ebook img

ERIC EJ931955: Using Multiple-Exemplar Training to Teach a Generalized Repertoire of Sharing to Children with Autism PDF

2011·0.54 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ931955: Using Multiple-Exemplar Training to Teach a Generalized Repertoire of Sharing to Children with Autism

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2011, 44, 279–294 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2011) USING MULTIPLE-EXEMPLAR TRAINING TO TEACH A GENERALIZED REPERTOIRE OF SHARING TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH, SHARON A. REEVE, AND KENNETH F. REEVE CALDWELLCOLLEGE AND DAWN B. TOWNSEND INSTITUTEFOREDUCATIONALACHIEVEMENT The current study examined the utility of multiple-exemplar training to teach children with autismtoshare.Stimulifrom3of4categoriesweretrainedusingatreatmentpackageofvideo modeling,prompting,andreinforcement.Offerstoshareincreasedforall3childrenfollowing theintroductionoftreatment,withevidenceofskillmaintenance.Inaddition,within-stimulus- category generalization of sharing was evident for all participants, although only 1 participant demonstrated across-category generalization of sharing. Offers to share occurred in a novel setting, with familiar and novel stimuli, and in the presence of novel adults and peers for all participants during posttreatment probes. Keywords: autism,generalization,multiple-exemplartraining,socialbehavior,sharing,video modeling _______________________________________________________________________________ Prosocial behavior refers to a broad class of ties for positive social interactions with peers responses that includes smiling, cooperating, andisanintegralcomponentofinteractiveplay. taking turns, making friends, expressing empa- Severalstudiesillustratesuccessfulprocedures thy, helping others, and sharing (Barton & for teaching children of typical development Ascione, 1979; Bryant & Budd, 1984; Chan- how to share with their peers. Warren et al. dler, Lubeck, & Fowler, 1992; Cooke & (1976) for example, used modeling and rein- Apolloni, 1976; Reeve, Reeve, Townsend, & forcement to increase offers to share and Poulson, 2007; Rogers-Warren & Baer, 1976; corresponding acceptances of share offers in Warren,Rogers-Warren,&Baer,1976).Bryant two groups of preschool children with reported andBudd(1984)suggestedthatsharingmaybe generalization to a new setting. Barton and an especially important social behavior for Ascione(1979)examinedthegeneralizationand young children because it increases opportuni- durability of sharing by teaching preschool children to share verbally (e.g., ‘‘here you go’’), physically (e.g., handing a toy), or both. This study was conducted as partial fulfillment of the Physical sharing increased for all children requirements for the first author’s MA degree in applied during training, but it generalized and main- behavioranalysisfromCaldwellCollege.Portionsofthese tained only for children who were taught to datawerealsopresentedatthe34thannualconventionof the Association for Behavior Analysis, May, 2008, in share verbally. Verbal sharing increased during Chicago.WethankthestudentsandstaffattheY.A.L.E. training only for children who were taught to Schoolfor their participation. share verbally or both verbally and physically; Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sharon A. Reeve, Department of Applied however, verbal sharing did not generalize Behavior Analysis, Caldwell College, 120 Bloomfield across settings. Bryant and Budd (1984) Avenue, Caldwell, New Jersey 07006 (e-mail: sreeve@ extended this training package to six preschool- caldwell.edu). doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-279 ers with disabilities, focusing on teaching 279 280 DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH et al. specificcomponentsofsharingresponses:offers, children with autism, multiple-exemplar train- requests,andacceptances.Allthreecomponents ing has been used to teach helping behavior of sharing increased after implementation of (Reeve et al., 2007), empathy (Schrandt, instructions, modeling, and behavior rehearsals. Townsend, & Poulson, 2009), and appropriate Compared to children of typical develop- affect (Gena, Krantz, McClannahan, & Poul- ment, children with autism often display son, 1996), among others. especially marked impairments in sharing Stimulithatadequatelyreflectthediversityof (Baron-Cohen,Leslie,&Frith,1985;Eisenberg the stimulus characteristics likely to be present & Fabes, 1998; Rheingold & Hay, 1980; under generalization conditions are used during Rutter, 1978; Volkmar, Carter, Sparrow, & multiple-exemplar training. The identification Cicchetti, 1993; Wing, 1988). Despite these of these stimuli by the teacher is known as a deficits, few studies have examined methods to general case analysis (Engelmann & Carnine, teach children with autism to share. Sawyer, 1982; Horner, Sprague, & Wilcox, 1982; Luiselli, Ricciardi, and Gower (2005) increased Sprague & Horner, 1984). When responding verbal and physical sharing in one child with comesunderthecontrolofthestimulusfeatures autism through the use of priming before play present in the exemplars used during training, sessions, along with in-session prompts and the presentation of novel stimuli that share reinforcement;however,theydidnotassessskill these features should also occasion the same generalization. Recently, DeQuinzio, Town- response. send, and Poulson (2008) used a forward The purpose of the present study was to chaining procedure to teach children with extend the findings of DeQuinzio et al. (2008) autism to approach a peer and emit a show- by establishing a generalized repertoire of give-play sharing response chain with multiple sharing in four children with autism. A toy exemplars. Sharing of toys increased for all multiple-exemplar teaching procedure similar participants and generalized to nontrained toys, to that of Reeve et al. (2007) was used. novel locations, and different peers. However, Specifically, children were taught to share items DeQuinzioetal.conductedtrainingandtesting from multiple classesof materials(artmaterials, with only one class of materials (toys), so it is snack foods, toys, and gym materials), and not clear whether sharing could be successfully generalization was assessed both within classes taught using other classes of materials or of materials and across nontaught classes of whether sharing would generalize to untrained materials. In addition, teaching was conducted classes of materials. in different settings to promote generalization Because the utility of a behavior-change acrosslocations,anddiscriminationofnonshar- procedure is increased when responding gener- ingsituationswastargetedandassessed.Finally, alizes across persons, settings, and materials we assessed whether sharing was under appro- (Stokes & Baer, 1977; Stokes & Osnes, 1989), priate stimulus control by sampling situations it is important to implement procedures that and stimuli for when sharing was appropriate promote generalization. One strategy to accom- and situations in which it was not. Also similar plish this involves the teaching of multiple to Reeve et al., a combination of video exemplars. Multiple-exemplar training has been modeling and prompting was used for error used to teach numerous skills to individuals correction. Video modeling provided a means with moderate to severe disabilities, including to standardize the presentation of the models appropriate vending machine use (Sprague & andhasbeenshowntopromoteacquisitionand Horner, 1984) and setting and clearing tables generalization of skills for children with autism (Horner, Eberhard, & Sheehan, 1986). For (Krantz, MacDuff, Wadstrom, & McClanna- TEACHING SHARING 281 han, 1991; LeBlanc et al., 2003; Nikopoulos & ing machine. Pre- and posttreatment sessions Keenan, 2004; Taylor, Levin, & Jasper, 1999). were conducted in the usual daily classroom to assess generalization of sharing. Materials and target responses for the study METHOD were selected based on data collected with first- Participants through third-grade general and special educa- Four children with a diagnosis of autism tion students during a cumulative 2 hr of participated. They attended a private school for observations in public elementary schools individuals with autism and had received their conductedduringfreeplay,snacktime,outdoor diagnoses through independent agencies prior recess, and gym. Materials used included to school enrollment. Direct observation and modeling clay, crayons, stamps, jump rope, parentorteacherreportsconductedpriortothe balls, puppets, pretend food, blocks, small start of the study indicated that none of the plastic animals, books, cars, and snacks. The participantsengagedinsharingintheclassroom majority of the observed sharing statements or at home. Steven was 8 years 1 month old, were requests or directions to share another Isaac was 7 years 10 months old, Bobby was student’s materials (e.g., ‘‘want one?,’’ ‘‘here 8 years 1 month old, and Aiden was 7 years you go,’’ ‘‘wanna play?,’’ ‘‘your turn,’’ and 6 months old. Each participant had previous ‘‘here’’).Theclassroomteacherswerealsoasked experience learning skills with discrete-trial what types of materials the participants were instruction and using token-based motivational likely to use during free time and group time, systems. According to reports by their teachers, when offering to share would be appropriate. each participant also demonstrated the prereq- The four stimulus categories were art mate- uisite verbal skills needed to make the target rials, snack foods, toys, and gym materials. vocal responses used in the present study (e.g., Stimulus categories and items assigned to each ‘‘Want to try it?’’ ‘‘You try it’’), displayed participant are listed in Table 1. In each of correct imitation of various verbal and motor these four categories, five different materials responses using both in vivo and video models, served as multiple exemplars of the same and followed simple directions to engage in stimulus category. For example, art materials specified activities. included crayons, dot paint, markers, colored Setting and Materials pencils,andglitterpens.SnackfoodsforSteven, Experimental sessions were conducted in a Bobby, and Aiden included pretzels, cookies, school office that was not used for the chips, candy, and marshmallows. Specific snack participants’ daily education. The office con- foods were different for Isaac because he tained a desk and three chairs, bookshelf, filing displayed disruptive behavior in response to cabinet, computer, printer, and telephone. the removal of the original snacks (which were Additional materials included all relevant stim- highly preferred) during the initial treatment ulus materials (i.e., toys, gym equipment, art session. To reduce disruption, snack foods for materials, and snacks), a portable DVD player, Isaac included carrots, celery, apples, and small audio voice recorders, a token board, a raisins. During pre- and postintervention gen- video camera with tripod, and data sheets. eralization probes, however, snack stimuli for Approximately every 2 weeks, experimental Isaac were the same as those used for the other sessions were conducted in the school’s kitchen three participants. Toys included Lego blocks, to promote generalization. The kitchen con- cars, magnets, Play-Doh, and Peg-Board. Gym tainedtworoundtableswith fivechairsateach, materials included a Velcro mitt and ball, stove, sink, refrigerator, dishwasher, and vend- basketball, scooter, Hippity-Hop, and Velcro 282 DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH et al. Table 1 Assignment of StimuliandVerbal Responses AcrossParticipants Steven Isaac Bobby Aiden Teaching Art: Snack: Toys: Gym: crayons celery magnets Hippity-Hop dotpaint apples Play-Doh Velcromittandball markers raisins Legoblocks magneticdarts Snack: Toys: Gym: Art: pretzels cars scooter crayons cookies magnets Hippity-Hop coloredpencils chips Play-Doh Velcromittandall dotpaint Toys: Gym: Art: Snack: Legoblocks basketball markers candy cars scooter coloredpencils pretzels magnets Velcromittandball crayons cookies Within-category Art:coloredpencils Snack:carrots Art:dotpaint Art:markers probe Snack:candy Toys:Legoblocks Gym:basketball Snack:chips Toys:Play-Doh Gym:Hippity-Hop Toys:cars Gym:scooter Across-category Gym:scooter Art:crayons Snack:chips Toys:cars probe Verbalresponses ‘‘Wouldyouliketotrythis?’’ ‘‘Trythis.’’ ‘‘Whydon’tyoutry?’’ ‘‘Here,youtryit.’’ ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ ‘‘Trythis.’’ ‘‘Wouldyouliketotrythis?’’ ‘‘Here,youtryit.’’ ‘‘Here,youtryit.’’ ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ ‘‘Trythis.’’ Pre-and Snack: Snack: Snack: Snack: posttreatment marshmallows(G) marshmallows(G) chips(P) marshmallows(G) generalization candy(P) candy(P) marshmallows(G) chips(P) trialsa cookies(T) pretzels(T) pretzels(G) candy(T) Art: Art: Art: Art: glitterpens(G) crayons(P) glitterpens(G) glitterpens(G) coloredpencils(P) glitterpens(G) dotpaint(P) markers(P) markers(T) markers(G) crayons(T) dotpaint(T) Gym: Gym: Gym: Gym: scooter(P) Velcrodarts(G) Velcrodarts(G) Velcrodarts(G) basketball(G) Hippity-Hop(P) basketball(P) magneticdarts(P) Velcrodarts(G) Velcromittandball(T) Hippity-Hop(T) Hippity-Hop(T) Toys: Toys: Toys: Toys: Peg-Board(G) Peg-Board(G) Peg-Board(G) cars(P) Play-Doh(P) Legoblocks(P) cars(P) Peg-Board(G) Legoblocks(T) cars(T) Play-Doh(T) magnets(G) aIncludesstimuliusedonlyduringpre-andposttreatmentgeneralizationtrials(G),probetrialsduringexperimental sessions (P), andteaching sessions (T). darts. One exemplar in each category was natural settings, no assessment was made randomly selected for use in pre- and posttreat- regarding the participants’ relative preferences ment generalization probes only (i.e., glitter for any of the materials. pens, marshmallows, peg board, and Velcro darts). In addition, materials that served as Video Models discriminative stimuli for nonsharing responses The video models used to teach sharing were present, including papers to put in a depicted two peers sharing an activity shown backpack,anacademicworksheet,booksonthe from a third-person viewpoint (i.e., scene floorthatneededtobeplacedonatable,adirty perspective). Specifically, a 7-year-old boy was tabletop and a towel, and an article of clothing. first shown either sitting at a table or standing, It should be noted that although the materials engaged in an activity. A second 7-year-old boy used for sharing and nonsharing trials were all then came on camera, approached the first boy, selected based on observations of peers in and stood within 0.6 m of him. Following the TEACHING SHARING 283 Table 2 Video Model Types AcrossParticipants Videomodel Verbalresponse Stimulussets Steven ‘‘Wouldyouliketotrythis?’’ cookies,cars ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ markers,crayons ‘‘Trythis.’’ magnets,chips Isaac ‘‘Trythis.’’ magnets,chips ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ Velcromittandball,pretzel ‘‘Here,youtryit.’’ ball,Play-Doh Bobby ‘‘Whydon’tyoutry?’’ coloredpencils,Velcromittandball ‘‘Trythis.’’ Play-Doh,scooter ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ magnets,markers Aiden ‘‘Here,youtryit.’’ football,coloredpencils ‘‘Wouldyouliketotrythis?’’ cookies,cars ‘‘Doyouwanttotry?’’ pretzels,Velcromittandball approach of the second boy, the first boy held correct nonsharing response was scored if the outhis item and emitteda verbal offer to share. child responded on a nonsharing trial by Sixvideomodelswereusedforeachparticipant, completing the task without offering to share, and each video model was approximately 8 to eitherphysicallyorverbally.Table 2depictsthe 10 s in duration. Stimuli featured in each video randomized assignment of verbal responses model corresponded to stimuli used during taught to participants. training trials for that particular participant. A teacher with master’s level training in Similarly, verbal offers to share emitted by the behavior analysis served as a second observer peer in the video model were verbal responses and scored sharing and nonsharing either in assigned to that particular participant during vivo or via videotapes of a session. For each treatment. Table 2 provides a list of the stimuli trial, the scoring of the two observers was and responses featured in the video models for compared, and only an exact match was each participant. For example, one of Steven’s considered an agreement. Percentage agreement video models depicted a peer coloring with was calculated by dividing the number of crayons who asked, ‘‘Do you want to try?’’ in agreements by the number of agreements plus response to the approach of the second peer. disagreements and multiplying by 100%. Interobserver agreement data were collected on Response Measurement the dependent variable during 54% of Steven’s Trainedobservers scoredparticipantrespons- sessions, 44% of Isaac’s sessions, 47% of es on a trial-by-trial basis using pencils and Bobby’s sessions, and 40% of Aiden’s sessions. paper data sheets. A correct offer to share was The mean percentage of agreement across all scored only if both the motor and verbal trial types (training, probe, and nonsharing component occurred. A correct motor response trials) and across all participants was 99% consistedoftheparticipantholdingoutanitem (session range, 97% to 100%). A trained to the experimenter within 5 s of her approach observer also collected data on the accuracy of to within 0.6 m of the participant. A correct the experimenter’s presentation of discrimina- verbal response consisted of a request for the tive stimuli, reinforcement, trial types, and experimenter to engage in an activity with the video model error correction. These procedural presented item (e.g., ‘‘Do you want to try?’’; integrity data were collected during 95% of all ‘‘Here, you try it’’) or an approximation of the baseline, treatment, maintenance, and pre- and response (e.g., ‘‘Do you want it?’’; ‘‘try’’). A posttreatment sessions. The mean percentage 284 DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH et al. accuracy on procedural components was 99% promote response generalization, three of the (session range, 98% to 100%). In addition, a five experimenter-defined verbal offers to share second observer scored procedural integrity for were randomly assigned to each participant and 46% of Steven’s sessions, 34% of Isaac’s were rotated across trials. At the start of each sessions, 36% of Bobby’s sessions, and 34% of trial, the experimenter approached and stood Aiden’s sessions. The mean percentage of within 0.6 m of the participant while he was agreement on the accurate presentation of engaged in a specific activity. The purpose of procedural components was 99% (session presenting these discriminative stimuli was to range, 98% to 100%). set the occasion for the participant to offer to share the item or items with which he was Design and Assignment of Categories engaged. Finally, five nonsharing discrimina- A concurrent multiple-probe design across tion trials were interspersed among the nine participantswasused.Threeofthefourpossible teaching and four probe trials. To reduce stimuluscategorieswereassignedtothetraining potential order or sequence effects, the order condition for each participant; the fourth ofthetrialswasrandomized,withtheexception category was used to assess across-category thatallsessionsbeganandendedwithatraining generalization of sharing responses (see Ta- trial. ble 1). Each category contained five possible Baseline.Theparticipantwasseatedatadesk. stimulus exemplars: Three were directly target- The experimenter presented stimulus materials ed as stimulus exemplars (partially counterbal- for that trial (e.g., crayons and a blank coloring anced across participants), a fourth exemplar page) with a verbal direction to engage in the was used to assess within-category generaliza- targetactivity(e.g.,‘‘colorthepicture’’).Within tionof sharing during trainingsessions, andthe 5sofpresentingthematerials,theexperimenter fifthexemplarwasusedtofurtherassess within- approached the participant and stood within and across-category generalization during the 0.6 m of him. If the participant emitted a pre-andpostinterventiongeneralizationprobes. correct verbal and motor offer to share, the For example, Steven’s teaching trials included experimenter responded ‘‘sure’’ or ‘‘thanks,’’ crayons, dot paint, and markers (art materials took the offered item, and manipulated the category); pretzels, cookies, and chips (snack itemappropriately.Nopromptsormodelswere foods category); and Lego blocks, cars, and provided, and no tokens were delivered for magnets (toy category). Probe trials for Steven sharing.Theexperimenterdeliveredtokensona included a scooter (gym materials category) to variable-ratio (VR) 2 schedule for following the assess across-category generalization, colored direction to engage in the task and attending to pencils to assess within-category generalization the experimenter. The experimenter did not forartmaterials,candytoassesswithin-category respondiftheparticipantemittedonlyacorrect generalizationforsnackfoods,andPlay-Dohto motor response or a correct verbal response. If assess within-category generalization for toys. the participant did not emit a correct response Procedure within 5 s of the presentation of the discrim- Eachbaselineandtreatmentsessionconsisted inative stimuli, the experimenter removed the of 18 trials. Of these, nine were teaching trials materials and walkedaway. After a 5-s intertrial (threeexemplarsfromeachofthethreetraining interval, the experimenter presented the partic- categories). Four additional trials were general- ipant with the next activity and a new trial ization probes with the fourth exemplar from began. At the conclusion of a session, partici- each of the three training categories and one pants could trade in their tokens for previously exemplarfromthefourthstimuluscategory.To selected activities or materials (e.g., access to a TEACHING SHARING 285 video game, preferred snacks or toys) that were for that trial a second time and approached and not used during the sessions. stood within 0.6 m of the participant. If the Treatment. The steps of the procedure are participant offered to share within 5 s, the depicted in Figure 1. As in baseline, the experimenter accepted the offered item; re- experimenter presented the participant with sponded with ‘‘yes,’’ ‘‘sure,’’ or ‘‘thank you’’; stimulus materials, gave the direction to engage manipulated the item appropriately; and deliv- inthetargetactivity,waited5s,approachedthe ered a token to the participant’s token board. If participant, and stood within 0.6 m of him. the participant did not offer to share within 5 s Following a correct sharing response during of the second opportunity to do so, the training trials, the experimenter accepted the experimenter provided a physical prompt by offered item; confirmed the offer to share by using hand-over-hand guidance to assist the saying ‘‘yes,’’ ‘‘sure,’’ or ‘‘thank you’’; manip- child in handing the items with which he was ulated the offered item appropriately; and then engaged to the experimenter. Simultaneously, delivered a token. After appropriately manipu- the experimenter activated a voice recorder that lating the item and returning it to the emittedarecordingofthetargetvocalresponse. participant, the experimenter walked away and If the participant emitted only one component gave the direction to clean up. Materials were of the response (e.g., physical or vocal), the removed from the table and the trial ended. A experimenter delivered the corresponding new trial began following a 5-s intertrial promptfortheabsentcomponent.Forexample, interval. if the child handed an item to the experimenter If the participant did not respond within 5 s but did not emit a target vocal response, the of presentation of the discriminative stimuli, experimenter provided only an audio prompt responded incorrectly, or emitted only one by activating the voice recorder. If the child component of the sharing response (e.g., motor emitted a target vocal response but did not response or vocal response alone), the experi- physically offer to share, the experimenter menterremovedthematerialsandimplemented provided only a physical prompt with hand- an error-correction procedure similar to that over-hand guidance. The physical and auditory described by Reeve et al. (2007). First, the prompts were repeated until the participant experimenter presented a video model of a emitted the correct response independently. scenario in which appropriate sharing was As noted previously, generalization probe depicted. Video models were used only when trials and nonsharing trials were interspersed a participant failed to emit appropriate sharing with training trials. During probe trials, the behavior during training trials. For ease of procedure was identical to that during baseline. implementation and to promote generalization During nonsharing trials, the experimenter of sharing, the video models did not necessarily presented the nonsharing stimuli to the partic- show the same activity in which the participant ipants and emitted an instruction appropriate was engaged during that trial. For example, a for those stimuli (e.g., ‘‘Can you please put the videomodeldepictingapeereatingcookiesand books on the shelf?’’; ‘‘Can you please wipe the asking ‘‘Would you like to try this?’’ could be table?’’; ‘‘Put the papers in your backpack’’). presented to Steven during a trial in which he Whentheparticipantcompletedthenonsharing was playing with cars and the target response task (e.g., placed papers in a backpack), the was ‘‘Would you like to try this?’’ experimenter delivered a token and the trial None of the participants required prompting ended. If a child emitted a sharing response to watch the video. When the video model (e.g.,‘‘Wanttotry?’’)duringanonsharingtrial, ended,theexperimenterpresentedthematerials the experimenter paused for 5 s and then 286 DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH et al. Figure 1. Instructional procedure for sharing trialsduring treatment. TEACHING SHARING 287 presented the same nonsharing trial a second ule, the posttreatment generalization probe time while verbally instructing the child to trials were conducted. The schedule of rein- complete the task. This continued until the forcement for Aiden was not systematically child completed the task and did not emit a thinned due to his completion of participation sharing response. These nonsharing trials were at the end of the school year. included to ensure that the participants attend- Pre- and posttreatment generalization probes. ed to therelevant discriminative stimuli that set The experimenter conducted probes assessing the occasion to offer to share and to prevent generalizationofsharingtonovelsettings,novel overgeneralization of the target response to people, and novel stimuli during two sessions nonsharing stimuli. prior to baseline and two sessions after the One participant (Isaac) engaged in disruptive trainingcriterionwasmet.The12trialsineach behavior during the general prompting proce- session consisted of three trials of teaching dure during training trials. To facilitate skill stimuli, four trials of probe stimuli that served acquisition and reduce the occurrence of as within- and across-category generalization disruption, additional prompts were used probes during training sessions, three trials of during treatment. A gestural prompt was added the fifth stimuli from each of the three training to the discriminative stimuli in each trial, and a categories that were not presented during behavior-specific praise statement was delivered training, and two trials of additional stimuli for offers to share. During teaching trials, after fromthefourth,nontrainingcategory.Withthe presenting the stimulus materials and giving exception of the scooter, Hippity-Hop, dot Isaacaverbaldirectiontoengageintheactivity, paint,andmagnets,stimuliusedinthepre-and the experimenter approached Isaac, stood posttreatment generalization probes were never within 0.6 m, and extended an open palm presented in experimental sessions. Table 1 toward him. Following errors, the video model depicts stimulus types used in pre- and andauditory or manualprompts were delivered posttreatment generalization probe trials for as in the treatment procedure. In addition, each participant. During these probe sessions, a when Isaac shared correctly, the experimenter novel instructor presented the stimulus materi- emitted a behavior-specific praise statement alsandgavethedirectiontoengageinthetarget (e.g., ‘‘Great! You handed it to me and said, activity. In addition, 5 s after the presentation ‘try this.’’’). The experimenter no longer of the materials, a peer was directed to ‘‘see delivered behavior-specific praise when Isaac what [participant’s name] is doing’’ and to met the mastery criterion and no longer approachtheparticipantandstandwithin0.6m delivered the gestural prompt after the schedule of him. The peer, who was approximately the of reinforcement was thinned. same age as the participant, was another child The mastery criterion was offering to share with autism. This trial format was unique independentlyonatleast89%(eightofnine)of because it was a peer who approached the the teaching trials for four consecutive sessions. participant rather than the person who initially The fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of token presentedthematerials.Theparticipantdidnot delivery for correct responding was thinned to receive tokens for correct responses or on-task VR 2 when the participant met the mastery behavior, nor did he receive prompts or video criterion. After criterion-level responding oc- models following incorrect responses. No curredduringteachingtrialsfortwoconsecutive additional prompts were provided if the sessions at VR 2, the schedule was thinned to participant emitted a correct offer to share, VR 5. After two consecutive sessions of and the peer accepted the item. If the criterion-level responding on the VR 5 sched- participant emitted a correct response and the 288 DENISE MARZULLO-KERTH et al. peer did not take the item, the instructor from 0% during baseline to 75% during directed the peer to accept the item and to treatment. Bobby demonstrated an increase in manipulate it appropriately (e.g., ‘‘You can eat offers to share on teaching trials from 0% the chip,’’ or ‘‘Take the crayon and color with during baseline to 100% during treatment and Steven.’’). The materials were removed and the from 0% during baseline to 50% during trial ended if the participant did not emit a treatment for probe trials. Aiden also demon- correct response within 5 s of the presentation stratedanincreaseinofferstoshareonteaching of the discriminative stimuli. trials from a mean of 2% during baseline to Maintenance. Maintenance sessions were 89% during treatment, and from 0% during conducted 1, 2, and 5 weeks after the baseline to 75% during treatment for probe posttreatment generalization probes for Steven, trials. and 1, 2, and 3 weeks after the posttreatment Higher percentages of correct responses generalization probes for Isaac and Bobby. occurred during within-category probe trials Maintenance sessions were not conducted for (Steven, M 5 80%; Isaac, M 5 67%; Bobby, Aidenbecausetheschoolyearendedandhewas M 5 63%; Aiden, M 5 60%) than during no longer available to participate. Procedures across-category probe trials (data not shown). were identical to those in baseline except that Specifically, Steven offered to share during the experimenter did not deliver tokens for on- across-category generalization probe trials dur- task behavior. ing only one treatment session (M 5 9%). Bobby never demonstrated across-category gen- RESULTS eralization during treatment, and Aiden dem- onstrated generalization during two treatment Figure 2 shows the percentage of teaching, sessions (M 5 13%). Isaac was the only probe, and nonsharing trials in which each participant who demonstrated across-category participant independently offered to share, generalizationonseveraloccasions(M556%). across consecutive sessions. Probe data are Throughout experimental sessions, all partic- collapsed across both within-category and ipants displayed some degree of vocal response across-category generalization trials. During generalization beyond the three scripted target baseline, none of the participants offered to phrases they were taught. Steven made nine share during any trial, with the exception of Aiden,whohandedanonpreferredfooditemto unscripted vocal offers to share, Isaac made the experimenter on two occasions and said seven,Bobbymadetwo,andAidenmadeseven. ‘‘here.’’Followingthesuccessiveintroductionof Examples of nontaught offers to share included treatment across participants, systematic in- ‘‘Wouldyouliketodraw?’’;‘‘Wouldyouliketo creases in offering to share occurred during build?’’; ‘‘ride the scooter’’; ‘‘try, please.’’ both teaching and probe trials. The percentage None of the participants offered to share of trials in which Steven offered to share during during any trial presented during the two teachingtrials systematically increased from 0% pretreatment generalization probe sessions (Fig- during baseline to 100% during treatment. ure 2). After each participant met the mastery During probe trials, the percentage of trials in criterion, however, the percentage of trials in which he offered to share increased from 0% which each participant offered to share in the during baseline to 75% during treatment. presenceofbothfamiliarandnovelstimuli,ina Isaac’s offers to share during teaching trials novel setting, and with novel adults and peers changed in a similar manner, increasing from increased compared to the pretreatment gener- 0% during baseline to 100% during treatment. alization probes. Steven offered to share with a His offers to share during probe trials increased peer on 100% of the trials presented for both

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.