ebook img

ERIC EJ931957: Infant Sign Training and Functional Analysis PDF

2011·0.16 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ931957: Infant Sign Training and Functional Analysis

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2011, 44, 305–314 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2011) INFANT SIGN TRAINING AND FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS MATTHEW P. NORMAND, MYCHAL A. MACHADO, KRISTIN M. HUSTYI, AND ALLISON J. MORLEY UNIVERSITYOFTHEPACIFIC We taught manual signs to typically developing infants using a reversal design and caregiver- nominatedstimuli.Wedeliveredthestimulionatime-basedscheduleduringbaseline.During theintervention,weusedprogressivepromptingandreinforcement,describedbyThompsonet al.(2004,2007),toestablishmands.Followingsigntraining,weconductedfunctionalanalyses andverifiedthatthesignsfunctionedasmands.Theseresultsprovidepreliminaryvalidationfor the verbal behavior functional analysis methodology and further evidence of the functional independence ofverbaloperants. Keywords: sign language, functional analysis, verbalbehavior _______________________________________________________________________________ Anumberofstudiespublishedinrecentyears However, the degree to which sign language report the success and potential benefits of taught under well-controlled conditions gener- teaching sign language to infants (e.g., Good- alizes to other settings has received relatively wyn&Acredolo,1993;Goodwyn,Acredolo,& little attention. Thompson et al. (2007) report- Brown, 2000). However, in many cases this ed that signing generalized across experimenters research has been limited by the absence of and settings, but they assessed only two systematic teaching procedures and the lack of participants, and the generalization sessions any direct observation of sign training. Recent- incorporatedthesameteachingproceduresused ly,Thompsonandcolleaguesdescribedeffective in the initial training. In addition, the experi- prompting and reinforcement procedures for menters did not identify the conditions under teaching sign language to infants (Thompson, which the sign would be expected to occur. Cotnoir-Bichelman, McKerchar, Tate, & Dan- According to Skinner’s (1957) taxonomy of cho,2007;Thompson,McKerchar,&Dancho, language, the basic unit of a verbal behavior 2004). The acquisition of sign language was analysisistheverbaloperant,describedinterms accompanied by reductions in problem behav- of the primary controlling variables over the ior (e.g., crying and whining) when sign verbal response form, be it manual or vocal. In training was combined with extinction for recent years, the clinical functional analysis problem behavior (Thompson et al., 2007). methodology first described by Iwata, Dorsey, These reports are important contributions to Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994) has the infant sign-language literature because of been modified for the purposes of evaluating their technological precision and sound exper- the function of emerging language in young imental methodology. children with autism and other developmental disabilities according to Skinner’s functional taxonomy (Kelley et al., 2007; LaFrance, We thank Holly White, Jennifer Moreno, Rodnesha Wilder, Normand, & Squires, 2009; Lerman Dunn, Dawn Howard, and the staff and management at et al., 2005; Normand, Severtson, & Beavers, the Garden of Eden Day Care Center for their assistance 2008). This methodology shows promise for inconducting thisstudy. AddresscorrespondencetoMatthewNormand,Depart- assessingtheestablishedfunctionofsignstaught mentofPsychology,UniversityofthePacific,3601Pacific to young children using general acquisition Ave., Stockton, California 95211 (e-mail: mnormand@ strategies such as those described by Thompson pacific.edu). doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-305 et al. (2004, 2007). A more complete under- 305 306 MATTHEW P. NORMAND et al. standing of the variables that evoke and Lerman et al., but they omitted the intraverbal maintain signs would enable a better under- condition andmodified the echoiccondition so standing of the conditions under which one as to be appropriate for a manual sign rather would expect the sign to occur, and would than a vocal response. suggest the operant functions to be targeted in The present study extends the existing future teaching sessions if certain functional research on sign-language training in infants analysis conditions did not evoke functional andyoungchildrenbyreplicatingtheprocedure communication. described by Thompson et al. (2007) and In their seminal report, Lerman et al. (2005) incorporating the trial-based verbal behavior described a functional analysis in which a functional analysis methodology described by number of test and control conditions corre- Kelley et al. (2007). Our goals were to test for sponding to Skinner’s taxonomy were used to generalization in controlled situations and to evaluatethefunctionofemergingspeechinfour assess the specific variables that controlled children with developmental disabilities. The signing after structured sign training. To our analysis was designed to test three of the knowledge, this is the first report of the verbal behavior functional analysis methodology fol- elementary verbal operants described by Skin- lowing specific functional communication ner: the mand, tact, and intraverbal. In training with typically developing children, addition, an echoic condition was introduced and only the second to target manual rather for one participant after undifferentiated re- than vocal language. sponding across the three primary test condi- tions. Each condition involved the manipula- tion of relevant antecedents and consequences, METHOD and the dependent measure was the number of Participants and Setting target responses observed per minute. The Three children participated. At the time of results suggested that the verbal operants were their initial participation, Julie and Ed were functionally independent and that the func- 8 months old and Yvonne was 15 months old. tional analysis distinguished among the various None of the participants exhibited any vocal functions. Moreover, the analysis most often language, but Ed’s mother reported that he identified a mand function for the target vocal would occasionally use informal sign language responses. (i.e., communicative hand gestures not used in Kelley et al. (2007) assessed the functions of American sign language [ASL]). Caregiver vocal speech with four young children with nominations of preferred food and tangible developmental disabilities using a trial-based items were used for the target response and procedure in which the dependent measure reinforcer selection. All three participants during each condition was the percentage of attended a local day-care center. Sessions for trials during which the target response was Julie were scheduled during the center’s normal observed. Despite the procedural modification, hours of operation. For Yvonne and Ed, the results indicated at least one clear function sessions were scheduled in their homes at a for each response assessed, similar to the results time mutually convenient for caregivers and reportedbyLermanetal.(2005).However,the experimenters. To avoid contrived periods of mand was not the most commonly identified food restriction or access, which might cause verbaloperant(cf. LaFranceet al., 2009).More distress on the part of the infant, all sessions recently, Normand et al. (2008) assessed the were scheduled at times that naturally followed function of a manual sign with a young boy periods of food deprivation (e.g., first thing in with autism using the procedure described by the morning, after nap time) or satiation (e.g., INFANT SIGN TRAINING 307 after a scheduled mealtime). All sessions were occurred during each interval. Interobserver 5 min in duration and were conducted in an agreement scores were scored for 89%, 73%, unoccupiedareaofthehomeorday-carecenter. and 59% of sessions for Julie, Yvonne, and Ed, Sessions were conducted one to three times per respectively. Mean agreement levels were 93% day,severaldays perweek,andwere videotaped (range, 80% to 100%) for Julie, 89% (range, for purposes of interobserver agreement and 67% to 100%) for Yvonne, and 89% (range, intervention integrity analyses. The local insti- 60% to 100%) for Ed. For the functional tutional review board approved all procedures. analysis sessions, the total number of trials on which the two observers agreed on the occur- Response Measurement and rence or nonoccurrence of an independent sign Interobserver Agreement was divided by the total number of trials scored During all sessions, observers recorded the and the quotient multiplied by 100%. Interob- number of independent and prompted signs server agreement scores were scored for 100%, exhibited by the participant. For all partici- 90%,and100%offunctionalanalysesforJulie, pants, independent signing was defined as Yvonne, and Ed, respectively. Mean agreement demonstrating the correct hand orientation levelswere100%forJulie,96%(range,90%to and hand movement in the absence of any 100%) for Yvonne, and 99% (range, 96% to prompts. A sign was scored as prompted if the 100%) for Ed. response occurred following any visual (i.e., experimenter modeling of the sign, experiment- Design and Procedure er showing the target item) or physical A reversal design was used to evaluate the assistance (i.e., hand-over-hand guidance) from effectsofsigntrainingwithallparticipants.The the experimenter. functional analysis conditions were arranged The target response for Julie was an informal according to a multielement design. sign for ‘‘applesauce,’’ defined as bringing the Initialbaseline.Thedesignatedreinforcerwas open palm of one hand to the crown of the delivered on a fixed-time 10-s schedule head. The target response for Ed was an (Thompson et al., 2004). Julie and Yvonne informal sign for ‘‘rattle,’’ defined as the palm received a bite of applesauce or pears (respec- of one hand hitting the thigh with a double tively) 10 s after consuming the previous bite. movement. The target response for Yvonne was Ed received 10 s of access to the rattle. a modified ASL sign (i.e., a communicative Reinforcer delivery was independent of the handgestureapproximatingbutnotmatchinga participant’s behavior during these sessions. formal ASL sign) for ‘‘pears,’’ defined as Sign training. After the initial baseline phase, bringing the palm of one hand to the nose. sign training was implemented for each partic- Interobserver agreement was calculated by ipant. For Julie and Yvonne, a model prompt having a second independent observer collect was delivered immediately at the beginning of data on independent and prompted signing each session and also according to a progressive either during the session or from a videotape delay schedule after consumption of the after the session. For sign training, each 5-min previously delivered reinforcer. If no signing session was divided into 1-min intervals, and occurred within 5 s of the model prompt, the the total number of intervals on which the two researcher physically prompted the target re- observers agreed was divided by the total sponse. The delay to model prompts was number of intervals scored and the dividend systematically increased across sessions on a multiplied by 100%. An agreement was scored progressive schedule (e.g., 0 s, 5 s, 10 s, and if both observers recorded independent or 20 s) until three consecutive sessions occurred prompted signing as having occurred or not with 10% or more of signs occurring indepen- 308 MATTHEW P. NORMAND et al. dently at the longest delay. The delay to the previously delivered reinforcer. Sessions physical prompts remained constant at 5 s continued until an increase in independent followingmodelprompts,andnopromptswere signs was observed. Booster sign-training ses- delivered once the participant had met the sions were conducted on days between func- response criterion at the longest delay. tional analysis sessions. For Ed, a verbal prompt (‘‘What do you Functional analysis. Three test and corre- want?’’) was delivered immediately at the sponding control conditions were arranged beginning of each session and also according according to Skinner’s (1957) functional tax- to the progressive delay schedule after removal onomy of verbal operants. Skinner described of the previously delivered reinforcer. If no the response form of a particular verbal operant signing occurred within 5 s of the verbal as occasioned by specific variables, either verbal prompt, the experimenter repeated the verbal or nonverbal, and maintained by specific or prompt while simultaneously holding the target generalized forms of reinforcement. The mand item within Ed’s sight line. If no signing response form, for example, occurs when an occurredwithin5softheverbal–visualprompt, organism is in a state of deprivation for a the experimenter modeled the target response. specific reinforcer, and receipt of thatreinforcer If no signing occurred within 5 s of the model contingent on the mand will strengthen similar prompt, the experimenter physically prompted response forms in the future, under similar Ed to perform the target response. The delay to conditions. The controlling variables for the verbal prompts was systematically increased mand, tact, and echoic or mimetic (manual across sessions (e.g., 0 s, 5 s, 10 s, and 20 s) equivalent to the echoic condition; Michael, until three consecutive sessions occurred with 2004; Vargas, 1986) are outlined in Table 1, 10% or more of signs occurring independently alongwiththespecifictestandcontrolvariables at the longest delay. The delay to verbal–visual, used in the functional analyses. Two test model, and physical prompts remained con- sessions were conducted for every one control, stant, and no prompts were delivered once the and the procedure was replicated twice for each participanthadmettheresponsecriterionatthe of the three verbal operants tested. The specific longest delay. arrangement of the conditions was similar to The targetreinforcer foreach participant was that described by Lerman et al. (2005), but a delivered immediately after all prompted and trial-based procedure was used (Kelley et al., independent signing. If the participant exhibit- 2007). However, unlike the Lerman et al. and ed an approximation of the target sign, the Kelley et al. arrangements, the mimetic condi- experimenterphysicallypromptedhimorherto tion was conducted for all participants. In complete the target sign correctly and then addition,theintraverbalconditionwasomitted, delivered the reinforcer. because the means by which one would select Return to baseline. Procedures were identical the appropriate antecedent verbal response to the initial baseline condition; however, the forms is unclear, making difficult any clear reinforcer schedule was yoked to half the interpretation if responding is not observed in interresponse time from the preceding sign- this condition. training phase. All functional analysis conditions were ar- Return to sign training (booster sessions). ranged to evaluate the function of the sign as a Procedures were similar to the initial sign- mand, tact, or mimetic. For the mand test training phase, except that the delay to each conditions, access to the target food item or subsequent model or verbal prompt was held rattle was restricted for 60 min prior to the constant at 5 s after consumption or removal of session,andaccesstothefooditemorrattlewas INFANT SIGN TRAINING 309 Table 1 Summary ofControlling Variablesfor the Targeted Verbal Operants Skinner’staxonomy Functionalanalysismethodology Operant Antecedent Consequence Condition Antecedents Consequence Mand Listenerplus Accesstothespecific Test 1hrdeprivation;listenerinclose Accesstothespecified deprivationfora reinforcer proximity;verbalorvisual reinforcer specificreinforcer prompt Control Freeaccesstospecifiedreinforcer; Noprogrammed listenerseatedacrossroom consequences Tact Listenerplus Generalized Test Sightofitem;listenerinclose Briefpraise nonverbal reinforcement proximity;verbalprompt stimulus (e.g.,praise) Control Itemremoved;listenerseated Noprogrammed acrossroom consequences Echoic Listenerplus Generalized Test Itemremoved;listenerinclose Briefpraise (mimetic) nonverbalstimulus reinforcement proximity;modelprompt (e.g.,praise) Control Itemremoved;listenerseated Noprogrammed acrossroom consequences provided following each instance of the target independent signing (Kelley et al., 2007). The sign. For example, Julie’s access to applesauce length of control sessions was yoked to the was restricted for 60 min prior to conducting a length of the corresponding test condition. The mand test session, and access to applesauce mand test conditions were 5 min in length (10 during the session was contingent on the trials), so the mand control conditions were occurrence of the target sign. For the mand 5 min in length (10 30-s intervals). control conditions, participants had free access tothetargetfooditemorrattlefor30minprior RESULTS to the session (i.e., they were placed in close proximity to the target item and were allowed Figure 1 shows the results of sign training, toconsumeormanipulatetheitemfor30min), booster sessions, and functional analysis for and there were no programmed consequences Julie. Julie did not sign during the initial for independent signing. baseline phase. Her frequency of independent Forthetacttestconditions,participants were signing reached the criterion to move to the given free access to the target food items or delayed model prompt in four sessions rattle for 30 min prior to and throughout the (20 min of training). Independent signing test condition. Brief verbal praise was delivered increased steadily across 32 sign-training following each instance of the target response. sessions (a total of 2.7 hr of sign training) For the tact control, mimetic test, and mimetic and decreased to zero within five sessions after control conditions, participants had free access reinstatement of baseline. When sign training to the target food items or rattle for 30 min was resumed, independent signing increased to prior to the session. The target food items or levels slightly higher than those observed rattlewasnotpresentduringthesesessions.The during the initial sign-training phase within experimenter provided no programmed conse- five sessions, and signing was maintained quencesforindependentsigningduringthetact during three booster sessions conducted be- and mimetic control conditions; independent tween functional analysis sessions. In total, the signing in the mimetic test conditions resulted experimenter conducted 48 sessions (4 hr of in brief verbal praise. training) with Julie. Test conditions consisted of 10 trials, with Julie signed most often during the mand test results expressed as the percentage of trials with sessions of the functional analysis, with a few 310 MATTHEW P. NORMAND et al. Figure1. Julie’stotalnumberofindependentsignsfor ‘‘applesauce’’ in each session during baseline (BL), sign Figure2. Yvonne’stotalnumberofindependentsigns training (ST), and booster training (Bst) (top). The for ‘‘pears’’ in each session during baseline (BL), sign percentageoftrialsduringwhichJuliesigned‘‘applesauce’’ training (ST), and booster training (Bst) (top). The during test and control sessions for each verbal operant percentage of trials during which Yvonne signed ‘‘pears’’ (bottom). during test and control sessions for each verbal operant (bottom). signs observed during the mimetic test sessions. No signing was observed during the mand higher levels of independent signing than those control, mimetic control, tact test, or tact observed in the initial sign-training sessions. control sessions (Figure 1, bottom). These Four subsequent baseline sessions produced results indicate that the sign for ‘‘applesauce’’ little to no signing. Sign training was again functioned as a mand and as a mimetic. reinstated, and rates of independent signing Figure 2 shows the results of sign training, comparable to the previous sign-training con- booster sessions, and functional analysis for ditionwereobservedwithinthreesessions.Two Yvonne. Yvonne did not sign during the initial booster sessions conducted on days between baseline condition. Her frequency of indepen- functional analysis sessions resulted in slightly dent signing reached the criterion to move to decreased rates of independent signing; howev- the delayed model prompt in five sessions er, these levels still exceeded baseline levels. In (25 min of training). Independent signing total, the experimenter conducted 38 sessions increased steadily during 13 sign-training (3.2 hr of training) with Yvonne. sessions (a total of 1.1 hr of sign training) and Yvonne signed often during all mand test increased during the initial return to baseline; sessions, with a few signs observed during the this was followed by an immediate decrease to mimetic test condition (Figure 2, bottom). No near-zero levels. Reinstatement of sign training responding was observed in the mand control, for 10 sessions (50 min of training) resulted in tact test, or tact control conditions. These INFANT SIGN TRAINING 311 signed during the mand test and mimetic test conditions and occasionally signed during the tact test condition (Figure 3, bottom). Al- though more variable than the other partici- pants, these results suggest that the sign for ‘‘rattle’’functionedasamandandasamimetic. DISCUSSION All participants learned to sign under the training conditions, with the frequency of signing much higher in the sign-training conditions than in baseline. Independent sign- ing was observed within a few brief training sessions for all participants. In addition, results provided support for some functional indepen- dence of the targeted verbal operants. That is, the functional analysis identified a specific condition or conditions that evoked signing. Figure 3. Ed’s total number of independent signs for The greatest amount of signing was observed in ‘‘rattle’’ineachsessionduringbaseline(BL),signtraining the mand test condition, with signing also (ST), and booster training (Bst) (top). The percentage of observed in the mimetic test condition. Signing trials during which Ed signed ‘‘rattle’’ during test and was almost never observed in the tact test controlsessions for each verbal operant(bottom). condition or in any of the control conditions. These results are consistent with previous results indicate that the sign for ‘‘pears’’ reports, in that young children (i.e., 8 to functioned as a mand and as a mimetic. 15 months old) quickly acquired signing skills Figure 3 shows the results of sign training, in a few training sessions (Thompson et al., booster sessions, and functional analysis for Ed. 2004, 2007) using a teaching procedure that Ed signed independently during the initial consistedofdelayedmodelandphysicalprompts baseline, but independent signing decreased to combinedwithreinforcement(Thompsonetal., zero within four sessions. His frequency of 2007). The functional analysis results support independent signing reached the criterion to previousresearch,inthatthesignsoccurredonly move to the delayed model prompt in four underspecifictestconditions(Kelleyetal.,2007; sessions (20 min of training). Independent LaFrance et al., 2009; Lerman et al., 2005; signing steadily increased during 19 sign- Normandetal.,2008),andrespondingoccurred training sessions (a total of 95 min of training) mostofteninthemandcondition(Lermanetal., and then decreased to near zero within three 2005).However,unlikeinthesepreviousstudies, sessions during the return to baseline. After the experimenters in the current study both reinstatement of sign training, independent taught the manual signs and conducted the signing increased to levels higher than those functionalanalysis,withsimilarsettingsusedfor observed during the initial sign-training phase. training and testing. Thus, one might be more Intotal,theexperimenterconducted32sessions likely to see clearly differentiated responding (2.7 hr of training) with Ed. during the functional analysis, as opposed to The results from Ed’s functional analyses are situationsinwhichthetargetverbalresponsewas less clear than those of Julie and Yvonne. Ed establishedundersomewhatdifferentconditions 312 MATTHEW P. NORMAND et al. and with different individuals than those experimental and test conditions arranged involved in the functional analysis. That re- according to Skinner’s (1957) taxonomy, all sponding also was observed in the mimetic test such results have been reported with young condition,albeitlessfrequentlythaninthemand children with autism or other developmental test condition, might be explained by the disabilities (Kelley et al., 2007; LaFrance et al., promptingproceduresusedduringsigntraining. 2009; Lerman et al., 2005; Normand et al., As a result of systematic model prompting, the 2008). infants had the opportunity to sign under Given the potential generality of the meth- conditions relevant both to mand and mimetic odology, it might be a useful tool for the functions. These training procedures might longitudinal study of both typical and atypical establish multiple verbal operants simultaneous- language development. For example, ongoing ly, an outcome that could be beneficial in sign- descriptive analyses of child and caregiver trainingprograms.Futureresearchisnecessaryto interactions could be conducted (e.g., Moerk, evaluate this possibility. 1990;Vollmer,Borrero,Wright,VanCamp,& These results extend previous research in Lalli, 2001) in combination with repeated several ways. First, the present study served as a functional analyses conducted at regular inter- preliminary validation of the verbal behavior vals. The results of these descriptive analyses of functional analysis methodology in that the verbal behavior could be used to inform the signs were taught under specific stimulus arrangement of functional analysis conditions. conditions (i.e., as mands) and the functional Likewise, the results of the functional analysis analysis subsequently identified a mand func- could inform the interpretation of possible tionforthesigns.Thisisthefirstreportedstudy behavioral contingencies suggested by the inwhichverbaloperantswereestablishedbythe results of the descriptive analysis. These types experimentersandthenassessedusingtheverbal of arrangements could inform both basic and behavior functional analysis methodology first applied research on language development. describedbyLermanetal.(2005).AsNormand These areas are promising avenues for future et al. (2008) noted, the functional analysis research. Similar methods that involve descrip- methodology developed by Iwata et al. (1982/ tive and functional analyses of verbal behavior 1994) has been validated by a sizable research also could be used to inform clinical practice if literature that has reported successful clinical results are used to track progress through interventions based on the results of functional language acquisition programs and to identify analyses. This kind of empirical validation is deficits in functional communication. Perhaps not generally applicable to verbal behavior more important, a more complete understand- analyses. A viable alternative to treatment ingofthetypesofstimuluscontrolproducedby evaluations might be to establish novel verbal language acquisition procedures could lead to operants under specific sources of stimulus more effective and efficient teaching technolo- control and then assess function using the gies. Further research on verbal behavior functional analysis methodology. The results functional analyses seems to be a promising reported herein constitute such a validation. tool for such an endeavor. Second, this is the first report of the verbal Third, the current study is one of only a few behavior functional analysis methodology used empirical demonstrations of functional inde- with typically developing individuals in general pendence among verbal operants exhibited by andinfantsinparticular.Althoughanumberof typically developing individuals (cf. Lamarre & studies have demonstrated that this methodol- Holland, 1985). The demonstration of func- ogy produces differential responding across tionally independent verbal operants is difficult INFANT SIGN TRAINING 313 in typically developing populations, because used in the present study (and in each of the language acquisition occurs quickly and multi- verbal behavior functional analysis studies ple sources of stimulus control are readily published to date) might be limited are that established. Early sign training offers a unique (a) a nonverbal stimulus was absent in the tact opportunity to capture early language develop- control condition, and (b) verbal discriminative ment in typically developing infants and stimuli similar to those arranged in the test toddlers and might also be applied to very early conditions were absent in the tact and mimetic vocal development in this same population. A (echoic) control conditions. Future studies number of inferences concerning language could address these concerns by incorporating development can be made based on Skinner’s nonverbal stimuli that are irrelevant to the (1957)verbalbehavior analysis.One exampleis target response into the tact control condition the claim that mands, because they are the only and delivering irrelevant verbal prompts during verbaloperantthatdirectlybenefitsthespeaker, the tact and mimetic control conditions on developpriortoanyotheroftheverbaloperants schedules yoked to those in the test conditions in early language learners (Bijou & Baer, 1965; (similar to the arrangement of the intraverbal Lerman et al., 2005; Novak, 1996; Skinner, control condition reported by Lerman et al., 1957; Sundberg, 2007; Sundberg & Michael, 2005). 2001). Although this is a plausible hypothesis, Future research could focus on several other no strong empirical support for it exists in the areas. First, the sign-language acquisition pro- literature. Functional analyses could be used to cedures reported thus far used delayed physical evaluatesuchclaimsempiricallybyassessingthe ormodelpromptstoestablishsigning(Thomp- variables that control the vocal or nonvocal son et al., 2004,2007).A procedure potentially language observed in very young children as as or more effective might be to identify a they begin to develop functional communica- salient motor response already exhibited by the tion skills. infant and then to shape a clear manual sign. In addition to the strengths noted above, Second, more attention should be given to the several limitations of the current study warrant degree of generalization that results from the attention. With respect to the sign-training reported sign-teaching procedures, especially as procedures, the signs were taught only under assessed in natural settings with caregivers, and mand conditions. Future research should eval- the variables that influence such generalization uate the degree to which other verbal operants should be systematically investigated. Third, to (e.g., tacts or intraverbals) could be established establish further the validity of the verbal with infants and young toddlers. Also, the most behavior functional analysis methodology, an relevant controlling variable (food deprivation) experimenter who is blind to the specific was only loosely controlled during training. conditions under which the sign was taught Although this seems not to have affected should conduct the functional analysis as a acquisition adversely, more careful control over means of increasing its internal validity. the relevant variables for each operant being In summary, the results reported herein taught might improve acquisition rates or demonstrate that typically developing infants generalization. and young toddlers can be taught to sign under With respect to the verbal behavior func- conditions that correspond to those described tional analysis, perhaps the most pressing by Skinner (1957) as controlling the mand and question is the degree to which the control mimetic (echoic). In addition, the verbal conditions were adequately arranged. Two behavior functional analysis methodology pro- likely ways in which the control conditions duced differential signing across the test and 314 MATTHEW P. NORMAND et al. control conditions, evoking the most signing in Lerman,D.C.,Parten,M.,Addison,L.R.,Vorndran,C. M., Volkert, V. M., & Kodak, T. (2005). A mandtestconditionsandsomewhatlesssigning methodology for assessing the functions of emerging in mimetic test conditions, thus providing one speech in children with developmental disabilities. of the few demonstrations of functional inde- JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 38,303–316. Michael, J. L. (2004). Concepts and principles of behavior pendence among verbal operants in typically analysis (rev. ed.). Kalamazoo, MI: Society for the developing children. The results of the func- Advancement ofBehavior Analysis. tional analysis also suggest that the methodol- Moerk,E.L.(1990).Three-term contingencypatternsin ogy might be a useful tool for investigating mother-childinteractionsduringfirstlanguageacqui- sition.JournaloftheExperimentalAnalysisofBehavior, language development and for guiding lan- 54, 293–305. guage-acquisition programs. Normand, M. P., Severtson, E. S., & Beaver, G. A. (2008). A functional analysis of nonvocal verbal behaviorofayoungchildwithautism.TheAnalysisof REFERENCES VerbalBehavior,24,63–67. Novak, G. (1996). Developmental psychology. Reno, NV: Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1965). Child development: ContextPress. II. Universal stage of infancy. New York: Appleton- Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Century-Crofts. Appleton-Century-Crofts. Goodwyn, S. W., & Acredolo, L. P. (1993). Symbolic Sundberg, M. L. (2007). Verbal behavior. In J. O. gestureversusword:Isthereamodalityadvantagefor Cooper, T. E. Heron, & W. L. Heward (Eds.), onset of symbol use? Child Development, 64, Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed., pp. 526–547). 688–701. Upper SaddleRiver, NJ:Merrill/Prentice Hall. Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. (2000). Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of Impact of symbolic gesturing on early language Skinner’sanalysisofverbalbehaviorforchildrenwith development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, autism.BehaviorModification,25,698–724. 81–103. Thompson, R. H., Cotnoir-Bichelman, N. M., Iwata,B.A.,Dorsey,M.F.,Slifer,K.J.,Bauman,K.E., McKerchar, P. M., Tate, T. L., & Dancho, K. A. & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional (2007). Enhancing early communication through analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior infant sign training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 197–209.(Reprinted from Analysis and Analysis, 40, 15–23. Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, Thompson, R. H., McKerchar, P. M., & Dancho, K. A. 1982) (2004). The effects of delayed physical prompts and Kelley, M. E., Shillingsburg, M. A., Castro, M. J., reinforcement on infant sign language acquisition. Addison, L. R., LaRue, R. H., & Martins, M. P. JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 37,379–383. (2007).Assessmentofthefunctionsofvocalbehavior Vargas,E.A.(1986).Intraverbalbehavior.InP.N.Chase in children with developmental disabilities: A repli- &L.J.Parrott(Eds.),Psychologicalaspectsoflanguage cation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, (pp.128–151).Springfield, IL: Charles CThomas. 571–576. Vollmer,T.R.,Borrero,J.C.,Wright,C.S.,VanCamp, LaFrance, D., Wilder, D. A., Normand, M. P., & C., & Lalli, J. S. (2001). Identifying possible Squires, J. L. (2009). Extending the assessment of contingencies during descriptive analyses of severe functions of vocalizations in children with limited behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior verbalrepertoires.TheAnalysisofVerbalBehavior,25, Analysis, 34, 269–287. 19–32. Lamarre, J., & Holland, J. G. (1985). The functional Received May 7,2010 independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the Final acceptance September 20,2010 Experimental Analysisof Behavior, 43, 5–19. Action Editor,Rachel Thompson

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.