EXAMINING THE APPROPRIATENESS OF THE PPVT-III AS A MEASURE OF VOCABULARY DEVELOPMENT IN LINGUISTICALLY DIVERSE KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN by Amy Elizabeth Platt A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology Ontario Institute for Studies in Education University of Toronto © by Amy Elizabeth Platt 2010 EXAMINING THE APPROPRIATENESS OF THE PPVT-III AS A MEASURE OF VOCABULARY DEVELOPMENT IN LINGUISTICALLY DIVERSE KINDERGARTEN CHILDREN Doctor of Philosophy 2010 Amy Elizabeth Platt Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology University of Toronto Abstract In order to evaluate the appropriateness of a standardized vocabulary measure for linguistically diverse kindergarten children this study examined response patterns of young Canadian children using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test –Third Edition (PPVT-III, Dunn & Dunn, 1997). Questions have been asked about whether the PPVT-III is an appropriate measure to use with young English Language Learners (ELL) because items may be culturally or linguistically biased. Further, it is of both psychometric and educational relevance to understand measurement of English vocabulary acquisition among young English First Language (EL1) and English Language Learner (ELL) children. PPVT-III scores were compared in two language groups – 52 EL1 and 73 ELL 4- and 5-year-old children. There were significant differences between the two groups in overall standard scores, with EL1 students performing significantly better than ELL students. In contrast, demographic factors of grade, gender and maternal education were not related to PPVT-III standard scores. A Mantel-Haenszel Differential Item Functioning (DIF) analysis provided a detailed picture of how individual test items functioned psychometrically in the two language groups. This analysis showed limited indications of DIF in the first 168 items of the PPVT-III, suggesting minimal test bias on the PPVT-III. Further analyses examined teachability of items, that is, how easily words can be taught, and the semantic categories of items, that is, parts of speech such as verbs, nouns and adjectives. These ii further analyses showed that the mean proportion of correct scores for all types of words was significantly different for EL1 and ELL children. Based on these detailed analyses it was concluded that differences in PPVT-III standard scores for EL1 and ELL 4- and 5-year-old children were not due to test bias, nor were they due to specific item properties. The mean differences observed between EL1 and ELL children were thus taken to reflect item impact, a difference in overall receptive vocabulary. In addition to establishing the utility of the PPVT-III for linguistically diverse kindergarten children this study also provides a descriptive picture of how EL1 and ELL children show variation across different types of word learning. iii Acknowledgements My supervisor, Dr. Janette Pelletier, was a constant support throughout the entire process of my PhD program. She offered continual motivation and support and was always available to answer questions, read drafts and discuss important issues. Her immense knowledge and experience in the field of early childhood research allowed this project to be so successful. I would also like to thank my committee members, Dr. Carl Corter and Dr. Ruth Childs. Their expertise in research methods, child development and educational measurement were an invaluable help. Without them this project also would never have been completed. I would also like to acknowledge the support of Dr. Olesya Falenchuk. She offered guidance and support throughout the analysis and writing process. I am grateful for the support of my family and friends. My husband, Aaron Platt, provided unwavering support throughout this academic endeavor. He has supported this process from the beginning and has pushed me to keep going when I felt the challenges were too great. My son Avery has supported me with love and smiles. His presence kept me working and looking towards my final goal. My parents, Margie and Jerome Shore, have inspired me with a love of learning and the confidence that I can do whatever I put my mind to. Without their early influence, none of this would have come to fruition. I am also thankful to the children, families and schools who participated in the research. Without them this important research would not have been possible. In addition, I am thankful to the large research team who worked throughout the year to collect the data. This was an extensive task carried out by a dedicated group of excellent researchers. Finally, The Institute of Child Study is where I began my formal education and completed my formal education. I am grateful to all of the people who have worked tirelessly iv over the years to maintain the vision of a child centered learning environment with research at the core. v Table of Contents ABSTRACT II ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS IV TABLE OF CONTENTS VI LIST OF TABLES VIII LIST OF FIGURES IX INTRODUCTION 1 LITERATURE REVIEW 9 Early Vocabulary Acquisition 9 Measuring Vocabulary 17 Differences in Children’s Performance on the PPVT-III in Relation to Demographic Factors and Word Categorization 27 METHODS 45 Participants 45 Instruments 47 Procedure 50 RESULTS 60 PPVT-III total score analysis 60 Individual Item Analyses 64 Item Category Analyses 74 DISCUSSION 80 vi Overview 80 Vocabulary Differences as a Function of Demographic Differences 80 Differential Item Functioning on the PPVT-III 84 Word Type Differences on the PPVT-III 86 The Appropriateness of the PPVT-III in a Canadian Multilingual Context 88 Implications 88 Limitations and Future Directions 91 Conclusions 92 REFERENCES 94 Appendix A 109 Appendix B 110 Appendix C 110 vii List of Tables Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations of PPVT-III Scores by Gender, Grade, Language Status and Family Education 60 Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations for Groups of Students Defined by Grade, Gender, Language Status and Maternal Education 61 Table 3 MH-DIF Statistics for Dichotomous Items 25 through 96 68 Table 4 Means and Standard Deviations Proportion Correct Scores by Teachability 77 Table 5 Means and Standard Deviations Proportion Correct Scores by Semantic Category 77 Table 6 Paired Samples t-test Based on Teachability and Semantic Category 78 Table 7 Basic Nouns by Teachability Word Type 78 VIII List of Figures Figure 1 Staircase Graph for ELL 65 Figure 2 Staircase Graph for EL1 66 Figure 3 Percentage of Correct and Incorrect Responses for EL1 and ELL children on the PPVT-III 67 Figure 4 Teachability categorization of words per set on the PPVT-III 75 Figure 5 Semantic categorization of words per set on the PPVT-III 76 IX Introduction Early vocabulary acquisition plays an important role in the development of decoding skills, reading comprehension and ultimate school success (Hart & Risley, 2003, 1995; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; Walker, Greenwood, Hart & Carta, 1994). Phonemic awareness, word identification and vocabulary are all considered instrumental in developing competent reading skills that lead to successful reading comprehension (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000). However, any of these three skills alone is not adequate for mature reading comprehension; reading requires the combination of these three skills as well as oral comprehension (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000). Research in recent years has paid increasing attention to the positive roles that oral language development and vocabulary acquisition play and the negative implications of low word knowledge for reading comprehension (White, Graves & Slater, 1990). In order to understand what is both heard and read, young children need to have a well-developed receptive vocabulary, that is, the understanding of spoken words. However, there are many challenges for young children in acquiring the meaning of a new word. A primary challenge is the multiple meanings a single word may hold (Heibert & Kamil, 2005). In English, multiple meanings of a single word are common and words are often used in different contexts, thus creating a lexical challenge for children learning English. To make sense of new English words, children must rely on the context in which they hear a word or on the pairing of a spoken word with an object or action. A secondary challenge is discerning the meaning of a new word from context when it is heard without an additional explanation, for example, “hare” and “hair”. However, despite these two challenges it seems that children manage to learn the meanings of 1
Description: