ebook img

Existential-Phenomenological Perspectives in Psychology: Exploring the Breadth of Human Experience PDF

358 Pages·1989·12.418 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Existential-Phenomenological Perspectives in Psychology: Exploring the Breadth of Human Experience

Existential-Phenomenological . Perspectives in Psychology Exploring the Breadth of Human Experience With a Special Section on Transpersonal Psychology Existential-Phenomenological Perspectives in Psychology Exploring the Breadth of Human Experience With a Special Section on Transpersonal Psychology . Edited by Ronald S. Valle John F. Kennedy University Orinda, California and Steen Halling Seattle University Seattle, Washington With a Foreword by James F. T. Bugental PLENUM PRESS • NEW YORK AND LONDON Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Existential-phenomenological perspectives in psychology: exploring the breadth of human experience: with a special section on transpersonaI psychology I edited by Ronald S. Valle and Steen Halling; with a foreword by James F. T. Bugental. p. CID. Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. ISBN-13: 978-0-306-43044-2 e-ISBN-13: 978-1-4615-6989-3 001: 10.1007/978-1-4615-6989-3 1. Phenomenological psychology. 2. Existential psychology. 3. TranspersonaI psychology. I. Valle, Ronald S. II. Halling, Steen. BF204.5.E94 1989 88-39126 ISO.19'2-dcl9 CIP 1098765432 © 1989 Plenum Press, New York A Division of Plenum Publishing Corporation 233 Spring Street, New York, N.Y. 10013 All rights reserved No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the Publisher To Christin and the Only One -RV To my parents, Poul and Margit Halling -SH Contributors Marc Briod, Department of Philosophy, Oakland University, Rochester, Minnesota Emily Davies, P.O. Box 671264, Chugiah, Alaska Judy Dearborn NiII, Department of Journalism, Seattle University, Seattle, Washington Constance T. Fischer, Department of Psychology, Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania William F. Fischer, Department of Psychology, Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Robert Frager, Institute for Culture and Creation Spirituality, Holy Names College, Oakland, Cal- ifornia and Institute for Transpersonal Psychology, Menlo Park, California Amedeo Giorgi, Saybrook Institute, 1772 Vallejo Street, San Francisco, California Steen Halling, Department of Psychology, Seattle University, Seattle, Washington Bernd Jager, New School for Psychoanalysis, Santa Rosa, California Mark King, School of Social Work, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania Michael Leifer, Seattle Mental Health Institute, Seattle, Washington Ralph Metzner, California Institute of Integral Studies, 765 Ashbury Street, San Francisco, California Donald Moncrieff, 84 Glendale Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Donald Moss, Haight Clinic Psychological Services, 109 South Jackson Street, Spring Lake, Michigan Donald E. Polkinghorne, Graduate Department of Counseling! School Psychology, California State University, Fullerton, California Dianne Powers, Highline Evaluation and Treatment Center, Seattle, Washington Robert D. Romanyshyn, Department of Psychology, University of Dallas, Irving, Texas Jan O. Rowe, Department of Psychology, Seattle University, Seattle, Washington Susan Schneier, Graduate School for the Study of Human Consciousness, John F. Kennedy Univer sity, Orinda, California Ronald S. Valle, Graduate School for the Study of Human Consciousness, John F. Kennedy Univer sity, Orinda, California Jeanne van Bronkhorst, Pierce County Health Department, Tacoma, Washington Rolf von Eckartsberg, Department of Psychology, Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania vii viii CONTRIBUTORS Frederick J. Wertz, Department of Social Sciences, Fordham University, College at Lincoln Center, New York, New York Brian J. Whalen, University of Dallas, Rome, Italy Bryan Wittine, Graduate Program in Transpersonal Counseling Psychology, John F. Kennedy University, Orinda, California Foreword When I began to study psychology a half century ago, it was defined as "the study of behavior and experience." By the time I completed my doctorate, shortly after the end of World War II, the last two words were fading rapidly. In one of my first graduate classes, a course in statistics, the professor announced on the first day, "Whatever exists, exists in some number." We dutifully wrote that into our notes and did not pause to recognize that thereby all that makes life meaningful was being consigned to oblivion. This bland restructuring-perhaps more accurately, destruction-of the world was typical of its time, 1940. The influence of a narrow scientistic attitude was already spreading throughout the learned disciplines. In the next two decades it would invade and tyrannize the "social sciences," education, and even philosophy. To be sure, quantification is a powerful tool, selectively employed, but too often it has been made into an executioner's axe to deny actuality to all that does not yield to its procrustean demands. The objectivist view of psychology is very like the precolumbiam view of the world: It regards all that is not familiar as dangerous, mythical, or nonexistent. The whole western hemisphere of human life, the subjective, is thus feared, treated as illusory, or denied. (I use this term, subjective, to refer to the whole' 'internal" or personal realm of experiencing.) Such subjective or experiential processes as values, purpose, ideals, intention, dreams, fantasy, love, courage, and dread are reduced to epi phenomena by the radical objective view. (Strange that those who so vigilantly-and often ruthlessly-advance this myopic view seldom note their own subjective processes!) In this vein, it is worth remembering the words of Paul Tillich: " 'Man resists objectification, and ifman'sresistance is broken, man himself is broken" (1951, p. 98). Clearly, the very essence of being human is the capacity for subjectivity, for inner living, inner experiencing, and inner intending. In the 1950s, the effort to model psychology on nineteenth century physical science came increasingly to dominate academic and research centers. Yet in consulting rooms there was a less visible but growing counterforce: the movement of psychologists from preoccupation with testing into the practice of psychotherapy. Thus the experience component of our field was being kept alive. The cultural revolution of the 1960s and early 1970s brought further attention to emotional responses, fantasy, and consciousness (especially its altered states). These are, of course, subjective experiences, and so the way was prepared for the reentry of the exiled dimension of life. Today we are beginning to emerge from what is, in the long view, a relatively brief interregnum of radical positivism and its consort, psychological behaviorism (Bugental, 1975-1976). The emergence is certainly far from complete, and many academic citadels are still occupied by those who doggedly seek to produce a science in the model of an earlier century's notion of physics. Still the recognition that this is a truncated and distorting perspective is gaining ground steadily. It is astonishing to see a well-known objectivist researcher forced to attack caricatures of view points that value inner experiencing in an. effort to excuse the paucity of results from his or her own orientation-one that rejects all but overt behavior (Skinner, 1987). Still it is instructive to observe ix x FOREWORD how feeble a psychology becomes when it tries to deny human inner life. Behaviorists so readily slide into an objective-subjective dichotomy that smacks of the good-evil preachments of fundamentalist religionists! More can be learned about this change by comparing the contents of this book with an earlier, related volume. A clear evidence of the renewal of concern with internal processes was the 1978 publication of Existential-Phenomenological Alternativesfor Psychology (Valle & King). A decade has passed since then, and the title of the present volume is Existential-Phenomenological Perspec tives in Psychology. The difference between alternativesfor and perspectives in signals a significant evolution in our discipline. No longer is the existential-phenomenological perspective an outsider that may optionally be considered. It has become a recognized view in psychology-a development to which that earlier volume significantly contributed. Looking back again, we need to note that 30 years ago Rollo May's Existence burst on the psychotherapy scene with an impact that caused many to reexamine, and a number to open, their conceptual frameworks (no small achievement that!). For me and for others, May's words spoke to what we were daily experiencing in our consulting rooms but what seemed to be blind spots in the dominant literature of the psychoanalysts and behaviorists. (Rogerian thinking had been helpful but too incomplete and light-sided for the deeper, more tragic and destructive material into which our work took us.) The 20 years between Existence and the first of the two books I compared before saw the rise of many psychotherapeutic schools and systems. Though they shared little else, many of these neces sarily gave priority to the subjective realm. While psychoanalysis and behaviorism, essentially efforts to objectify human beings, largely dominated the field, this earlier book marshaled the evidence of the consulting room to support the appropriateness of the more subjective perspective. Change continues, of course, and so our conception ofthe subjective realm has grown as well. A significant portion of the 1978 volume was given to six existential therapists. An almost equal portion is now given to the developing field of transpersonal psychology-a topic not even listed earlier in either the table of contents or the index. Clearly, experience, in all its myriad fonns, is an important area of psychology again. What does it mean to be concerned with experience as contrasted with behavior? It means to attend to the person as a subject rather than an object. It means to recognize that this subject is indeed a source of what is actual and not solely the receptacle of contingency. It means to affinn that the human is a different order of phenomenon than any other. It means that the hope of an objective impersonal science is revealed as vain, partial, and self-defeating. Many years ago, Franz Kafka pointed out a primary difference between an object and a person-to understand why a stone rolls down a hill, we must look to see what force loosened it from its place at the top, but to see why a person climbs the hill, we must discover what that person seeks at the top. It is the contrast between causation and intention that distinguishes the subjective or experien tial realm. Because humans are physical objects in part, we are subject to gravity as are all objects. However, because we are not only objects, we can also make gravity subject to our intent. Thus although the law of gravity has not been repealed-we are able to use its force to send our kind to the moon, to send a probe through and beyond our solar system. The key to this more-than-objective nature of the human lies in our reflexive awareness. The sunflower is aware-but probably not conscious-as it follows its lord across the sky. The deer is aware as it evades the hunter, and it is conscious is some measure. But the human-when most truly human-is not only aware but aware of being aware. This throws wild cards into the deck and provides a powerful counterforce to contingency. Narrow scientistic psychology has had to deny the reality of reflexive awareness. It simply would ruin most research designs. Similarly, the human "organism" must usually be treated as though it is empty and inert until the experimenter "stimulates" it. The fact that a person is a process, always intending, always evolving, and continually observing and reinterpreting what is going on puts persons quite completely outside of the range of anything like adequate experimental controls. So the FOREWORD xi experimentalist prefers to pretend none of this is true, and in the laboratory reduces the human to an object. In the ecology of our world, human beings playa unique and important part: We are-so far as we know-the only creatures to be aware of being aware. This is the miraculous quality of our subjectivity. From this gift (which can often seem a curse) come our capacities to have intentions, to reinterpret experience, to bring into being newness, and to create/discover meaning. The universe is not meaningless, for we are part of the universe and we are the meaning creators. Subjectivity, inner experiencing, the essence of being human, demand that we take account of experience. Only in that neglected realm can we come to grips with the great issues of our lives. The neglect has resulted in our understanding of ourselves falling far behind our understanding of the physical world. We must mobilize all our potential if we are to prevent that discrepancy from destroying us all. It is to this task that this book contributes as it celebrates the subjective with skill, breadth of vision, and wisdom. REFERENCES Bugental, J. F. T. (1975-1976). Toward a subjective psychology: Tribute to Charlotte Buhler. Interpersonal Development, 6, 48-61. Skinner, B. F. (1987). Whatever happened to psychology as the science of behavior? American Psychologist. 42. 780-786. Tillich, P. (1951). Systematic theology (Vol. I). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Valle, R. S., & King, M. (Eds.). (1978). Existential-phenomenological alternatives/or psychology. New York: Oxford U ni versity Press. James F. T. Bugental Novato, California Preface If my heart could do my thinking And my head begin to feel, I would look upon the world anew And know what's truly real. -Van Morrison This volume is both a new book and a revised version of Existential-Phenomenological Alternatives for Psychology (Valle & King, editors), published in 1978 by Oxford University Press. Twelve of the 20 chapters contained herein are completely new, and the 8 chapters retained from the original have been updated and substantially revised. Plenum Press is now the publisher, a section on transpersonal psychology has been added, and one of the editors, Steen Halling, is new to this project. Even with these substantial changes, however, the book's basic format remains much the same, as does the impetus behind the book's development. Our aim is to provide a unified and accessible presentation of the existential-phenomenological approach to a wide range of topics in psychology that may be helpful to students as well as our colleagues. It is as true today as it was in 1978 that there are few, if any, psychology texts that provide a comprehensive overview of this approach at an introductory level (i.e., one that does not assume prior knowledge of existential phenomenology). In terms of its content, this book addresses many of the standard topics in psychology (e.g., research, perception, learning, development, personality, social psychology, psychopathology, and psycho therapy) from an existential-phenomenological perspective. In addition, there are chapters address ing issues central to human life (i.e., forgiveness, the passions, aesthetic consciousness, and spiritual awareness) that have received little if any systematic attention in mainstream psychology. Since 1978, the existential-phenomenological approach has gained much ground and has be come an increasingly significant and accepted force in most psychological circles. We have chosen the word perspectives (rather than alternatives) for this book's title to acknowledge this growth and reflect the nature of this change in our field. Signs of this development include the greater number of presentations on existential-phenomenology and related topics at the annual convention of the Ameri can Psychological Association; articles that present and discuss the existential-phenomenological approach in the APA 's principal journal, American Psychologist .. the existence of at least four journals in North America that focus on phenomenology (the Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, Methods, Phenomenology and Pedagogy, and the Review of Existential Psychiatry and Psychology); and the continued success of professional gatherings such as the International Human Science Re search Conference (held annually since 1982). In addition, discussions of existential phenomenology are more common (even if often less than adequate) in textbooks for courses in abnormal, social, developmental, and introductory psychology. Even with this increased visibility, however, misunderstandings of existential-phenomenolog ical psychology continue to abound. The discussions in textbooks are seldom written by scholars with xiii

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.