MISCELLANEOUS NOTES Table 1: Thedietand its preference bythe Nicobar significant variation in precipitation (Kruskal Wallis test, Megapode H=7.81, df=2, p=0.02).The datareportedhereagreedwith other reports for the genus Megapodius: that they are Fooditem Rankbasedon Rankbasedon omnivorous(Jones etal. 1995). Volumetric frequency proportion of occurrence This study is funded by the Ministry ofEnvironment andForests, GovernmentofIndiaandthelogistic supportby Macarangapeltata 1 1 the Forest Department, Andaman and Nicobar Islands, has Dictyoptera (cockroaches) 2 2 been invaluable in the execution of this study. We thank Snails 2 3 V.S.Vijayan,LalithaVijayan,ReneDekker,DarylJones,Yash Litters 3 2 Beetles 4 4 VeerBhatnagar,JuguluMehtoandPremRamfortheirvaluable Ants 4 4 inputs in this study. Unidentified 4 3 Grasshopper 4 5 December12,2002 K. SIVAKUMAR1 Pauropodans 5 5 SANKARAN R. Hermitcrab 5 5 Reptilescale 6 6 SalimAliCentreforOrnithology andNatural History, Like other megapodes, the Nicobar Megapode also Anaikatty,Coimbatore640108, forages by scratching and raking the debris on the ground. TamilNadu, India More precipitation reduced the foraging activities of the 'PresentAddress: Wildlife Institute ofIndia, = Nicobar Megapode (Spearman correlation test: r -0.46, P.O. Box. 18,Chandrabani, p= 0.05). Foraging patterns varied significantly between the DehraDun24800 Uttaranchal, India. 1, years (Kruskal Wallis test: H= 9.23, df= 2, p<0.01), due to E-mail:[email protected] REFERENCES Ali, S. & S.D. Ripley (1983): Handbook ofthe Birds of India and ofAustralian birds. Emu 12: 8-18. Pakistan. Compact edition. Oxford University Press, Bombay. Coles,C.(1937): SomeobservationsonthehabitsoftheBrushTurkey 737 pp. (Alecturalathami).ProceedingsoftheZoologicalSocietyofLondon Altmann, J. (1974): Observational study of behaviour: sampling 107(A): 261-73. methods. Behaviour 48: 227-265. Gill,H.B.(1970): BirdsofInnisfailandhinterland.Emu 70: 105-116. Booth, D. T. (1986): Crop and gizzard contents oftwo Mallee fowl. Hess,H.C.(1997):Stomachflushing:samplingthedietofRed-cockaded Emu 86: 51-53. Woodpecker. Wilson. Bull. 109(3): 535-539. Brookes,G.B. (1919): Reportoninvestigationsinregardtothespread Jones,D.N.,R.W.R.J.Dekker&C.S.Roselaar(1995):TheMegapodes. ofprickly pear by the Scrub Turkey. QueenslandAgricultural Oxford University Press. 262 pp. Journal II: 26-28. Kennedy, M & P. Fitzmaurize(1972): Someaspects ofthebiologyof Cleland,J.B. (1912): Examination ofcontentsofstomachs andcrops gudgeonGobiogobio(L.)inIrishwaters.J. Fish. Biol 4:425-440. 11. GRIT USE IN THE SARUS CRANE GRUSANTIGONE Introduction K.S.Gopi Sundar pers. obs.), no information exists on Grit is generally ingested by granivorous and the amount and kind of grit ingested by Sarus Cranes. In herbivorous birds, and to a smaller extent by insectivorous this paper, we present information on grit use by the Sarus birds (Weltry and Baptista 1988). The main purpose ofgrit Crane in India based on contents of three Sarus Crane intakebybirds isto facilitatethe mechanicalgrindingin the gizzards. gizzard ofany hard, coarse material ingested by the bird. However, gritmayalso providecalciumto femalebirdsjust METHODS beforethe breedingperiod(Harper 1964). Grit use patterns arefairlywellunderstoodforGalliformes(Dalke 1937;Sadler The gizzards ofthree SarusCranes(one eachofasub- 1961) and for ‘cornfield birds’ (Best and Gionfriddo 1991; adult, male and female bird), whichhad been collected after GionfriddoandBest 1996). Whileseveralobservationshave theirdeath dueto suspected pesticide poisoning(S. Sharma, beenmadetowardsingestionofgritparticlesbySarusCranes pers. comm.), were obtainedfromKeoladeo GhanaNational (J. Langenberg pers. comm., M. Nagendran pers. comm.. ParkinBharatpur,India.Theageofthesebirdswereunknown 106 J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc., 102 (1), Jan-Apr 2005 MISCELLANEOUS NOTES and could only be inferred from the development ofthe red a colouration onthehead(Ali and Ripley 1980). The gizzards werefoundtocontain largeamountsofvegetablematterand grit. The gizzards were sliced open, all the contents were flushed onto petri dishes, and the grit separated, washed and dried. Characterization ofgrit was done following Best and Gionffiddo(1991).Threemeasurementsusedtocharacterize the grit were: 1) Size - the average of the longest and the shortest dimensions of each particle was calculated by measuring the two dimensions with Vernier callipers to the nearest0.01 mm.2)Shape-gritshapeindexwascalculatedby dividingthelongestdimensionwiththeshortest.Theparticles representing a spherical shape would have an index of 1.0 Size classes (mm) and grit with values greaterthan unity were deemed oval to oblong, and 3) Roundness - gritroundness was measured by b classifying all particles in five categories, namely Angular, Sub-angular, Sub-rounded, Rounded and Well-rounded. Roundness index was calculated by giving grit particles a value of 1 forAngular, 2 for Sub-angular and so on; values tendingtowardsfivewouldmeangritwithcompletelysmooth surfaces. mm Grit particles with the longest dimension <1 were excluded from the analysis, assuming that they were accidentally ingested by the cranes, or were broken down fromlargerparticles. Veryfewparticles(<1%)wereexcluded fromthetotalsampleforthisreason.All gritparticlesofeach bird wereweighedtogethertothe nearest0.0 gm. 1 RESULTS Shape index classes c Atotalof523 gritparticleswerecountedandmeasured fromthegizzardsofthethreeSarusCranes.Thetotalweight, number ofgrit particles, mean grit size and mean grit shape areindicatedinTable 1.Thoughthemalebirdhadthehighest mean grit size, the two largest grit particles (> 10 mm) were found in the other two birds. The largest range ofgrit sizes was seen inthe sub-adultand female bird(Fig. la). Most grit mm particles(80.7%)inallthreegizzardswerebetween2-6 in size (Fig. lb). While the sub-adult and the female bird had mostgritparticles in the size range of2-4 mm, the male bird had mostgritparticles inthe4-6 mmrange(Fig. la). Mostof theparticles(69%)hadashapeindexof1.5-2.5 (Fig. 1b). The malebirdhadthe largestvarietyofshapes(9 classes)andthe Roundness categories female, the least(6 classes). All three birds used most ofthe gritparticles inthe shape index range of1.5-2 (Fig. lb). The Fig. 1: a. Percentcompositionofgritparticlesacrossdifferentsize mostrepresentedgritroundnesscategorieswereSub-angular classes; and Sub-rounded (77.6% ofall particles, Fig. lc). The least b. Percent composition of grit particles across different shapeindexclasses; represented roundness category was Well-rounded (n=l) in c. Percent composition of grit particles across different theentiresample.The largestproportionofAngularparticles roundnesscategories J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc., 102 (1), Jan-Apr 2005 107 , MISCELLANEOUS NOTES Table 1: Characteristicsofgritin Sarus Cranes (Table 1). Mostofthegritparticlesusedbythemalebirdwere alsoofahighersize classcomparedtothose intheothertwo Gizzards Numberof Total Mean Shape Mean birds(4-6mmasagainst2-4mm). Largersamplesizeswill be sampled grit weight size index Roundness required to adequately explore gender and age related particles (gm) (mm) differences in grit use patterns in Sarus Cranes. Sub-adult 108 13.46 3.92 2.01 245 From this study, itappearsthat Sarus Cranes generally mm (1.1-10.41) (1.18-4.55) use grit particles between 2-6 in size and opt for grit Adultmale 220 35.95 4.5 2.2 2.94 particles with intermediate degrees of roughness and (1.28-8 57) (1.11-5.62) roundness. Intheory, angularparticleswithsharpedgeshave Adultfemale 195 22.65 3.68 1.94 2.74 the greatest efficiency in digesting coarse food substances, (1.03-11.8) (1.17-3.89) but also pose the risk ofdamaging internal digestive organs. Rangeofmeansizeandshapeindexofgritisgiveninparentheses In contrast, well-rounded particles have the lowest risk with respecttophysical injury,butwillbe leastefficientinbreaking was seen in the sub-adult bird. All three birds used most of down food. In this respect, Sarus Cranes seem to be using thegritparticlesintheSub-roundedcategory(Fig. lc). Mean gritparticlestomaximizedigestionwhileavoidingexcessive roundness ofgrit particles was almost the same in all three physical damage to digestive organs. This data set is useful birds(Table 1). in that it provides information to aviculturists and zoo managersonthekindofgritthatshouldbeprovidedtocaptive DISCUSSION Sarus Cranes. While use ofgrit in birds is known to differwith body ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS size, gender, reproductive status and availability, diet is knowntobethemost importantfactorinfluencinguseofgrit WethankMrs. ShrutiSharma,Director,KeoladeoGhana (Gionfriddo and Best 1996). Increase in number of grit National Park for providingthe Sarus samples forthe study. particles isusually in bird species with adiet ofhard, coarse A. Tamim helped with the transport ofthe samples and S.P. material,particularlyseedsandvegetablematter(Gionfriddo Rajkumarassisted in the autopsy, storage ofgizzards, and in and Best 1996), and in species with a varied, unspecialized sexingthe birds. Wethankthe Director, Wildlife Institute of diet. The Sarus Crane (Grits antigone) in India is known to Indiaforfacilitiesand infrastructureprovidedundertheSarus beomnivorous, itsdiet includinggrainofseveral kind,plant CraneProject. M.S. Rana(Wildlife InstituteofIndia), andB. shoots, tubers of aquatic plants, frogs, lizards and other Didrickson(InternationalCraneFoundation)providedlibrary reptiles, grasshoppers and other insects, vegetable matter, support. KSGS thanks J. Langenberg, D. Mudappa, M. fruits, molluscs (Hume and Marshall 1879, Baker 1929, Nagendran, T.R.S. Raman and M. Spalding for discussions Ghorpade 1975), fish (Law 1930) and occasionally eggs of and comments on a previous draft. other birds (Sundar 2000). The omnivorous habit of the species most likely facilitates the intake of such large December30,2002 K.S.GOPI SUNDAR1 quantities ofgrit. Many ofthe foods reported for the Sarus B.C. CHOUDHURY are calcium-rich, and it is unlikely that grit is ingested to WildlifeInstituteofIndia, supplement calcium, but primarily fulfils a mechanical P.O. Box 18,Chandrabani, function. DehraDun248001, Uttaranchal, India. Sarus Crane males are larger than females, and sub- adultsareconsiderablysmallerthanadultbirds(AliandRipley 'PresentAddress: C/o Wildlife SocietyofIndia, 1980).Thismayexplainthedifferenceinthehighernumberof M-52,GreaterKailashPart-1 grit particles and the correspondingweight ofthe gizzard of NewDelhi 110048,India the male bird, andthe smallervalues forthe othertwo birds Email: [email protected] Ali, S. & S.D. Ripley (1980): Handbook ofthe Birds of India and wadersandothersemi-sportingbirds. PartVII.J. BombayNat. Pakistan. Vol 2: Megapodesto Crab Plover. OxfordUniversity Hist. Soc. 33: 4. Press, Delhi. Best, L.B. & J.P. Gionfriddo (1991): Characterization ofgrit use by Baker,E.C.S.(1929):ThegamebirdsoftheIndianEmpireVol.V.The cornfieldbirds. WilsonBull. 795:68-82. 108 J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc., 102 (1), Jan-Apr 2005 MISCELLANEOUS NOTES Dalke, P.L. (1937): Food habitsofadultpheasants in Michiganbased Harper, J.A. (1964): Calcium in grit consumed by hen pheasants in on crop analysis method. Ecology 18: 199-213. east-central Illinois. J. Wild/. Manage. 28: 264-270. Ghorpade,A.M. (1975):Noteson thefeedingoftheSarusCraneGrus Law, S.C. (1930): Fish eating habit of the Sarus Crane (Antigone antigone antigone (Linnaeus). J Bombay Nat. Hist Soc. 72: antigone). J. BombayNat. Hist. Soc. 34: 582-583. 199-200. Sadler, K. (1961): Gritselectivity by the female pheasant duringegg Gionfriddo, J.P. & L.B. Best (1996): Grit-use patterns in North production.J Wildl. Manage. 25: 339-341. American birds: the influence ofdiet, body size, and gender. Sundar, K.S.G (2000): Eggs in the diet ofthe Sarus Crane (Grits Wilson Bull. 108: 685-696. antigone). J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc. 97: 428-429. Hume, A.O. & C.H.T. Marshall (1879): The Game Birds of India, Weltry,J.C.& L. Baptista(1988):TheLifeofBirds.SaundersCollege Burma & Ceylon. Vol III. John Bale, London. Pp. 4-5. Publishers,N.Y. 12. OBSERVATIONS OF MATE CHANGE AND OTHER ASPECTS OF PAIR-BOND IN THE SARUS CRANE GRUS ANTIGONE Sarus Cranes Grus antigone are known to be and the frequency of duet calls (which is also used for monogamous and pair bond is thought to last throughout the advertisement during territorial defence, Archibald 1976) livesofthebirds(AliandRipley 1989).Themaleandfemaleof increased. Inmid-August,thepairconstructedanestofTypha apairare known to strengthen the pair bond by synchronized reeds in the marshland and the female laid two eggs, both of behaviours such as duet calls, dance, guard-calls and alarm- which were removed byvillagers. calls(Masatomi 1994;Archibald 1976).Theactualdurationof OnSeptember8,2000,duringaroutinevisittothearea, pair bond, prevalence ofmate change and factors leadingto another bird was seen in the territory and repeated attempts such changes are unknown in Sarus Cranes. In this note, I by the resident pair to chase the visitor (duet calls, tertial- report the first evidence of divorce or separation in Sarus elevation strutsandco-ordinatedguardcalls)failedtoelicita Cranes with observations on other aspects ofpair bond and response. In a surprising move, the visitor attacked the territoriality. residentfemale(identifiedbysmallersizeandpostureduring Overtwohundred and fiftybreeding,territorialpairsof duet calls). At first, the male assumed threat postures and Sarus Cranes were monitored between December 1999-July carried out displacement preens, tertial-elevation struts, and 2002 as part ofa study on their biology and conservation in a directed walk threat at the visitor. These attempts failed to the districts ofEtawah and Mainpuri in Uttar Pradesh. The displace the visitor, which instead renewed attacks on the study area has the highest known density ofterritorial pairs residentfemale. Thevisitorwassmallerthantheresidentmale, and number ofSarus Cranes anywhere in the world. Pairs/ but largerthan the resident female. The resident female and familieswererecognizedbycolour-bandingthejuvenileseach thevisitorsparredforalmostfiveminutes,peckingeach other year, with individual physical marks on adult birds, or by and indulging in rapid, violent bouts ofjump-rakes. The location. Pairs, particularlythosewith young, wereobserved residentfemalewasclearly losingthebattle;the largersizeof one to seven times a week (average offour observations) to the visitor could have proved to be an advantage. At this collect data on breeding success, feeding habits, territory point, the resident male, which was otherwise circling the size, and activitybudgets. Mate change was observed in two fighting pair of cranes, walked in and pecked the resident instances and is discussed in this note. “Divorce” or change female. The resident female began running away from the ofpairmembership was actually observed in one pair, while visitor,givingalarm-calls, andthemaleresponded bycalling mate change could be deduced in another. In addition, other synchronously, but did not come to the rescue. The visitor relevant aspects of pair bonding, territoriality, and related became very aggressive and mounted a fresh attack on the behaviours observed are described and discussed. The resident female by kicking, jump-raking, wing-thrashing, behaviour terminology used throughout the text follows sittingonthe backofthe bird and peckingthe neck and head Ellisetal. (1998). hard and rapidly, finally chasing the bird into a pond and forcing it to swim to the opposite bank. The male was now Matechangeobservation 1 chasing the resident female as well, and clearly supporting Theterritory ofone pairwas very close to the town of the visitor, though he synchronouslyanswered thealarm and Etawah, and consisted ofa large Typha-bordered marshland guard-callsofthe resident female. and crop fields. The pair had successfully raised one chick On emerging from the lake, the resident female was during the breeding season in 1999-2000, and was seen to pursued once again by the visitor. This time, the female defend the territory from other Sarus pairs and groups deliberately, but cautiously, approached human observers throughouttheobservation period. In earlyAugust, the adult standing on the side ofthe marshland and stood as close as birdswereobservedtochasethesub-adultfromtheterritory, 5 m. Thisdissuadedthevisitorfromattackingfurtherand she J. Bombay Nat. Hist. Soc., 102 (1), Jan-Apr 2005 109