjectgapcreationpro- ing hypothesis, suggesting that the parser does not create an cesses.IfEnglishspeakersconstructthegapsitebeforeencoun- object gap until it checks the transitivity information of the tering the verb, just like speakers of verb-final languages, then verb. One concern about this design, however, is whether the disruptionshouldbeobservedinfiller-gapconfigurations when no-gap condition was truly a neutral baseline against which a the verbturns out to be intransitive, relative to transitive verbs transitivitymismatchcouldbemeasured,asthegapandno-gap (e.g., The party that the student arrived/planned...). According conditions differed substantially in both the linear and struc- totheconservativeactivegapfillingmechanismoutlinedabove, turalpositionoftheverb.AsStaub(2007)pointsout,onepiece the parserwaitsforatransitive verbbeforepostulating the cor- of data suggesting that the control may not have been com- responding gapstructure. Here,nodisruption isexpected atan pletelyneutralisthe factthatreadingtimesontheintransitives intransitive verb,since the parser has not postulated a gap that werenumerically(butnon-significantly) shorterinthegapcon- wouldrequireatransitiveverb. dition than in the no-gap condition. It is important to note Two previous studies are relevant to the two hypotheses here that the gap conditions (5a) and (5c) contain an extra NP about active object gap creation in English. Previous work by (i.e., the head of the relative clause) prior to the critical verb Pickering and Traxler (2003) examined the effect of subcatego- region in comparison to the no-gap conditions (5b) and (5d). rization frequency in optionally transitive verbs (e.g., Those are This may have led to a difference in the amount of contex- thelines/propsthattheauthorspoke[about]...).Itwasfoundthat tualinformationavailablepriortotheverb.Increasedcontextual readers did not take subcategorization frequency into account informationcanfacilitateprocessingforsubsequentlexicalitems in deciding where to posit a gap, as there was a strong prefer- (Stanovich and West, 1983; Van Petten and Kutas, 1990; Kutas encetopositagapintheverbobjectposition(NPcomplement) and Federmeier, 2000), and for this reason, lexical access for even with verbs that more frequently take a PP complement. the intransitive verb in the gap condition may have become Theabsenceofsubcategorizationfrequencyeffectinactiveobject faster and masked the potential reading time slowdown associ- gapcreationcouldbetakentoindicatethatverbinformation is atedwiththestructuralmanipulation.Inanattempttoprovidea not relevant for object gap creation processes. However, all of better test of the predictions of the hyper-active and conserva- the verbs in Pickering and Traxler’s study could grammatically tive active gap filling accounts, the current study used relative accommodateanNPcomplement,andtheparsermaytherefore clause islands as a control condition, which allowed the target havereliedonthetransitivityinformationoftheverbtocreatean sentences to more closely match in informational content and object gap.Therefore,thisfindingdoesnotdistinguishthe pre- wordposition. dictions of the two proposed mechanisms for active object gap creation. Toour knowledge,the onlyprevioustestofthese twoactive Experiment 1 objectgapcreationhypothesesisinExperiment3ofStaub(2007). The test sentences in this experiment (5a–d) manipulated the Experiment1wasaself-pacedreadingstudythatwasdesignedto transitivityoftheverb(calledvs.arrived)andsentencestructure test the predictions of the hyper-active and conservative active (relativeclausewithagapvs.simpledeclarativewithnogap).The gap filling hypotheses, while addressing methodological con- fillerwasmanipulatedtobeanimplausibleobjectofthetransitive cerns about previous work. We employed the transitivity mis- verb(gadget-called).Underthehyper-activegapfillinghypothe- match paradigm used in Staub (2007) in order to test whether sis,theparserineffectpredictsthepresenceofatransitiveverb, averbtransitivitymanipulationaffectsreadingtimeattheverb. andthereforethereadingprocessesinthegapconditionsshould Critically,inthebaselineconditionsthecriticalverbwasembed- be disrupted in either intransitive or transitive condition, but dedinsidearelativeclausestructure,asyntactic‘island’domain fordifferentreasons:whentheverbturnsouttobeintransitive, that prohibits filler-gap dependency formation (Ross, unpub- and processing should also be disrupted when the verb is tran- lished doctoral dissertation; for a review, see Szabolcsi and den sitive because of the plausibility mismatch effect. On the other Dikken,2003).AsamplesetofstimuliisshowninTable1. hand,theconservativeactivegapfillingmechanismpostulatesa A number of previous studies have shown that the parser gap only after checking whether the verb is capable of hosting respects island constraints in real-time syntactic processing, anobjectNP,andthereforereadingdisruptionispredictedonly such that it avoidsactively constructing filler-gap dependencies in the transitive gapcondition due tothe plausibility mismatch that span syntactic island boundaries (Stowe, 1986; Kluender effect. and Kutas, 1993; McKinnon and Osterhout, 1996; Traxler and Pickering,1996;McElreeandGriffith,1998;WagersandPhillips, (5) a. Thegadgetthatthemanagercalledoccasionallyabout... 2009; Omaki and Schulz, 2011; Yoshida, unpublished doctoral b. Themanagercalledoccasionallyaboutthegadget... dissertation).Therelativeclauseislandcondition thusprovided c. Thepartythatthestudentarrivedpromptlyfor... a baseline measure of reading times for the critical transitive d. Thestudentarrivedpromptlyfortheparty... and intransitive verbs, independent of processes of filler-gap FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 4 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling TABLE1|SamplematerialsandconditionsforExperiment1. Analysisregions 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 Transitive,non-island The city that the author wrote regularly about was namedforanexplorer Transitive,island The city that the author who wrote regularly saw was namedforanexplorer Intransitive,non-island The city that the author chatted regularly about was namedforanexplorer Intransitive,island The city that the author who chatted regularly saw was namedforanexplorer Examplequestion Wasthecitynamedforanexplorer? dependencycompletion.Theuseofislandconfigurationsallowed madeavailableinSupplementary Materials.Thetransitivenon- ustoaddressthemethodologicalconcernswithpreviouswork. island and island conditions were taken from the implausible First, this design allowed the baseline condition to present a semantic fit conditions in Omaki and Schulz (2011), who used fillerNPpriortothecriticalregion,suchthatthesameamount a modified version of the plausibility manipulation materials of contextual information from the lexical itemswaspresentin fromTraxlerandPickering(1996).OmakiandSchulzreplicated advance of the critical verb region across the four conditions. Traxler and Pickering’s plausibility mismatch effect with native Second,thewordpositionforthecriticalregions(Regions7and and non-native speakers alike, confirming that the semantic fit 8inTable1)wascloselymatchedacrossconditions(wordposi- between the filler and the verb affects the reading time for the tions6and7inthenon-islandconditions,wordpositions7and verbwhenthe verbisina gapfilling (i.e.,non-island) environ- 8intheislandconditions),anditwasalsoplacedawayfromthe ment, but not when the verb is inside a relative clause island. earlyportionofthesentence. Critically,itwasalsofoundthattheimplausibleverb-fillercom- Furthermore, following Staub’s design, we selected transi- bination in a non-island environment (e.g., city-wrote) led to a tive verbs that are implausible hosts for the filler. Under this significant slow down at the verb compared to its island coun- design,the hyper-active gapfilling hypothesis predicted aread- terpartwiththesameimplausibleverb-fillercombination.Thus, ing time slowdown in both the non-island transitive and the even though the current experiment did not include a plausi- non-island intransitive conditions relative to their island coun- ble counterpart of the implausible transitive verbcondition, we terparts, but for a different reason in the two cases. In the couldbeconfidentthatareadingtimecontrastbetweenthetran- transitive condition, the slowdown would reflect a plausibil- sitivenon-islandandislandconditionsresultsfromthesemantic ity mismatch effect triggered by the poor semantic fit between misfit betweenthe fillerand the verb. Inother words, the find- the filler and the verb. In the intransitive condition, the slow- inginOmakiandSchulz’sstudysupportsthenotionthatisland downwouldresultfromatransitivitymismatcheffectduetothe conditions in general can be used as baseline conditions for a mismatch between the expected subcategorization property of readingdisruptionassociatedwithactiveobjectgapcreation.The the verb(i.e.,transitive) and the actualsubcategorization prop- intransitive conditions were modeled after the transitive condi- erty of the verb. On the other hand, the conservative active tionsbyreplacingtheoptionally transitiveverbwithunergative gap filling hypothesis predicted an interaction. A reading time orunaccusativeintransitiveverbs(LevinandRappaportHovav, contrast should be observed between the non-island transitive 1995). condition and the island transitive condition due to the plausi- The non-island and island conditions differed in the num- bilitymismatcheffect,butnocorresponding contrast shouldbe ber of relative clauses. The non-island condition had only one observedbetweenthetwointransitiveconditions,giventhatthe relative clause (the city that the author wrote/chatted regularly parser should not actively create an object gap in either condi- about), such that the object position of the verb wrote/chatted tion. Note that the lexical difference in the critical verb region was the first potential gap position after the embedded sub- acrossconditionswasnotproblematic,sincethecriticalcontrast ject was encountered. In the island conditions, the critical verb wasbetweennon-island andislandconditions within eachverb was embedded inside another relative clause the author who type. wrote/chatted regularly, such that linearly this was still the first verb but grammatically the filler should not be accessible to Method the verb due to the relative clause island constraint. Thus, the Participants first verb served as the critical region for testing the plausibil- We recruited 32 native speakers of American English from the ityandtransitivitymismatcheffects.Allthetransitiveverbswere UniversityofMarylandcommunity.Theyreceivedacoursecredit optionally transitive, such that the sentences in the island con- or were paid $10 for their participation and were naïve to the ditions were all ultimately grammatical. The subcategorization purposeoftheexperiment. frequency of the optionally transitive verbs was not controlled, sincePickeringandTraxler(2003)havedemonstratedthatplau- Materials sibility mismatch effects are attested for optionally transitive We used 28 setsof four sentences like those shown inTable 1. verbsregardlessofsubcategorizationfrequency.Inallfourcon- All of the stimuli from experiments reported in this paper are ditionsthesameadverbimmediatelyfollowedtheverb,making FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 5 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling itpossibletoobservepotentialspill-overeffects.The28sentence for R (Bates et al., 2014). The fixed effects of island structure setswerecounter-balanced acrossfourlistssothateachpartici- type (non-island vs. island) and verb transitivity (transitive vs. pantsawonly oneversion ofthe targetitemsand consequently intransitive) were coded using sum contrasts, with one levelof readseventokensofeachcondition.Inaddition,72fillersofsim- thefactorcodedas−0.5,andtheotheras0.5.Thissumcontrast ilar length and complexity were constructed and addedto each coding makes the mixed effect model estimates roughly com- list. parabletotheactualaveragereadingtimecontrasts. Themodel includedrandominterceptsforparticipantsanditems.Forran- Procedure dom slopes, we used the following procedure to determine the The self-paced reading task was implemented on the Linger optimal random effect structure (for discussions: Jaeger, 2011; software developed by Doug Rohde (http://tedlab.mit.edu/∼dr/ Barr et al., 2013). First, we constructed a fully crossed model Linger/).Weusedaword-by-word,non-cumulativemovingwin- that included the fixed effects and an interaction term as ran- dowpresentation(Justetal.,1982).Inthisdesign,eachsentence dom slopes for both participants and items.Thisfully specified initially appears as a series of dashes, and these dashes are model failed to converge, plausibly due to the complexity of replaced byaword from lefttoright everytimethe participant the model and missing data points in some of the trials (Barr presses the space bar. In order to ensure that the participants etal.,2013).Next,wesimplified the random effect structure by werepayingattentionwhile readingthesentences,allsentences only keeping the verb transitivity factor as a random slope for were followed by yes-no comprehension questions, and feed- participants and items. In our experimental design, the island back was provided if the questions were answered incorrectly. structureisinvariantacrossallitems,anditisalsoknowntobe Comprehensionquestionsneveraddressedthecriticalfiller-gap robustacrossindividuals,regardlessofworkingmemorycapacity portion of the sentence. At the beginning of the experiment, (see Sprouse et al., 2012). On the other hand, the verbs dif- participants were instructed to read at a natural pace and to fered across items, and it is possible that the subcategorization answer the questions as accurately as possible. Seven practice bias differs across participants. This mixed effects model con- itemsprecededtheself-pacedreadingexperiment,andtheorder verged for all regions. We computed p values for linear mixed ofpresentationwasrandomizedforeachparticipant.Theexper- effectsmodelsusingthe lmerTestRpackage(Kuznetsova etal., imenttook∼30min.Theexperimentprotocolforthisstudywas 2014). approvedbytheInstitutionalReviewBoardattheUniversityof Maryland. Results Comprehensionaccuracy DataAnalysis The mean comprehension question accuracy for experimental Thedatafromtwoitemswereexcludedfromanalysesduetocod- itemsacrossparticipantsanditemswas93.0%.Forthenon-island ingerrors.Onlytrialsinwhichthecomprehensionquestionwas conditions,thetransitiveitemswereansweredwithanaccuracy answeredaccuratelywereincludedintheanalysis,whichaffected of93.7%(SE=1.9),andtheintransitiveitemswithanaccuracy 5.7%ofthetrials.Wealsoanalyzedthedatawithoutexcludingthe of94.6%(SE=1.4).Fortheislandconditions,thetransitiveitems trialsbasedoncomprehensionaccuracy,buttheoverallpatternof were answered with an accuracy of 91.5% (SE = 1.7), and the resultsdidnotchange. intransitiveitemswithanaccuracyof92.0%(SE=2.2).Themean Self-pacedreadingtimesforthetargetsentenceswereexam- accuracy did not differ reliably across conditions, although the ined for each successive region, although the words after the factthatthemeanaccuracyforislandconditionswasnumerically auxiliary waswere combined into a single region because these lowermayreflectthe complexity difference betweennon-island laybeyondthecriticalregionsandwereunlikelytoshoweffects andislandconditions. relevantforthecriticalmanipulation.Thecriticalregionswherea potentialplausibilityortransitivitymismatcheffectwasexpected Readingtimedata consist of Region 7 (i.e., the verb wrote/chatted) and the fol- Theregion-by-region meanreadingtimeforthe transitivecon- lowing Region 8 (i.e., the adverb regularly), in which spill-over ditionsispresentedinFigure1,andthemeanregion-by-region effects could be observed. Regions 1 through 6 were predicted reading time for the intransitive conditions is presented in to show no difference across conditions, since they were lexi- Figure2. cally matched. Regions 9 through 11 could reveal reading time Inthenon-criticalRegions1–6,therewerenosignificantdif- differencesafterthefiller-gapdependencyiscompleted(Region ferencesinRegions1,2,4–6(ps>0.06).InRegion3therewasa 9 hosts the true gap site), and with a possible additional dif- maineffectofverbtype(Estimate=−17.3,SE=7.6,t=−2.27, ference in the island conditions due to the structural com- p<0.05),duetoslowerreadingtimesinthetransitiveconditions plexity associated with the extra relative clause in these condi- than in the intransitive conditions (381 vs. 358 ms). Since this tions. regionwaslexicallymatchedacrossconditions,weconcludethat Reading time data that exceeded three standard deviations this is a spurious effect. Butgiventhat the effect wassmalland fromthegroupmeanateachregionandineachconditionwere occurredwellaheadofthecriticalregions,thisunexpectedeffect excluded,affecting1.7%ofthedata.Theremainingreadingtime was unlikely to have impacted the observations in the critical data were analyzed using linear mixed effects models (Baayen regions. et al., 2008). These analyses were conducted in the R environ- At the critical verb in Region 7 there were no signifi- ment(RDevelopmentCoreTeam,2011),usingthelme4package cant differences (ps > 0.1). The following spill-over region FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 6 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling Discussion In Experiment 1, we tested the predictions of two hypotheses about active object gap creation. The hyper-active gap filling hypothesispredictedthepresenceofreadingdisruptionatintran- sitiveverbs,becauseencounteringanintransitiveverbinafiller- gap context would be incompatible with the object gap struc- ture constructed earlier. On the other hand, the conservative activegapfillinghypothesispredictednosuchreadingdisruption, because the parser should first consult the transitivity informa- tionoftheverbtodecidewhethertopositanobjectgapornot.As abaselineforestimatingthedegreeofdisruptionattheverb,we usedrelativeclauseislandconstructions,whichblocktheassocia- tionofthefillerwiththecriticalverb.Theresultswereconsistent with the predictions of the hyper-active account: in the region followingtheverb,weobservedslowerreadingtimesforintran- sitiveverbsinnon-islandconditionsthanincorrespondingisland conditions. Previous work has shown a filler-gap plausibility mismatch FIGURE1|Meanreadingtime(ms)forthetransitivenon-islandand effect at the verb such that mismatched transitive verbs in a islandconditions.Errorbarsindicatestandarderrorofthemean. non-island environment elicit longer reading times than their plausiblenon-islandorplausible/implausibleislandcounterparts (Traxler and Pickering, 1996; Omaki and Schulz, 2011), and herewereplicatedthisfinding.Thiseffectcanbeinterpretedas the result of active association of the filler with the transitive verb, which in these stimuli resulted in a verb–object plausi- bility mismatch. On the other hand,the slowdown observed in the intransitive non-island condition relative to the intransitive island condition can be interpreted as a transitivity mismatch. This suggests that the parser does not wait for bottom–up evi- dence from the verb that the verb can syntactically license a gap,butratherattemptstoconstructthedependencybeforethis information is available. This slowdown cannot reflect the cost of maintaining the filler in working memory, because a filler is also being maintained at this position in the baseline island condition. Itisalsoimportanttonotethattheshorterreadingtimesinthe criticalregionsoftheislandconditionsaretheoreticallyinforma- tive.Thesefindingssuggestthatthereadingtimeincreaseinthe FIGURE2|Meanreadingtime(ms)fortheintransitivenon-islandand non-island conditions is specifically due to an expectation vio- islandconditions.Errorbarsindicatestandarderrorofthemean. lation following premature gapcreation. Aplausible alternative explanation of the reading disruption in the non-island condi- tionsisthatitreflectsamoregeneralcostassociatedwithdelaying (Region 8) revealed no main effect of verb type, but there gapcreationdecisions.Underthisalternativeaccount,weshould was a main effect of structure type (Estimate = −92.0, expecttoobservereadingdisruptionintheislandconditionsas SE = 16.4, t = −5.61, p < 0.001), reflecting the fact that well,because gapcreation mustwaituntil the verbthat follows the non-island conditions produced significantly slower read- therelativeclauseislandregion(e.g.,sawinRegion9).However, ing times than the island conditions (529 vs. 435 ms). There thispredictionisnotsupportedbythedata,asthereadingtimein was no significant interaction of verb type and structure type theadverbregion(Region8)oftheislandconditionswasreliably (p>0.1). shorterthaninnon-islandconditions. Region9 consisted of asecond verbinthe island conditions In Regions 9 and 10, the island conditions were read more and a preposition in the non-island conditions. We observed slowlyfor bothlevelsofverbtype.Region9corresponds tothe a main effect of structure type in Region 9 (Estimate = 63.7, word that licensed the true gap site across all conditions, and SE = 15.9, t = 4.01, p < 0.001), as well as in Region 10 hence this slowdown could reflect a difference in the so-called (Estimate =46.1,SE=11.5,t =4.0,p<0.001),inthesecases integration cost (Gibson, 1998, 2000) between non-island and due to slower readingtimesin the island conditions (Region 9: island conditions. Previouswork on filler-gap dependency pro- 519vs.451ms,Region10:451vs.406ms).Region11revealedno cessing has demonstrated that increased complexity and length significantdifferences(ps>0.09). differences result in increased processing difficulties at the gap FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 7 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling site, as measured by reading time (Gibson and Warren, 2004; rather to a word category expectation mismatch in the adverb Wagers and Phillips, 2014) and reduced accuracy in speeded regionthatwastriggeredbytheverbitself.Thisaccountiscon- acceptabilityjudgmenttasks(McElreeetal.,2003).However,the sistent with the conservative active gap filling hypothesis, since readingtimedifferenceinRegion9maysimplybeduetolexical theparser’sexpectationregardingfiller-gapdependencycomple- differences(prepositionsinthenon-islandconditionsvs.verbsin tionisbasedontheinformationfromtheverb.Incidentally,the theislandconditions), sothereadingtimecontrastbetweenthe readingdisruptionobservedinthetransitiveconditionsofStaub islandandnon-island conditions maynotreflectanintegration (2007) wasatthe verbregion. One possible reason for this dis- costdifference. crepancyisthedifferenceinthedependentmeasure:Staub(2007) Notethatitisunlikelythatthereadingtimecontrastbetween usedaneye-trackingduringreadingmethodwhileweusedself- non-island and island conditions in Region 8 is related to the pacedreadinginExperiment1.Aneye-trackingduringreading overallcomplexityoftheconstructionsusedinourstimuli,given method generally provides better temporal precision than the that on all accounts that we are aware of, island domains have self-pacedreadingmethod(Rayner,1998;RaynerandPollatsek, beenarguedtobesyntacticallymorecomplexandmoretaxingfor 2006). Thus, an eye-tracking replication of Experiment 1 may working memory resources (Deane, 1991; Kluender and Kutas, yieldatransitivitymismatcheffectontheverbregion,andpro- 1993;Kluender,1998,2004;HofmeisterandSag,2010).Thefact videstrongerevidenceforthehyper-activegapfillinghypothesis. thattheputativelylesscomplexnon-islandconditionswereread ThisisaddressedinExperiment2. moreslowlyallowsustoattributetheslowdowntoprocessesthat uniquely occur in the non-island conditions, namely filler-verb Experiment 2 association. In summary, the presence of both a plausibility mismatch Experiment 2 addressedtwo methodological concerns raised in effect and a transitivity mismatch effect lends support to the Experiment 1 by removing sources of slowdown in the transi- hyper-active gap filling hypothesis, and argues against a con- tiveconditions,andalsobyusingtheeye-trackingduringreading servative active gap filling hypothesis under which transitivity method. information is consulted before attempting to create an object gap.ThisfindingdirectlycontrastswiththatofStaub(2007),who Method didnotfindevidenceforatransitivitymismatcheffect. Participants However,thisconclusionisnotwarranteduntiltwomethod- We recruited 33 native speakers of American English from the ologicalconcernsareaddressed.First,thedesigninExperiment1 Johns Hopkins University community, but data from one par- wasmodeledafterStaub(2007),whousedaplausibilitymismatch ticipant were removed due to calibration errors. Participants design for transitive verb conditions, and transitivity mismatch received course credit or $10 for their participation. They were design for intransitive verb conditions. Our findings differed allnaïvetothepurposeoftheexperiment. fromStaub’saswefoundmismatcheffectsforbothtransitiveand intransitive non-island conditions, but it is possible that some Materials nuisance factor common to both non-island conditions led to Weused28setsoffoursentencesasshowninTable2.Thisexper- aslow-downacrosstheboard.Strongerevidenceforthehyper- imentusedthesametransitivitymismatchlogicasExperiment1 active gap filling hypothesis can be obtained if we replicate the andmanipulatedtheverbtransitivitytype(intransitivevs.tran- transitivity mismatch slowdown in the intransitive non-island sitive). However, in this experiment the semantic fit between condition, while at the same time observing no reading dis- the filler and the transitive verb wasalways plausible,such that ruption in the transitive non-island condition. Experiment 2 noreadingdisruption wasexpected atthetransitive verbinthe accomplishedthisbymakingthefillerandtheverbsemantically non-islandcondition.AsinExperiment1wemanipulatedstruc- fitinthetransitiveconditions.Theabsenceofreadingdisruption ture type (non-island vs. island), using conditions with relative inthetransitiveconditionswouldsuggestthatthedisruptionin clause island structures as baseline conditions. Relative clause thenon-island,intransitiveconditionisduetotheintransitivity islandsprovideaneffectivebaseline,sincetheyincludethesame oftheverb. fillerNPandotherlexicalmaterialasthenon-island condition, Second,itisimportanttonotethatourevidenceforreading whilepreventingdependencycompletionatthecriticalverb.As disruptionfortransitiveandintransitiveverbs(i.e.,theslowdown in Experiment 1, the transitive verbs were optionally transitive innon-islandconditionscomparedtoislandconditions)wasnot and the true gap position occurred outside the island domain, observeduntilthespill-overadverbregion.Spill-overeffectsare allowingthesentencetocontinuegrammatically. widelyobservedinself-pacedreadingexperiments,anditisthus The28sentencesetswerecounter-balancedacrossfourlistsso commontoattributespill-overeffectstoprocessestriggeredina thateachparticipantsawonlyoneversionofthetargetitemsand precedingregion.However,inourexperimentthereisanalterna- consequently read seven tokens of each condition. In addition, tiveexplanationfortheeffectintheadverbregionthatwouldnot 76fillersofsimilarlengthandcomplexitywereconstructedand requirehyper-activegapfilling.Fortheintransitivecondition,the addedtoeachlist. slowdownintheadverbregioncouldindicatethattheparserhad Procedure expectedthepresenceofapreposition,whichwouldallowstruc- turalintegrationofthefiller.Underthisalternativeaccount,the An Eyelink 1000 eye-tracker (SR Research: Mississauga, ON, slowdownisnotduetoatransitivitymismatchontheverb,but Canada) was used to record eye movements. The participant’s FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 8 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling TABLE2|SamplematerialsandconditionsforExperiment2. Analysisregions Sentenceinitial Pre-verb Verb Post-verb Sentencefinal Transitive,non-island Thebookthat theauthor wrote regularly aboutwasnamedforanexplorer Transitive,island Thebookthat theauthorwho wrote regularly sawwasnamedforanexplorer Intransitive,non-island Thebookthat theauthor chatted regularly aboutwasnamedforanexplorer Intransitive,island Thebookthat theauthorwho chatted regularly sawwasnamedforanexplorer Examplequestion Wasthebooknamedforanexplorer? headwasstabilizedbyachinrestandaforeheadrest.Theposition author who in island conditions), in order to ensure that there oftherighteyeonlywasmonitoredatasamplingrateof1000Hz. werenounexpectedreadingbehaviordifferencesthatmightcom- The eye-tracker display allowed a maximum of 120 characters promise the interpretation of the data from the critical region, per line, in 10 pt Monaco font. Some filler sentences were dis- (b) the verb region, which is the critical region where poten- playedontwolines,butalltargetsentencesweredisplayedonone tialtransitivity mismatcheffectsmight beobserved,and(c) the line.Stimuliweredisplayedona21.5-inchSamsungSyncMaster post-verb region, which corresponds to the post-verbal adverb monitor,andparticipantswereseated65cmfromthecomputer and could be used to probe for potential spill-over effects. The screen.Beforetheexperimentstarted,participantswereseatedin data in the remaining regions are not reported, because read- front oftheeye-trackerandreceivedinstructions forthe exper- ing timesat these regions are not critical for distinguishing the iment.Acalibration routine wasperformed atthe beginning of competinghypotheses.Moreover,afterthepost-verbregion,the theexperiment,andtheexperimentermonitoredthecalibration lexicalitemswerenotheldconstantacrossconditionsandthere- accuracy throughout the session, recalibrating when necessary. foreanyobserveddifferenceswouldbedifficulttointerpret.The The experiment started with written instruction on the display islandconditionscontainedoneextraword,i.e.,therelativepro- and sevenpractice trials. Atthe beginning of eachtrial,a black noun(e.g.,who),whichcouldhaveaffectedreadingtimesinthe circlewasdisplayedontheleftsideofthemonitor,whichcorre- pre-verb region as well as regression measures for subsequent spondedtothelocationofthebeginningofthesentence.Thetext regions. wasdisplayedaftertheparticipantsuccessfullyfixatedonthecir- FollowingthedataanalysisproceduresusedinStaub(2007), cle.Afterreadingeachsentence,theparticipantpressedabutton fourreadingtimemeasureswerecomputedforthethreeregions to remove the sentence display. Each sentence was followed by ofinterests:firstfixationduration,firstpasstime,regressionpath ayes-nocomprehensionquestion,andtheparticipantanswered time,andpercentregressions(Rayner,1998;RaynerandPollatsek, the comprehension question by pressing a left or right button. 2006;StaubandRayner,2007).Firstfixationdurationisthedura- Comprehensionquestionsneveraddressedthecritical filler-gap tion of the very first fixation in a region, regardless of whether portion ofthe sentence.Theentire experimentlasted∼35min. thereisasinglewordormultiplewordsinthatregion.Thismea- The experiment protocol for this study was approved by the sure is often used as an index of lexical difficulty (e.g., Reichle Homewood Institutional Review Board at the Johns Hopkins et al., 2003) but is also informative about the earliest syntactic University. processesthatimmediatelyfollowlexicalaccess(e.g.,Frazierand Rayner,1982;Sturt,2003). DataAnalysis Thefirst-passreadingtimeiscalculatedbysummingthefixa- Comprehensionaccuracyforthetargettrialswas90.7%,andtri- tionsinaregionbetweenthetimewhentheeye-gazefirstenters alsinwhichparticipants answeredthecomprehensionquestion the region from the left and the time when the eye-gaze exits incorrectly were removed from the eye movement analyses, as theregioneithertotheleftortheright.First-passreadingtimes data from these trials may reflect inattentive reading. For the alsoindexearlylexicalandsyntacticprocessesassociatedwitha remaining data, an automatic procedure pooled short contigu- region, but given that they consist of multiple fixations on the ous fixations. The procedure incorporated fixations of lessthan sameregion,theymayalsoreflectslightlylaterprocessesthanthe 80msintolargerfixationswhentheyoccurredwithinonecharac- firstfixationmeasure. terofeachotheranddeletedanyremainingfixationsoflessthan Regression path times are the sum of fixations from the time 80 ms, because little information can be extracted during such whenthe eye-gazefirstentersaregionfrom the lefttothetime shortfixations(RaynerandPollatsek,1989).Unusuallylongfixa- whentheeye-gazeexitstheregiontotheright.Regressionpath tionsgreaterthan800mswerealsoremoved,becausetheyusually time is identical to first-pass reading time if the eye-gaze first reflecttrackerlossesorotheranomalousevents.Thisprocedure exitsthe region tothe right, butifthe eye-gazeexitsthe region resultedintheexclusionof4.86%ofallfixations. to the left, then regression path timesare longer than the first- Forthepurposeofanalysisoftheeyemovementdata,thesen- passtimeastheyincludeallfixationsinpreviousregionsaswell tencesweredividedintofiveanalysisregions,asshowninTable2. asre-fixationsontheregionbeforeexitingtheregiontotheright. We report eye movement data in the following three regions: Thus,regressionpathtimesarelikelytoreflectslightlylaterpro- (a)thepre-verbregion(theauthorinnon-islandconditions,the cesses,suchasintegrationofthecriticalregionwiththepreceding FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 9 April2015|Volume6|Article384 Omakietal. Hyper-activegapfilling context. The percent regressions indicate the probability that a In the pre-verb region, the first pass time and regression readermadearegressiveeyemovementtoprecedingregionsafter path measures showed a main effect of structure (p < 0.001), fixating a given region. This measure includes only regressions with longer reading times in the island conditions than in the madeduringthereader’sfirstpassthroughtheregion,anddoes non-islandconditions.Thiseffectwasexpectedbecausethepre- notincluderegressionmadeafterre-fixatingtheregion. verb region in the island conditions contained the extra word Readingtimedata(i.e.,firstfixation,firstpass,andregression who, which made it more likely to attract multiple fixations in pathdurations)wereanalyzedusinglinearmixedeffectsmodels that region. No other significant effects were observed in this (Baayen et al., 2008), and percent regressions were analyzed by region. mixed effects logistic regression, asthe dependentmeasurewas In the verb region, evidence for the hyper-active gap filling categorical(seeJaeger,2008).Themixedeffectsmodelsincluded hypothesiswasfoundinfirstfixationdurationsaswellasinfirst randominterceptsforparticipantsanditems.Weusedthesame passmeasures.Bothmeasuresshowedamaineffectofstructure procedure asExperiment 1 tosimplify the random slope struc- withlongerreadingtimesfornon-islandconditions(ps<0.05). tureuntilthemodelsconvergedinallregionsandeyemovement First fixation durations showed a marginal interaction of struc- measures. This procedure led us to adopt verb transitivity as a tureandverbtransitivity(p=0.06),andfirstpasstimesshowed randomslopeforparticipantsanditemsforallfixationmeasures a significant interaction (p < 0.05). Planned pairwise compar- andregions,exceptforpercentregressionmeasuresinthepost- isons on first fixation durations and first pass times revealed verbregion.Here,weremovedtheverbtransitivityrandomslope thatreadingtimesinthenon-island,intransitiveconditionwere for participants, asthe transitivity biasvariance acrossdifferent significantly longer than in the island, intransitive condition verbs(ifany)ismorelikelytoinfluencethedatathanvariancein (ps<0.01),butnosignificant difference wasobservedbetween participants’experiencewiththeverbs. the transitive conditions. No significant effect wasobserved for When the critical region demonstrated a significant inter- theregressionpathdurations.Therewasamaineffectofstruc- action of verb and structure type, a planned comparison was tureinpercentregressions(p<0.05),withahigherpercentage conducted with separate mixed effects models to test for sys- ofregressioninthe islandconditions, whichlikelyreflected the tematicdifferencesbetweentheislandandnon-islandconditions greaterstructuralcomplexityintheislandconditions. within each verb type. These models included participants and In the post-verb region, there was a marginally significant itemsasrandomintercepts. interaction of verb and structure type (p = 0.066), but no sig- nificanteffectwasobservedinothereye-movementmeasures. Results Discussion Table3presentstheparticipantmeansoneachmeasureforeach regionaswellasthestandarderrorsoftheparticipantmeans,and Experiment 2 used an eye-tracking during reading method to Table4presentsasummaryofthestatisticalanalyses. investigate whether the parser uses verb transitivity informa- tion in deciding whether to postulate a gap at the verb object position. Firstfixationdurationsandfirstpasstimesforintran- sitiveverbsweresignificantly longerinastructure thatallowsa TABLE3|Experiment2participantmeanreadingtimesinmilliseconds (standarderror)andpercentregressions. gap (non-island condition) than when the same verb appeared in an island configuration. This effect was not observed when Measure Pre-verbregion Verbregion Post-verbregion the critical verb was transitive. The fact that there was a read- ingdisruption for intransitive verbsbutnot for transitive verbs Firstfixation is consistent with the prediction of the hyper-active gap filling Transitive,non-island 212(8) 249(12) 242(9) hypothesis. If the parser creates an object gap and integrates Transitive,island 217(7) 240(7) 243(9) the filler into the object position before having access to verb Intransitive,non-island 207(8) 256(10) 246(10) transitivity information, reading disruption in the non-island Intransitive,island 208(5) 231(8) 237(7) First-passtime intransitiveconditionshouldresultfromthemismatchbetween Transitive,non-island 287(14) 277(13) 283(13) thepredictedtransitivityandactualtransitivityoftheverb. Transitive,island 386(20) 275(10) 287(12) It is also important to note that in this experiment the crit- Intransitive,non-island 299(15) 303(13) 296(14) ical mismatch effects were observed in the verb region, unlike Intransitive,island 396(19) 266(11) 284(10) inExperiment1wherethemismatcheffectswereobservedonly Regressionpathtime in the spill-over adverb region. This constitutes stronger evi- Transitive,non-island 463(28) 373(24) 402(30) dence for hyper-active gap filling, because the mismatch effect Transitive,island 636(43) 406(31) 447(35) musthaveresultedfrom properties ofthe verbitself.Theques- Intransitive,non-island 472(38) 397(23) 492(35) tionofwhythe critical effectswereobservedinthe verbregion Intransitive,island 619(41) 425(38) 469(26) in Experiment 2 (unlike in Experiment 1, where the effect was Percentregressions found in the spill-over region) likely reflects task-based differ- Transitive,non-island 33.1(5.0) 17.1(3.5) 17.9(3.4) ences whose effects are seen well beyond the current studies. Transitive,island 33.2(4.0) 23.0(4.4) 24.4(3.4) Inhibition of the button pressing action in self-paced reading Intransitive,non-island 27.1(4.8) 16.2(2.8) 27.5(3.7) tasks is likely more difficult than inhibition of saccades in an Intransitive,island 32.7(4.7) 24.4(3.7) 22.4(3.1) eye-trackingtask. FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 10 April2015|Volume6|Article384

Description:
Akira Omaki1*, Ellen F. Lau2, Imogen Davidson White2, Myles L. Dakan2, Aaron Apple1 and Colin where internal arguments are fronted and hence precede the verb. cessing demands of verb-finality, or rather a property of a general noun phrase (NP) the city (called the filler) is dislocated from the.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.