ebook img

Freud and Modern Psychology: Volume 1: The Emotional Basis of Mental Illness PDF

251 Pages·1981·26.709 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Freud and Modern Psychology: Volume 1: The Emotional Basis of Mental Illness

FREUD AND MODERN PSYCHOLOGY VOLUME 1: THE EMOTIONAL BASIS OF MENTAL ILLNESS EMOTIONS, PERSONALITY, AND PSYCHOTHERAPY Series Editors: Carroll E. Izard, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware and Jerome L. Singer Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut HUMAN EMOTIONS Carroll E. Izard THE PERSONAL EXPERIENCE OF TIME Bernard S. Gorman and Alden E. Wessman THE STREAM OF CONSCIOUSNESS: Scientific Investigation into the Flow of Human Experience Kenneth S. Pope and Jerome L. Singer, eds. THE POWER OF HUMAN IMAGINATION: New Methods in Psychotherapy Jerome L. Singer and Kenneth S. Pope, eds. EMOTIONS IN PERSONALITY AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY Carroll E. Izard, ed. FREUD AND MODERN PSYCHOLOGY, Volume I: The Emotional Basis of Mental Illness Helen Block Lewis FREUD AND MODERN PSYCHOLOGY, Volume 2: The Emotional Basis of Human Behavior Helen Block Lewis A Continuation Order Plan is available for this series. A continuation order will bring delivery of each new volume immediately upon publication. Volumes are billed only upon actual ship· ment. For further information please contact the publisher. FREUD AND MODERN PSYCHOLOGY VOLUME 1: THE EMOTIONAL BASIS OF MENTAL ILLNESS HELEN BLOCK LEWIS Yale University New Haven, Connecticut PLENUM PRESS· NEW YORK AND LONDON Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Lewis, Helen B The emotional basis of mental illness. (Her Freud and modern psychology; v. 1) (Emotions, personality, and psychotherapy) Bibliography: p. Includes index. 1. Psychology, Pathological. 2. Emotions. 3. Interpersonal relations. 4. Freud, Sigmund, 1856-1939.5. Psychoanalysis. I. Title. II. Series. RC454.L48 616.89'001'9 80-20937 ISBN-13: 978-1-4684-3814-7 e-1SBN-13: 978-1-4684-3812-3 001: 10.1007/978-1-4684-3812-3 © 1981 Plenum Press, New York Sol'tcover reprint of the hardcover 1s t edition 1981 A Division of Plenum Publishing Corporation 227 West 17th Street, New York, N.Y. 10011 All rights reserved No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanicaL photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the publisher This book is dedicated to my grandchildren EMMA LEWIS BERNDT DAVID ASHER DANGERFIELD LEWIS Preface The tension between Freud's clinical discoveries about the power of human emotions and the theoretical framework in which he embedded these discoveries has been most eloquently detailed by Freud himself. His agoniz ing reappraisal. in 1926, of the libido theory of anxiety is just one example. But, as is usually the case, theoretical difficulties point to gaps in existing knowledge. At the time when Freud made his fundamental discovery that hysterical symptoms (and dreams) were understandable as reflections of for bidden ("strangulated") affect, anthropology was essentially nonexistent as a science. The cultural nature of human beings (our species' unique adaptation to life) could only be adumbrated by Freud (for example, in the myth of Totem and Taboo). As a consequence, the primacy of human attachment emotions in the acculturation process could not be postulated as a theoretical base. What Freud adopted as his base of theorizing was the most forward looking materialist concept of his time: the Darwinian concept of individual instincts as the driving force in life. Freud assumed that the vicissitudes of in stincts determine the fate of "ideas" in consciousness. Freud's theoretical base thus impelled him to speculate about the origin and fate of ideas instead of about the origin and fate of human emotional connectedness. This book is a small step along the road which should ultimately bring Freud's discoveries into a modem theoretical framework in psychology. I ac tually began this journey of revisiting Freud's works some years ago when I first undertook a phenomenological study of shame and guilt (Lewis, 1971). Shame and guilt are two "instinct" inhibitors that are also themselves "in stinctive" and transcultural. My way into that study was much facilitated by the research on field dependence in which I had participated for many years. Field dependence is a cognitive style that catches the self not only in its vii viii PREFACE characteristic relationship to its surround but also to its own value system. The self is another construct that was stimulated by Freud's work, although Freud, unfortunately, did not make much use of it. While the role of the self is different in shame and guilt, both states are universal modes of maintain ing threatened affectional bonds. That the two sexes differ in their use of the two affective-cognitive states was-the theme of my book, Psychic War in Men and Women (Lewis, 1976). A theoretical stance that sees emotional connectedness as the essential ingredient of humanity is itself largely a product of Freud's influence. The task of freeing his discoveries from the metapsychology he so painstakingly erected around his observations has been implicit in the work of many revi sionists since Freud, as well as in the work of such contemporary psychoana lytic critics as Lacan (1968) and Schafer(1978). Many have felt, and I concur, that accomplishing this task should improve techniques of psychoanalytic psychotherapy insofar as these have been hampered by Freud's metapsychol ogy. For example, conceptualizing symptoms as products of by-passed shame creates a very different therapeutic task than conceptualizing them as products of a "narcissistic" or "borderline" personality. As the reader will see, my own view of the enterprise centers on the need for psychology to build a theory of human emotions combining both the individual arousal and the communicative aspects of emotional life which Freud so brilliantly de scribed. It is a pleasure to acknowledge the assistance of many people who have helped in this work. The first outline of the work was developed in a course of undergraduate lectures at Yale, and the questions and comments of my students were an invaluable stimulus to my own thought. Dr. Zenia Fliegel has read all and Drs. Joel Allison, Sandra Buechler, Stanley Rachman, and Jerome L. Singer have read parts of the manuscript of Volume 1, and offered useful advice. Professor Carroll Izard has been a most helpful and encourag ing editor. My thanks are due to the Behavioral Science Publications Fund of the Yale Department of Psychology for financial assistance in the prepara tion of the manuscript. Most of all, my husband, Naphtali Lewis, has, as always, been my most reliable support. May, 1980 Contents INTRODUCTION 1 Chapter 1. THE SECULARIZATION OF GUILT: Reinterpreting Christoph Haizmann's Story 17 Chapter 2. HYSTERIA: The Problem of Forbidden Sexual Longings 31 Chapter 3. PHOBIAS: The Problem of Anxiety 63 Chapter 4. OBSESSIONAL NEUROSIS: The Problem of Sadism 101 Chapter 5. PARANOIA: The Problem of Homosex~ality 135 Chapter 6. DEPRESSION: The Problem of Grief and Mourning 167 Chapter 7. PSYCHOANALYSIS AS THERAPY TODAY: The Problem of Abreacting Shame and Guilt 201 BIBLIOGRAPHY 229 INDEX 241 ix Introduction This book asks the question: What has become of Freud's major discoveries and hypotheses in the light of nearly a century of subsequent research7 Freud's impact on twentieth-century thinking is literally immeasurable. It ranges from the flowering of the modern psychological novel to anthropologists' hypotheses about child-rearing practices as predictors of the severity of initiation rites. Why, then, a book that seeks to assess the im measurable outcome of Freud's work7 The answer lies in a paradox. On the one hand, the influence of psychoanalysis on present-day psychiatry is waning. Fifteen years ago, in one leading medical school that I know, the majority of residents in psychiatry were also in psychoanalytic training, or planning to get it. Today scarcely five percent intend to become psychoanalysts. On the other hand, some Freudian interpretations of behavior are so taken for granted in every day life that they are no longer recognized as Freud's. When feminists, for ex ample, criticize Freud for being a sexist, they are making use of a Freudian concept. Sexism refers to an unconscious bias. In order to defend his male ego, Freud believed certain fantasies about the inferiority of women. Or, to take another example, when we say someone is "uptight" or "tightass" we are using a slang expression for Freud's description of the anal character. This paradox - that Freud's discoveries about mental illness and its treatment are .f alling into disuse while his corollary discoveries about normal behavior have become household wisdom - invites inquiry. Freud's first discovery was that mental illness can result from guilt or shame over sexual longings. What this discovery implied is that emotions and, by implication, the social life of human beings are powerful enough to banish reason. This latter notion had been a part of folk wisdom and has 1 2 INTRODUCTION been expressed in literature since ancient times. Sophocles' Oedipus Rex and Shakespeare's Hamlet spring to mind as familiar examples. Freud, however, was the first person to trace sequences from forbidden sexual longings to their clearly recognizable translations as neurotic symptoms. Freud's scien tific work thus joined folk wisdom to become part of the twentieth-century Zeitgeist. Although the power of emotional forces had always been respected by folk wisdom, the scientific study of emotions was neglected in Freud's time and continues to be neglected at the present. This neglect has resulted in a hostile, not to say scornful. attitude toward the scientific validity of Freud's ideas. The fate of his work has thus varied enormously: among lay people it has had a revolutionary impact on cultural values; among experimental psychologists and other "hard-headed" scientists Freud's work is often dismissed as fanciful. Freud's own attitudes toward the emotions were partly governed by his own scientific training and were consequently ambivalent (to use a Freudian term). And to a certain extent Freud's defensive efforts to keep his discoveries scientific, as he understood the canons of science, actually hindered progress which could have been made in psychoanalysis as a therapy. Freud himself described his discoveries on two levels, a clinical level and a theoretical level. On the clinical level. which means speaking in ordinary descriptive language, he directly implicated the emotions - rage, terror, shame, guilt, sexual longings - in the formation of neurotic symptoms. Specifically, he uncovered a kind of "psychic alchemy" by which forbidden sexual longings are transformed into such symptoms as a bodily pain, a terri fying obsession, a phobia, or a profound depression. By studying his own dreams, Freud came to realize that the same transformation system at work in neurosis is at work in each of us in our nightly hallucinations. To this transformation system he gave the name "primary process," meaning to im ply that unfulfilled (sexual) longings begin to operate in earliest childhood even before they can be expressed in language. "Primary process" is the special. ancient language of emotional experience. It operates also in sophisticated metaphors. From the study of the similarity between dreams and neurotic symptoms he concluded that each of us has a ready potential for the formation of neurotic symptoms; in other words, that the boundary between normalcy and neuroticism is slim. Especially since he was able to discern echoes of his own childhood experiences in his dreams, he deduced that forbidden sexual longings occurred all during the course of development from childhood on, and persisted in their childhood form. From this he con cluded that emotions are "indestructible." All these discoveries clearly had implications for the understanding both of mental illness and of normal human behavior.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.