ebook img

ERIC EJ941693: A Comparison of Procedural Variations in Teaching Behavior Chains: Manual Guidance, Trainer Completion, and No Completion of Untrained Steps PDF

2011·0.59 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ941693: A Comparison of Procedural Variations in Teaching Behavior Chains: Manual Guidance, Trainer Completion, and No Completion of Untrained Steps

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2011, 44, 559–569 NUMBER3 (FALL2011) A COMPARISON OF PROCEDURAL VARIATIONS IN TEACHING BEHAVIOR CHAINS: MANUAL GUIDANCE, TRAINER COMPLETION, AND NO COMPLETION OF UNTRAINED STEPS STACIE L. BANCROFT NEWENGLANDCENTERFORCHILDREN ANDNORTHEASTERNUNIVERSITY JULIE S. WEISS NEWENGLANDCENTERFORCHILDREN AND MYRNA E. LIBBY AND WILLIAM H. AHEARN NEWENGLANDCENTERFORCHILDREN ANDNORTHEASTERNUNIVERSITY Wecomparedvariationsforteachingasequenceofresponsesthroughforwardchaining.Seven children who had been diagnosed with autism participated in a comparison of teacher completion (TC) of steps beyond the training step and manually guiding the student (SC) to complete steps beyond the training step. A no-completion (NC) condition, in which the steps beyond the training step were not completed, was added to the comparison with 4 of the participants.Resultsshowedthatlearningoccurredwithallprocedures,although5participants acquiredthechainsmostefficientlyintheSCconditionandtheother2learnedmostefficiently in the TC condition. Of the participants for whom an NC condition was included, the tasks were acquired with the shortest average session length and total training time. Despite the potential benefits of TC and SC procedures, NC is a viable option and may be preferable for some students. Keywords: behavior chains, autism,playskills _______________________________________________________________________________ Many socially important skills taught to Fleming, 2004),vocational tasks (Duran, 1985; children with autism and developmental dis- Maciag, Schuster, Collins, & Cooper, 2000), abilities consist of a series of sequenced following picture activity schedules (MacDuff, responses. A common method for teaching Krantz,&McClannahan,1993),andplayskills these skills includes conducting a task analysis (Libby, Weiss, Bancroft, & Ahearn, 2008). to break the chain of responses into a series of These chains can be taught using forward, manageable steps that can be taught one at a backward, or total-task chaining procedures. In time (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). forward chaining, steps are taught in a forward Breaking down a complex behavior and teach- sequence starting with trainingon the first step. ing its component parts as a chain have been After the student has demonstrated indepen- successful in teaching self-care skills (Horner & dence on this step, the second step becomes the Keilitz, 1975; Matson, Taras, Sevin, Love, & training step. The student then independently Fridley, 1990; Stokes, Cameron, Dorsey, & completes the first step and receives training on the second step within each trial. This progres- Address correspondence to Stacie L. Bancroft, New sion continues until the student independently England Center for Children,33Turnpike Road, South- performs each step in the chain. In backward borough, Massachusetts 01772 (e-mail: sbancroft@ chaining, the final step is trained first. After the necc.org). doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-559 student has demonstrated mastery on the final 559 560 STACIE L. BANCROFT et al. step,trainingisconductedonthesecond-to-last research in this area, practical options for step and the student independently completes completion of the untrained steps may include the final step. This progression continues until the teacher manually guiding the student to all steps in the chain are performed indepen- complete the chain or completing the chain in dently. In total-task chaining, training is the presence of the student. provided on each step until all steps are A teacher-completion strategy may have the completed independently. advantage of allowing learning through obser- Responsepromptsdeliveredontrainingsteps vation of the untrained steps. Griffen, Wolery, may include vocal instruction, modeling, or and Schuster (1992), for example, taught a physical guidance (e.g., Cuvo, Leaf, & Bor- food-preparation chain to one student while akove, 1978; Glendenning, Adams, & Stern- other students observed the steps. When the berg, 1983). Response prompts can be faded observingstudentswerepresentedwiththetask, until the student is independently performing they were able to complete most it. When the the training step. Libby et al. (2008) compared untrained steps become training steps, the the effectiveness and efficiency of least-to-most student may master them more quickly if the (LTM) prompt fading, most-to-least (MTL) steps have been modeled across many previous promptfading,andMTLwithadelay(MTLD) trials. Manually guiding the student to com- while teaching play skills via forward chaining. plete the chain may also have an advantage for MTLD provided an opportunity for the child similar reasons. When the student completes to initiate responding independently and the steps, he or she can learn through self- showed a possible advantage over MTL observation to complete the steps or through prompting. Both MTLD and LTM were found manually completing the steps. Either of these tobe most efficient,with fewererrorsoccurring methods may be helpful in capitalizing on with MTLD. learning opportunities when conducting a With forward chaining, it is unclear how to forward or backward chain. In other words, it best proceed with the steps that follow the may be beneficial for teachers to focus on a training step in each trial. The same problem is training step and in the same trial familiarize found with the steps that precede the training thestudentwithstepsnotyettrained.However, step in backward chains. For either type of both procedures add additional time to train- chaining, a clinician will have to decide how to ing. If the procedures do not dramatically complete the steps that are not yet the target of increase the speed of acquisition, it may not be training. In an eight-step forward chain, for worthwhile to complete the untrained steps. instance, the student may perform Steps 1 Not completing the untrained steps in a chain through3independentlywhilebeingtrainedon may permit more training trials in a given Step 4. After the student completes Step 4 with learning opportunity, require less hands-on thenecessarypromptingonagiventrial,Steps5 prompting, and may be preferred by some through 8 are still left in the chain. One students. procedural option is to terminate the trial In thepresentstudy,wecomparedtheeffects following completion of the training steps, of procedural variations for completing the leaving the remaining steps uncompleted (e.g., untrained steps in forward chains. We com- Hur & Osborne, 1993). However, the learner pared the effectiveness and efficiency of student may benefit from exposure to the remaining completion(SC)withmanualguidance,teacher steps.Inaddition,somebehaviorchains,suchas completion (TC) of untrained steps, and no washing hair or brushing teeth, may necessitate completion (NC) of untrained steps on the completion of the chain. Despite the lack of acquisition of behavior chains of similar PROCEDURAL VARIATIONS 561 difficulty. The method was similar to that used and were red, yellow, or green. Different base by Libby et al. (2008). colors were assigned across conditions for each participant and were counterbalanced across participants. For each structure, the individual METHOD building pieces varied in color and shape, but Participants and Settings onlyonewasusedforeachstepinthechain(see Three8-year-oldgirls(Tina,Pam,andKelly) Libby et al., 2008, for an image of constructs and four 10- to 16-year-old boys (Paul, Mario, used in this study). The four structures were Lance, and Ray) participated in the current evaluated by six independent raters (teachers study. All participants had been diagnosed with and administrators) and judged to be of autismandwereenrolledinaprivateresidential equivalent difficulty. The raters considered the treatmentprogramforchildrenwithautismand difficulty of discriminating between pieces, the other developmental disabilities. Tina and Pam relative difficulty of attaching pieces to each both communicated vocally, and they were other, and the relative symmetry of each noted to request some items and engage in structure. simple conversation with others. Kelly, Paul, Prior to the study, a paired-stimulus prefer- Mario, and Ray used a combination of vocal ence assessment (Fisher et al., 1992) was utterances and augmentative communication in conducted with each of the participants to the form of picture exchange communication determine highly preferred food items to be systems (PECS) or voice-output devices. Lance used as reinforcers during training trials. used PECS alone. All participants showed severe delays across all areas of skill acquisition. Response Measurement and Interobserver They had a history of exposure to complex Agreement behavior chains, such as showering or playing a Data were collected on the number of game, using chaining procedures. Each partic- consecutive steps with correct independent ipant demonstrated mastery in probes of responses on each trial. Correct order and generalized imitation as a prerequisite skill for placement of the Lego piece were required to inclusion in the study. score a step as being correctly completed. For Sessions were conducted in either the Paul, Mario, Lance, and Ray, data also were student’s classroom or in a work area in the collected on training duration. Reliability and student’s residence. Each participant completed procedural integrity were assessed by having training sessions in the same location. Materials eithertwoobserversviewavideotapeofsessions in the training sessions included a table and or having a secondary observer independently chairs, reinforcers, data sheets, a video camera record data during a session. Two trained when a secondary observer was not available, observers collected data and scored interobserv- and the materials needed for the task. eragreementandproceduralintegrityinatleast 33%ofsessions (range, 35%to 48%).Foreach Materials session in which reliability was calculated, Legoconstructionwasselectedasthetraining agreements and disagreements were determined task with the purpose of teaching the partici- for each step of each trial within the session. pants an appropriate leisure activity. In addi- Reliability was calculated by dividing the total tion, construction served as an analogue to number of agreements by the total number of other behavior chains that are typically taught. agreements plus disagreements and multiplying Each structure consisted of a base on which by 100%. Agreement scores averaged 98% seven other pieces were placed. The bases for across all participants (range, 94% to 100%). thefourstructureswerethesameshapeandsize, For procedural integrity, one observer assessed 562 STACIE L. BANCROFT et al. step-by step accuracy of the trainer’s delivery of The trainer used forward chaining in all reinforcement, delivery of prompts, and setting conditions. The trainer placed the pieces in thetraining step and prompt foreach trial.The front of the participant in a quasirandom mean accuracy score for procedural integrity arrangement and said, ‘‘let’s play.’’ Reinforce- across trainers and prompting techniques was ment was delivered immediately after correct 98% (range, 92% to 100%). completionofthetrainingstepandconsistedof the trainer saying ‘‘good job’’ and delivering a Procedure preferred food item. Following the training An eight-step task analysis was developed for step, the remaining steps of the chain were each structure that determined the order of completed according to the completion condi- placement of the Lego pieces. At the beginning tion, as described below. of a trial, the pieces were separated and Any errors made on the training step were presented in a random array in front of the immediately corrected with hand-over-hand participant. The first step of the task analysis guidance. Two consecutive errors led to an was for the participant to pull the base out of increaseintheintrusivenessofthepromptused. the group of pieces. The second, and all Errors on previously acquired steps of the chain following,stepsinvolvedpickingupandplacing were immediately corrected with hand-over- one block in the order determined by the task hand guidance. Two consecutive errors on a analysis. At the beginning of each training previously acquired step resulted in retraining session, the participant chose between two on that and all subsequent steps. highly preferred food items. The chosen item Each play structure was considered mastered was used as a reinforcer for that session. Each following two consecutive trials of independent session consisted of one presession probe trial and accurate performance on each step. This followed by 10 training trials. could be achieved in two ways. Training could During the probe trial, the trainer placed the continue through the last step, ending with two piecesofthestructureinfrontoftheparticipant trials of independent performance on the final in a random array and said, ‘‘let’s play.’’ The training step. Alternatively, independent and probetrialcontinueduntiltheparticipantmade accurateperformancecouldbedemonstratedon an error or went 15 s without responding. No a presession probe. As mentioned previously, if consequences were delivered for correct or performancewasindependentandaccurateona incorrect responses. The purpose of the preses- second probe, the play structure was considered sion probe was to determine intermittently mastered. Therefore, a structure could be whetheror notindependentperformance ofthe complete chain had emerged. If the participant mastered at the start of a session, as indicated did not complete the structure correctly, the 10 by performance during the additional probe, or training trials began. If the participant com- mastery could be achieved during the training pleted the structure correctly during the preses- trials.Followingmastery,ageneralizationprobe sion probe, an additional probe was conducted. wasconductedinadifferentsettingwithanovel The structure was considered mastered if the therapist. participant completed it correctly during the Sessions were conducted one to three times second probe. Performance on the probe trials per day, 2 to 5 days per week. Multiple sessions was not used to prescribe training steps. The on1daywereseparatedbyaminimumof10min training step for a given session was based on of unrelated activities. The same teacher worked theperformanceinthepriorsession.Probedata with each participant throughout training. from the first presession probe served as Teacher completion. In each trial, the partic- baseline. ipanthadtheopportunitytocompletepreviously PROCEDURAL VARIATIONS 563 mastered steps in the chain. After reaching the pendent steps completed at the end of each 10- training step, the teacher delivered prompts trial session for each of the three participants in accordingtoamost-to-leastpromptinghierarchy theTCandSCcomparison.If,forexample,the (see Libby et al., 2008, for a description of the participant was training on Step 5 in the final prompt hierarchy). Errorless completion of the trial of a session and had mastered Steps 1 training step, given the prescribed prompt, through4,afourwasrecordedasthenumberof resulted in reinforcement. As each step was steps with independent performance for that mastered,thenextstepbecamethetrainingstep. session. Sessions in which the participant Aftertheparticipantcompletedthetrainingstep, demonstrated independent performance during the teacher blocked the participant from further the initial probe and again during the repeated manipulating the blocks and completed the probe are marked with asterisks. Tina acquired remaining steps of the structure in front of the the chain in the TC condition in six training participant. The participant was free to observe sessions and then showed independent perfor- or not to observe the remaining steps. mance in the following presession probes. She Student completion. The SC condition was completed the SC condition in seven training identical to the TC condition, with the sessions. Kelly acquired the chain in the TC exception of the completion of the untrained condition in eight training sessions and in the steps. After student completion of the training SC condition in six training sessions. Pam step and delivery of a reinforcer given no errors required several sessions to complete the third on the training step, the teacher manually stepofthechainintheTCcondition,taking12 guided the student to complete the remaining sessions (Sessions 3 through 14) to master this steps in the chain. step. After mastering Step 3 and Step 4, Pam No completion. The procedure in the NC then demonstrated mastery of the eight-step condition was identical to the TC and SC chain in presession probes of Session 15. conditions,exceptthatthetrialendedfollowing Although Steps 5 through 8 had never been the training step. trained using the prompt hierarchy, Pam apparently learned to complete them through Design observing the therapist complete the chain. In An adapted alternating treatments design was the SC condition, she made steady progress, used to assess the completion conditions. Each mastering one step per session and completing participant was taught to put together two or the chain in nine sessions. All participants three eight-piece structures. Each structure was demonstrated generalization and maintenance assigned to a particular condition, and the by successfully building the structures in structures were counterbalanced across partici- alternate settings with different teachers. In pants. A comparison of TC and SC was summary, all three participants mastered the conducted with Tina, Pam, and Kelly. Paul, chain in both the TC and SC conditions. Tina Mario,Lance,andRayparticipatedinacompar- mastered the chain in the TC condition more isonofTC,SC,andNC.Sessionswerealternated quickly than in the SC condition, although the such that no more than two of any completion differences were negligible. Kelly and Pam conditionwereconductedconsecutively. mastered the chain in the SC condition in fewer sessions than in the TC session. RESULTS The results for the comparisons of SC, TC, None of the participants independently and NC are shown in Figure 2. Paul acquired completedanysteps beyondthefirstinbaseline thechainintheTCconditionin11sessions,in probes. Figure 1 shows the number of inde- the SC condition in six sessions, and in the NC 564 STACIE L. BANCROFT et al. Figure 1. The number of steps of the task analysis performed independently during training with the student- completion(SC)andteacher-completion(TC)strategiesforthreeparticipants.Asterisksdenotetrialsinwhichmastery was demonstrated in a presession probe. The open symbols represent performance on a generalization probe for eachcondition. PROCEDURAL VARIATIONS 565 Figure 2. The number of steps of the task analysis performed independently during training with the student- completion (SC), teacher-completion (TC), and no-completion (NC) strategies for four participants. Asterisks denote trials in which mastery was demonstrated in a presession probe. The open symbols represent performance on a generalizationprobe for eachcondition. 566 STACIE L. BANCROFT et al. condition in nine sessions. Mario acquired the DISCUSSION chainintheTCconditionin14sessions,inthe The majority of the participants acquired the SC condition in eight sessions, and in the NC chaininthefewestnumberoftrialswiththeSC condition in 13 sessions. As with Paul and procedure, with the remaining two participants Mario, it took Lance the greatest number of acquiring the chain in the fewest number of sessions (31) to complete the structure in the trainingtrialsintheTCcondition.Forthefour TCconditionandthefewestnumberofsessions participants included in the three-condition (23) to complete the structure in the SC comparison, the reduced session duration in condition. Lance completed the structure in NC produced the briefest overall training the NC condition in 26 sessions. Ray’s results durations. These data may be useful in clinical were contrary to the other participants. It took settings when deciding which completion Ray the greatest number of sessions (21) to procedure to use for a particular individual. complete the structure in the SC condition and The SC condition may be the best default the fewest number of sessions (12) to complete strategy to use with learners who tolerate the structure in the TC condition. He com- physical prompting. When problem behavior pleted the structure in the NC condition in 16 is associated with physical prompting, some sessions. version of the TC condition may produce AllsessionsintheNCcomparisonweretimed, learning while minimizing the likelihood of andaveragesessiondurationineachconditionis problem behavior. In addition, if an individual shown across participants in Figure 3. For each is noted to be on task for only brief periods of participant, the NC sessions were the briefest, time,anNCproceduremaybeoptimalbecause withaveragesessiondurationrangingfrom3.4to session length is the briefest and more trials can 5.7 min. This was expected, because only the be completed in a given session. stepsthroughthetrainingstepwerecompletedin Another variable that may be considered this condition. The TC conditions were length- when choosing a completion procedure for ier, with average session duration ranging from teachingabehaviorchainisthetotalamountof 6.6 to 11.2 min across participants. The SC training time. Although three of the four sessions were the lengthiest, with average dura- participants in the second comparison com- tion ranging from 8.3to14.2min. pleted the structure in the SC condition in the Table 1 shows the number of sessions to fewest number of sessions, these sessions took mastery, number of trials to mastery, and longer to complete than those in the NC number of errors per training session for each conditions. Structures built with the NC participant across all conditions. Generalization procedure were mastered in the shortest trials were included in the number of trials to amount of time for all participants, ranging mastery. All participants, with the exception of from 31 to 130 min. For Paul, Mario, and Ray,completedthetaskinfewertrialsintheSC Lance, mastery of the structures in the SC condition than in the TC condition. All condition took the next longest duration participants in the NC comparison completed (range, 85 to 194 min), and mastery of thetaskintheNCconditioninanintermediate structures in the TC condition took the longest number of sessions. Errors were consistent with duration (range, 123 to 204 min). For Ray, thispattern,withtotalerrorscorrelatedwiththe however,thetotaldurationoftheSCcondition frequency of trials across all participants. Errors (174 min), was almost twice that of the TC per session were therefore relatively evenly condition (89 min). These data suggest that the distributedacrossconditions,withnocondition additional time spent on demonstrating or producing consistently higher rates of errors. manually guiding the participant through the PROCEDURAL VARIATIONS 567 Figure 3. Average duration of sessions (top panel) and total duration of skill acquisition (bottom panel) for the student-completion, teacher-completion, andno-completion conditions for fourparticipants. untrained steps of the task may not lead to behavior. Given that several participants did more efficient skill acquisition. not require training on some of the final steps, It is possible that participants did not always it is likely that these participants did attend to attend to the completion steps in the TC and the steps (see Nevin, Davison, Odum, & SC conditions, limiting how much they could Shahan, 2007). benefit from these approaches. A measure of Another limitation includes the lack of overt attending was not included, because testing to determine the functioning level of attending includes both overt and covert theparticipants.Higherfunctioningindividuals 568 STACIE L. BANCROFT et al. Table 1 NumberofTraining Sessions, Training Trials, andErrors PerSession Sessions Trials Errors Participant TC SC NC TC SC NC TC SC NC Tina 8 7 74 64 1.5 1.12 Pam 14 9 137 88 1.6 2.0 Kelly 9 7 87 64 0.91 0.62 Paul 12 7 10 114 64 94 1.41 0.43 1.2 Mario 15 8 13 144 77 124 0.8 1.5 0.69 Lance 32 24 27 314 234 264 0.36 0.27 0.38 Ray 13 22 17 124 214 164 0.45 0.22 1.12 Note.TC 5teacher completion, SC 5student completion, andNC5 nocompletion. may be more likely to benefit from the behavior chains of various complexities may modeling or practice of untrained steps in the be an important extension to examine in future TC and SC conditions, respectively. More research. specifically, identification of necessary prereq- uisite skills for benefiting from TC or SC REFERENCES procedures requires further investigation. For Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). instance,itispossiblethatanindividualwithan Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle advanced imitative repertoire may benefit from River,NJ:Prentice Hall. the TC condition. However, given the consis- Cuvo, A. J., Leaf, R. B., & Borakove, L. S. (1978). Teaching janitorial skills to the mentally retarded: tently greater overall training duration for TC Acquisition,generalization,andmaintenance.Journal and SC conditions across all participants, the ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 11,345–355. benefits of TC and SC procedures for individ- Duran, E. (1985). Teaching janitorial skills to autistic adolescents. Adolescence,20(7), 225–232. uals with advanced imitative repertoires may be Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. limited. A final limitation of the study was the P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison method for determining equivalent difficulty of of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for personswithsevereandprofounddisabilities.Journal Lego structures. Although raters were given ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 25,491–498. general features of task difficulty to consider, a Glendenning, N.,Adams,G.L.,&Sternberg,L.(1983). more stringent method for ensuring task Comparison of prompt sequences. American Journal ofMentalDeficiency,88, 321–325. difficulty may have been beneficial. Griffen, A. K., Wolery, M., & Schuster, J. W. (1992). The opportunity to learn through observing Triadic instruction of chained food preparation others perform actions may be beneficial in responses: Acquisition and observational learning. JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 25,193–204. some situations that were not examined in the Horner, R. D., & Keilitz, I. (1975). Training mentally current research. For example, although all retarded adolescents to brush their teeth. Journal of participants demonstrated skill mastery in AppliedBehaviorAnalysis,8,301–309. Hur,J.,&Osborne,S.(1993).Acomparisonofforward another context and with another teacher, and backward chaining methods used in teaching long-termmaintenanceofskillswasnotassessed corsage making skills to mentally retarded adults. across conditions. The SC and TC procedures British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 39, may produce better long-term maintenance of 108–117. Libby,M.E.,Weiss,J.S.,Bancroft,S.L.,&Ahearn,W. skills than the NC condition does because the H. (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least- participant typically has more exposure to each to-most prompting on the acquisition of play skills. of the steps in the chain than he or she does in BehaviorAnalysis inPractice,1, 37–43. MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. an NC condition. Assessments of effectiveness (1993). Teaching children with autism to use and efficiency of these procedures across photographic activity schedules: Maintenance and

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.