ebook img

ERIC EJ941686: A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Receptive Labeling to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders PDF

2011·2 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ941686: A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Receptive Labeling to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2011, 44, 475–498 NUMBER3 (FALL2011) A COMPARISON OF METHODS FOR TEACHING RECEPTIVE LABELING TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS LAURA L. GROW MUNROE-MEYERINSTITUTE UNIVERSITYOFNEBRASKAMEDICALCENTER JAMES E. CARR AUBURNUNIVERSITY TIFFANY M. KODAK AND CANDICE M. JOSTAD MUNROE-MEYERINSTITUTE UNIVERSITYOFNEBRASKAMEDICALCENTER AND APRIL N. KISAMORE WESTERNNEWENGLANDCOLLEGE Many early intervention curricular manuals recommend teaching auditory-visual conditional discriminations (i.e., receptive labeling) using the simple-conditional method in which component simple discriminations are taught in isolation and in the presence of a distracter stimulus before the learner is required to respond conditionally. Some have argued that this proceduremightbesusceptibletofaultystimuluscontrolsuchasstimulusoverselectivity(Green, 2001).Consequently,therehasbeenacallfortheuseofalternativeteachingproceduressuchas theconditional-onlymethod,whichinvolvesconditionaldiscriminationtrainingfromtheonset of intervention. The purpose of the present study was to compare the simple-conditional and conditional-only methods for teaching receptive labeling to 3 young children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. The data indicated that the conditional-only method was a more reliable and efficient teaching procedure. In addition, several error patterns emerged during training using the simple-conditional method. The implications of the results with respect to current teaching practices inearly intervention programsarediscussed. Keywords: autism,conditionaldiscriminationtraining,earlyintervention,receptivelabeling, stimuluscontrol _______________________________________________________________________________ Conditional discriminations are one of the intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) (e.g., most commonly targeted skills in early and matchingidenticalitems,receptivelanguage).A conditional discrimination involves four com- Thisarticleis basedonadissertation submittedbythe ponents: a sample stimulus, the presentation of firstauthorunderthesupervisionofthesecondauthorin comparison stimuli, a response, and a conse- partial fulfillment of the requirements for the doctoral quence (Saunders & Spradlin, 1989, 1990). In degree in behavior analysis from Western Michigan University.WethankcommitteemembersWayneFuqua, atypicaltrialduringauditory-visualconditional Linda LeBlanc, Cynthia Pietras, and Ivy Chong for their discrimination training, the teacher presents an helpful comments. We also thank Scott McPhee, array (usually two or three) of visual compar- Courtney Lemons, Christine Foster, and Sarah Gregor ison stimuli (e.g., pictures of a bed, chair, and for their assistance with data collection and stimulus generation. table). Second, the teacher delivers an auditory Addresscorrespondence toJames E.Carr,Department sample stimulus (e.g., ‘‘point to chair’’). Third, of Psychology, 226 Thach Hall, Auburn University, the learner engages in a response (e.g., pointing Alabama36849(e-mail: [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-475 to or touching one of the pictures) or is 475 476 LAURA L. GROW et al. prompted to respond. Fourth, the teacher trial format and introducing increasingly diffi- provides differential consequences for correct cult discriminations over time. The rationale and incorrect responses. This example of iden- behind this approach is to break down the tifying an object from an array after hearing conditional discrimination into multiple, easier its name is commonly referred to as receptive steps and gradually increase the difficulty as the labeling or receptive identification. learner acquires simpler discriminations. A Accurate responses during conditional dis- graphical depiction of how the simple-condi- criminations require several prerequisite skills tional method can be applied to teaching a (McIlvane, Dube, Kledaras, Iennaco, & Stod- three-stimulus array receptive labeling program dard, 1990). Learners must observe and is shown in Figure 1. Steps 1, 2, and 6 consist differentially respond to comparison stimuli of teaching simple discriminations in isolation such as a visual array of pictures or objects. in a massed-trial format. Simple discrimination Learners must also attend and differentially training in isolation does not necessarily target respond to various sample stimuli that are or require any of the prerequisite skills to presented across teaching trials. Learners should complete conditional discriminations (e.g., attend to the sample stimulus and subsequently attending to the auditory sample stimulus). observe and respond to the comparison stimuli Steps 3 and 4 involve simple discrimination (i.e., a successive discrimination). training in the presence of a distracter stimulus. In a recent Internet survey, Love, Carr, It should be noted that the distracter stimuli Almason, and Petursdottir (2009) assessed a used in Steps 3 and 4 eventually function as a number of EIBI program practices and identi- discriminative stimulus (S+) during the simple- fied two common approaches to teaching conditionalmethod.Forexample,thedistracter conditional discriminations: the simple-condi- stimulus in Step 3 later functions as the S+ in tional and conditional-only methods. The Step4.ComparedtoSteps1and2,Steps3and survey respondents (EIBI program supervisors) 4 involve a more difficult discrimination that reported implementing the simple-conditional requires attending and differentially responding method most often (37% of respondents) to the visual comparison stimuli. However, the whereasslightlyfewerserviceprovidersreported learner is not required to attend to the auditory using the conditional-only method (32% of sample stimulus because it remains the same respondents). Other respondents (31%) report- across the massed trials. Steps 5, 7, 8, and 9 ed implementing either the simple-conditional involve either two- or three-stimulus array method or the conditional-only method de- conditionaldiscriminationsinwhichthelearner pending on the specific repertoire of the child. must attend to both the auditory and visual Given that most EIBI curricular manuals stimuli to access reinforcement for correct recommend the simple-conditional method responding. (e.g., Leaf & McEachin, 1999; Lovaas, 2003; In her discussion of stimulus control tech- Maurice, Green, & Luce, 2001), we were sur- nology,Green(2001)notedthatanindividual’s prised that EIBI program supervisors reported history with procedures like the simple-condi- using the conditional-only method almost as tional method may promote faulty stimulus often as the simple-conditional method. control during the final steps of conditional The simple-conditional method is based on discrimination training, particularly for recep- procedures described by Lovaas (2003) for tive labeling programs (i.e., auditory-visual teaching receptive labeling and matching in conditional discriminations). During simple EIBI programs. This method involves training discrimination training in isolation (i.e., Steps component simple discriminations in a massed- 1,2,and6),incidentallearningmayoccursuch RECEPTIVE LABELING 477 Figure 1. Visual depiction ofthree trialsduring each step ofthe simple-conditional method. that the learner observes the auditory discrim- without attending to the specific auditory inative stimulus and the correct visual stimulus stimulus (oreven thevisual stimulus)bysimply and relates those stimuli with each other; repeating the same response that was reinforced however, the skill is not directly taught or in the preceding trial. The discrimination in required(McIlvaneetal.,1990).Thus,alearner Steps 3 and 4 is made more difficult by couldrespondcorrectlyduringSteps1,2,and6 requiring differential responding to the visual 478 LAURA L. GROW et al. comparison stimuli, but attending to the cause prior presentations of auditory stimuli auditory sample stimulus is not yet required. were superfluous to the reinforcement contin- Collectively, Steps 1 through 4 may promote gency. As a result, the visual stimulus present overselectivity to the visual component of the during either response prompting or reinforcer antecedent stimuli by establishing a history of delivery in the preceding trial may come to reinforcementforrespondingtothesamevisual influence selection responses. comparison stimulus without requiring attend- Other error patterns may occur during the ing to the auditory stimulus. Thus, the visual simple-conditional method due to the arrange- comparison stimulus may control subsequent mentofthecomparisonstimuliduringtraining. responding rather than the auditory stimulus More specifically, the majority of steps in the and corresponding visual stimulus together, simple-conditional method include an array of producing faulty stimulus control. only two comparison stimuli, which may result There is some evidence that faulty sources of in several error patterns. Onesuch error pattern stimulation may exert control over responses as is position bias, in which the learner reliably afunctionofexposuretothesimple-conditional selects the stimulus in either the left or right. method. Lovaas (2003) described several error The schedule of reinforcement (i.e., variable- patterns that commonly arise during receptive ratio [VR] 2) associated with many types of labeling programs. Two types of error patterns, error patterns during a two-stimulus array termed win-stay responses, may result from a discrimination procedure may support the history of the early steps of the simple- maintenance of those error patterns over time conditional method. Molar win-stay responses, because the schedule is denser than arrange- characterized by a disproportionately high per- ments with a larger array of stimuli (Kangas & centage of responses to the particular visual sti- Branch, 2008; MacKay, 1991). mulus that served as the S+ in the preceding In contrast to the simple-conditional meth- acquisition step, might occur during the od, in the conditional-only method (described transition from simple discrimination training by Green, 2001), component simple discrimi- ofaparticularstimuluswithdistracterstoeither nations are not targeted prior to conditional simple discrimination training with a different discrimination training. Rather, all stimuli stimulus or conditional discrimination training (usually at least three) are presented and (e.g., transitioning from Step 3 to Step 4). targeted as a conditional discrimination from Molar win-stay responses likely result from (a) the onset of intervention. For example, the an immediate reinforcement history involving a experimenter presents three comparison stimuli particularvisualstimulusand(b)theavailability andirregularlyalternatespresentingoneofthree of thatstimulus as a responseoption during the corresponding auditory sample stimuli across subsequent teaching step. session trials. The conditional-only method is The early steps of the simple-conditional identical to Step 9 of the simple-conditional method may also promote molecular win-stay method depicted in Figure 1. The rationale responses that involve responding to the same behind this procedure is to begin with a visual stimulus that was targeted in the procedurethatrequiresthenecessaryrepertoires preceding trial, regardless of the presented needed to respond correctly (i.e., attending and sample stimulus in the current trial. Because differentiallyrespondingtothesampleandcom- the auditory discriminative stimulus remains parison stimuli). Furthermore, individuals may unvaried in Steps 1 through 4, the learner may be less likely to engage in consistent error pat- ignore the auditory sample stimuli in subse- terns (e.g., position biases, win-stay responses) quent conditional discrimination training be- duringtheconditional-onlymethodthanduring RECEPTIVE LABELING 479 the simple-conditional method because the conditionalmethod,theconditional-onlymeth- schedule of reinforcement for a particular od may promote appropriate stimulus control error pattern is thinner in the conditional- during conditional discrimination training and only method. An important difference between may decrease the likelihood of error patterns the simple-conditional and conditional-only that interfere with acquisition. To date, no methods is the arrangement of the sample and published studies have compared the relative comparison stimuli during training. That is, effectiveness and efficiency of the simple- multiple sample and comparison stimulus conditional and conditional-only methods for pairs are targeted in the same teaching session teaching conditional discriminations. There- from the onset of training in the conditional- fore, the purpose of the current study was to only method, whereas the numbers of sample compare the relative utility of the simple- and comparison stimuli are gradually increased conditional and conditional-only methods for acrossteachingsessionsinthesimple-conditional teaching receptive labeling to children with method. ASD. Gutierrez et al. (2009) conducted one of the first studies to evaluate the impact of different METHOD types of simple discrimination training on Participants and Setting subsequent conditional discrimination learning Three children with a diagnosis of an ASD in applied settings. Three children with an participated in the study. Erin was a 7-year-old autism spectrum disorder (ASD) were taught girl who had been diagnosed with pervasive toidentifyanumberofstimulireceptively.Half developmental disorder (not otherwise speci- of the stimuli were taught first using simple fied). She spoke in full sentences and indepen- discrimination training in isolation. After dently completed most age-appropriate daily mastery, each of the stimuli was targeted in living skills. Prior to the study, she often the presence of a distracter stimulus. The other displayed faulty stimulus control during recep- half of the stimuli were trained in a simple tive labeling programs in her early intervention discrimination format in the presence of program. She had extensive exposure (approx- distracter stimuli from the onset of training. imately 3 years) to both teaching methods Following both training methods, the previous- evaluated in the study. Sessions were conducted ly taught component simple discriminations in a room converted into a work area in Erin’s were targeted in a conditional discrimination home. format. The learner acquired the stimuli in Shane was a 4-year-old boy who had been fewer sessions when the stimuli were taught in diagnosed with autistic disorder. He communi- the presence of distracter stimuli (i.e., without cated using gestures and several spoken words isolation training). These results may provide (e.g., pretzels, juice, water) and needed assis- some preliminary evidence that Steps 1, 2, and tance to complete several age-appropriate daily 6 of the simple-conditional method may not living activities. He had a brief history (approx- build necessary prerequisites for responding imately 6 months) with the simple-conditional accurately during conditional discrimination method in his preschool program prior to the training or enhance future conditional discrim- study. Sessions were conducted in an unused ination learning. room in Shane’s home. As mentioned previously, the conditional- Devin was a 4-year-old boy who had been only method is a widely used procedure for diagnosed with autistic disorder and disruptive teaching receptive labeling in EIBI programs behavior disorder (not otherwise specified). He (Love et al., 2009). Compared to the simple- communicated using three- to four-word utter- 480 LAURA L. GROW et al. ances. He had a brief history (approximately auditory sample stimulus. For each trial, 3 months) with the blocked-trial procedure observers also collected data on the comparison (described in the Discussion section) and the stimulus that the participant selected and its conditional-only method at his early interven- location in the stimulus array. tion clinic prior to the study. Sessions were The primary dependent measure was the conductedin a smalltreatmentroom in aclinic numberofsessionsrequiredtomeetthemastery setting. criteria for the three-stimulus array receptive Inclusion for the study required that partic- labeling program in each condition. The ipants (a) exhibited little or no severe problem mastery criteria in Steps 1 through 8 of the behavior, (b) tolerated physical contact, (c) simple-conditional method required the partic- displayed a matching repertoire (evaluated via ipants (a) to emit a correct independent the Assessment of Basic Learning Abilities; response for eight of the nine trials in the Martin & Yu, 2000), and (d) had goals in their sessionand(b)torespondaccuratelyduringthe educationplansthatinvolvedreceptivelabeling. first presentation of each stimulus in the nine- trial session. The masterycriterionforStep 9of Materials the simple-conditional method and the condi- Stimuli(i.e.,picturesofobjects)wereprinted tional-only method was three consecutive on sheets of paper (8.5 in. [21.6 cm] by 11 in. sessions with 100% correct independent re- [27.9 cm]) and placed in clear page protectors sponses. to generate a trial sheet. The experimenter Asecondindependentobservercollecteddata placed a dark-colored sheet on top of each trial on several learner responses and aspects of the sheet (a) to prevent the participant from stimulus arrangement, including independent viewing the visual comparison stimuli prior to and prompted correct responses, the location of thedeliveryoftheauditorysamplestimulusand thefirstresponseofeachtrial,thespecificvisual (b) to provide an opportunity for the partici- stimulus associated with the first response on pant to complete an observing response (de- each trial, and observing responses. An agree- scribed in more detail below). ment was defined if both observers coded (a) a Dependent Variables, Measurement, and correct, prompted,or no response; (b) thesame Interobserver Agreement visual stimulus for the first response in each Apaper-and-pencilmethodwasusedtoscore trial; (c) the same location of visual stimulus responses during each trial of a nine-trial selected;and(d)theoccurrenceoftheobserving session. Observers scored a correct independent response. A disagreement was coded during response if the participant pointed to the S+ trials in which observers scored any of the within 5 s of the presentation of the auditory responses differentlyfromeachother.Point-by- stimulus without errors or experimenter-deliv- point agreement was calculated by dividing the ered prompts. Observers scored a prompted number of agreements in a session by the correctresponseiftheparticipantpointedtothe number of agreements and disagreements (i.e., S+ within 5 s of an experimenter-delivered nine) and converting the proportion to a prompt. The data collectors scored no response percentage. Across all evaluations for Erin, if the participant did not point to a stimulus interobserver agreement was assessed during within 5 s after the presentation of the auditory 37% of sessions, and mean agreement was 98% stimulus. The data collectors scored an observ- (range,67%to100%).ForShane’sevaluations, ing response when the participant oriented his agreement was assessed for 35% of sessions orhereyestowardeachstimulusinthearrayfor and averaged 97% (range, 89% to 100%). For approximately 1 s prior to the delivery of the Devin’s evaluations, agreement was assessed RECEPTIVE LABELING 481 Table 1 StimuliTaught During Each Evaluation Participant Evaluation Conditionalonly Simple-conditional Erin 1 aardvark,gazelle,hedgehog bison,lemur,warthog 2 crane,elk,squid newt,sloth,yak 3 Asia,Australia,S.America Africa,Antarctica,Europe Shane 1 F,J,M(lettername) B,S,T(lettername) 2 Africa,Antarctica,Europe Asia,Australia,S.America 3 D,K,L(lettersound) G,H,R(lettersound) Devin 1 bathing,coloring,dancing catching,giving,sitting 2 C,G,O(lettername) M,V,W(lettername) during 32% of sessions and averaged 97% function of the teaching methods used rather (range, 78% to 100%). than characteristics of the stimulus sets. Training Sets Procedure A training set was comprised of three stimuli Reinforcer identification. A paired-stimulus (i.e., three auditory and corresponding visual preference assessment (Fisher et al., 1992) was stimuli). A stimulus was included in the conducted with items suggested by either the training set if the participant responded with participant’sprimarybehaviortherapist(Devin) no greater than 33% accuracy during baseline or their parents (Erin and Shane) on the probes (described below). Stimuli were selected Reinforcer Assessment for Individuals with based on the participants’ goals in either their Disabilities (Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, & Amari, individualized education plans or EIBI pro- 1996)toidentifyahierarchyofpreferreditems. grams.Foreachevaluation,onetrainingsetwas ForDevinandShane,fooditemswereincluded assigned to the simple-conditional and condi- only in the paired-stimulus preference assess- tional-onlymethods.Thatis,sixstimuliintotal ment. For Erin, the paired-stimulus prefer- were taught during each comparative evalua- ence assessment was conducted with activities tion.Thespecificstimulitaughtinthestudyare (e.g., board game). We also conducted a brief displayed in Table 1. multiple-stimulus without replacement prefer- We attempted to equate the training sets by ence assessment (MSWO; DeLeon & Iwata, selecting similar stimuli for each evaluation. In 1996) prior to each session with the top four or addition,thestimuliweregroupedsuchthatthe fiveitemsthathadbeenidentifiedinthepaired- auditory stimuli contained the same number of stimulus preference assessment in an attempt to syllables and were as distinct as possible. One control for potential fluctuations in preference exception to this occurred in Devin’s second across sessions. evaluation. Similar visual stimuli were targeted Baseline probes. Nine-trial baseline probes of in the training set because Devin had a history relations among stimuli were conducted to of incorrect responses with this type of identify target stimuli for the subsequent discrimination. Continents were targeted dur- evaluations. During baseline, an array of three ing Erin’s and Shane’s second evaluations, and pictureswaspresentedtotheparticipant,andhe those stimuli were counterbalanced across or she was instructed to emit an observing participants to ensure that outcomes were a response (i.e., turn over the dark-colored sheet 482 LAURA L. GROW et al. to expose the trial sheet). The purpose of this token economies; therefore, Erin had the response was to facilitate the observation of the opportunity to exchange the stickers for backup visual comparison stimuli. If the participant reinforcers (i.e., the items identified in the did not observe the comparison stimuli after MSWO) after she had accumulated 20 stickers. the sample was presented, the experimenter When given the opportunity to make an prompted observing to each comparison stim- exchange, Erin was given a choice between ulus (e.g., pointing to the stimuli, tapping near selecting an activity and playing for 5 min or the stimuli) until it observation occurred. Next, savingtheactivitytimeandaddingittoafuture the appropriate auditory stimulus (e.g., ‘‘point break. to lemur’’) was presented and the participant Simple-conditional method. The simple-con- was given a 5-s opportunity to respond. The ditional method was based on procedures auditorysamplestimulusandpositionoftheS+ described by Lovaas (2003). The general in the comparison array were rotated across teaching procedure involved a series of nine trials in the manner recommended by Green steps (see Figure 1). Steps 1, 2, and 6 involved (2001). Regardless of whether the response was simple discrimination training in isolation for correct or incorrect, the experimenter removed thethreestimuliinthetrainingset.ForSteps1, the stimuli and did not provide differential 2, and 6, the experimenter repeatedly presented consequences for selection responses. The one sample and one comparison stimulus (i.e., experimenter retained stimuli if accuracy was one S+) in each session. Steps 3 and 4 entailed 33% or lower during the baseline probe. simple discrimination training in the presence Generalteachingprocedure.Asinbaseline,the of a nontarget distracter stimulus. During Steps experimenter initiated a trial by presenting the 3 and 4, the experimenter repeatedly presented comparison stimuli and prompting the partic- one sample stimulus and two comparison ipant to complete an observing response. Next, stimuli (i.e., one S+ and one S2) in each the experimenter presented the auditory sample teachingsession.TheS2inStep3servedasthe stimulus. If the participant made an error or S+inStep4,andtheS+inStep3functionedas did not respond within 5 s, the experimenter the S2 in Step 4. Steps 5, 7, and 8 included a initiated a least-to-most prompt hierarchy two-stimulus array conditional discrimination (Horner & Keilitz, 1975) that included two in which both visual stimuli functioned as the levelsof modelprompts anda physicalprompt. S+andS2acrosstrialsinonesession.ForSteps The less intrusive model prompt involved the 5, 7, and 8, the experimenter presented one of experimenter pointing within 3 in. (7.6 cm) of two sample stimuli, which were irregularly the correct visual stimulus. The more intrusive alternated, and the two corresponding compar- model prompt involved the experimenter ison stimuli in each session. Step 9 consisted of pointing within 1 in. (2.5 cm) of the correct thepresentationofthreestimulithatfunctioned stimulus. Physical guidance involved the least as both theS+ and S2 in a session. Thatis, the amount of hand-over-hand guidance necessary experimenter presented one of three sample to ensure a correct response. The experimenter stimuli that were irregularly alternated across simultaneously presented the auditory sample trials and the three corresponding comparison stimulus with all prompts. Contingent on stimuli in each session. correct independent responses during teaching For Steps 1, 2, 6, and 9, we semirandomly trials, the experimenter delivered enthusiastic rotatedtheS+amongtheleft,middle,andright praise and a small piece of food (Shane and positions. For Steps 3, 4, 5, 7, and 8, we Devin) or one sticker (Erin). Erin had an semirandomly rotated the S+ between the right extensive history (approximately 3 years) with andleftpositions.Thecomparisonstimuliwere RECEPTIVE LABELING 483 presented in an array of two because counter- Due to the transience of auditory sample balancingtwostimuliacrossthreepositionsina stimuli, the auditory stimulus was repeated manner that is consistent with procedures every 2 s following Erin’s observing response described by Green (2001) was difficult to (Green, 2001) until a response occurred or 5 s arrange in a nine-trial session without targeting elapsedwithnoresponse.Inaddition,weadded a particular stimulus in an uneven manner an error-correction procedure to increase the across positions. In a nine-trial session with an likelihood that Erin responded to the correct array of two comparison stimuli, a given stimulus and to establish a history of reinforce- stimulus was disproportionately targeted in the ment for responses to the correct stimulus left and right positions in one teaching session. (Rodgers & Iwata, 1991; Smith, Mruzek, For steps that included an array of two stimuli Wheat, & Hughes, 2006). If an error occurred, (Steps 3, 4, 5, 7, and 8), we developed two theexperimenterpromptedthecorrectresponse different stimulus presentations to target each and immediately re-presented an identical trial. stimulus proportionately in the left and right This process was continued until a correct positions over the course of two sessions. independent response occurred. Correct inde- Conditional-only method. The procedures pendent responses during error correction were identical to Step 9 of the simple- resulted in the delivery of a sticker. conditional method. That is, a three-stimulus Most-to-least prompting. During Evaluations array conditional discrimination was targeted 2 and 3 for Shane, the experimenter changed from the onset of training. The S+ irregularly the prompting procedure from least-to-most alternated among the left, middle, and right prompting to most-to-least prompting for two positions. The presentation of each of the reasons. First, low levels of accuracy led to an sample stimuli was rotated based on procedures overall reduction in the number of reinforcers recommended by Green (2001). earned during the session, which appeared to Additional procedures. In some cases, the evoke problem behavior (e.g., aggression and simple-conditional and the conditional-only disruption). A prior functional analysis from methods were insufficient for teaching the Shane’s EIBI program indicated that his training sets. In those situations, we imple- problem behavior was maintained by access to mentedadditionalprocedures,andtheselection foodandtoys(i.e.,positivereinforcementinthe ofthoseprocedureswasbasedonwithin-session form of tangible items). Second, Shane allocat- patterns of responses during training. edahigherproportionofresponsestoparticular Repeated auditory stimulus presentations, addi- comparison positions during teaching, which tional observing response, and error correction. may have interfered with the acquisition of the Based on error patterns in the simple-condi- conditional discrimination. During the first tional method during Evaluations 2 and 3 for session of most-to-least prompting, the exper- Erin, we hypothesized that her responses were imenter physically guided Shane to select the influenced by the specific visual comparison correct comparison stimulus. Next, the exper- stimulusthatwastargetedineithertheprevious imenter interspersed least-to-most probes after phase or trial. Following the initial delivery every two most-to-least prompting sessions (a) of the sample stimulus, Erin was instructed to to determine if prompts could be faded during emit a vocal observing response (i.e., repeat the most-to-least prompting sessions and (b) to auditory sample stimulus) to ensure that she assess levels of independent responses across attendedtotheauditorysamplestimulus.Next, teaching sessions. The prompt that occasioned the experimenter presented the comparison the highest proportion of correct responses stimuli and re-presented the sample stimulus. during the least-to-most probe was used during 484 LAURA L. GROW et al. the two subsequent most-to-least prompting mentationofthefollowingduringeachtrial:(a) sessions. During least-to-most probes, observers each type of prompt, (b) the order in which collected data on the same dependent variables prompts occurred, (c) the response interval as those noted for the least-to-most sessions in followingprompts,(d)consequencesforcorrect the prior phase. Thus, observers collected data and incorrect responses, and (e) the procedure onindependentresponsesthatwerecomparable to facilitate an observing response. Observers across phases. collected additional integrity measures for Erin Alternative observing response. During Shane’s during Evaluations 2 and 3. These included the second evaluation, he rarely attended to the experimenter’s correct implementation of (a) visual stimuli without repeated experimenter the procedure to facilitate the vocal observ- prompts. To enhance independent attendance ing response, (b) additional deliveries of the to the visual comparison stimuli, the experi- auditory sample stimulus, and (c) the error- menter prompted an alternative observing correction procedure. Observers also collected response prior to presenting the sample and treatment integrity measures for Shane. These comparison stimuli. The experimenter present- included the experimenter’s correct implemen- ed the dark-colored piece of paper on top of a tation of (a) the alternative observing response trial sheet in an upright position at Shane’s eye (Evaluation 2) and (b) a most-to-least prompt- level. This presentation is different from the ingprocedure(Evaluations2and3).Atrialwas original format in which the experimenter scored as correct if all experimenter responses presented the trial sheet laid flat on the table. were implemented as specified by the research A least-to-most prompting procedure was used protocol. When calculating the integrity mea- to teach Shane to pull the dark-colored paper sures, a trial was scored as incorrect if any positioned in front of the trial sheet to expose treatment integrity measure was scored as the visual comparison stimuli. incorrect by either observer. The percentage of Maintenance. The experimenter conducted correctly implemented trials was calculated by baseline probes (identical to those described dividingthenumberofcorrecttrialsbythetotal above)3weeksaftermasteryofeachtrainingset. number of trials and converting the propor- The purpose of these probes was to assess tion to a percentage. Treatment integrity was whether participants’ acquisition of the three- assessed during 37% of sessions and averaged arrayconditionaldiscriminationwasmaintained. 98% (range, 78% to 100%) for Erin. For Because Shane did not meet mastery criteria for Shane, treatment integrity was assessed during the stimuli trained using the simple-conditional 35%ofsessionsandaveraged97%(range,78% method in the third evaluation, we did not to 100%). For Devin, treatment integrity was conduct a maintenance probe for these stimuli. assessed during 32% of sessions and averaged 97% (range, 78% to 100%). Point-by-point Design interobserver agreement was calculated for all An adapted alternating treatments design treatment integrity measures and averaged at (Sindelar, Rosenberg, & Wilson, 1985) was least 97% for each participant. usedtocomparediscrimination learningduring the simple-conditional and conditional-only Error Analysis methods. Two or three comparative evaluations We conducted several error analyses during were conducted for each participant. phases that required additional intervention components to meet the mastery criterion. We Treatment Integrity reviewed the data sheets and retrieved informa- Toassesstreatmentintegrity,anindependent tionthatallowedadetailedanalysisofresponses observer recorded the experimenter’s imple- within each teaching session. An analysis of

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.