JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2010, 43, 463–472 NUMBER3 (FALL2010) BEHAVIORAL COACHING TO IMPROVE OFFENSIVE LINE PASS- BLOCKING SKILLS OF HIGH SCHOOL FOOTBALL ATHLETES JOHN V. STOKES AND JAMES K. LUISELLI MAYINSTITUTE DEREK D. REED MELMARKNEWENGLAND RICHARD K. FLEMING UNIVERSITYOFMASSACHUSETTSMEDICALSCHOOL Weevaluatedseveralbehavioralcoachingproceduresforimprovingoffensivelinepass-blocking skills with 5 high school varsity football players. Pass blocking was measured during practice drills and games, and our intervention included descriptive feedback with and without video feedback and teaching with acoustical guidance (TAG). Intervention components and pass blocking were evaluated in a multiple baseline design, which showed that video feedback and TAG were the most effective procedures. For all players, improved pass blocking matched a standardderivedbyobservingmoreexperiencedlinemenandwasevidentingames.Additional intervention was required to maintain pass-blocking proficiency. Issues pertinent to behavioral coachingandsport psychology research arediscussed. Key words: behavioral coaching, football, performance feedback, teaching with acoustical guidance _______________________________________________________________________________ Consistent with the cornerstones of applied effective with sports such as swimming (Hazen, behavior analysis, behavioral coaching involves Johnstone, Martin, & Srikameswaran, 1990; selecting measurable behaviors, implementing Koop & Martin, 1983), track (Shapiro & operant-based procedures, and evaluating be- Shapiro, 1985), figure skating (Hume, Martin, havior change as a function of intervention Gonzalez, Cracklen, & Genthon, 1985), tennis (Martin & Hrycaiko, 1983). Furthermore, (Buzas & Ayllon, 1981), and gymnastics cognitive and motor control researchers have (Wolko, Hrycaiko, & Martin, 1983). identified behavioral coaching as a critical Relative to football, which was the focus of element in developing expert and elite-level the present study, Ward and Carnes (2002) performance in sports (Starkes & Ericsson, foundthathavingfivelinebackersonaNational 2003). Behavioral coaching programs typically Association of Intercollegiate Athletics (NAIA) combine several procedures, such as verbal Division II football team set performance goals instruction, prompting, goal setting, perfor- and receive publicly posted feedback improved mance feedback, and positive reinforcement. their correct execution of reads, drops, and Research has shown behavioral coaching to be tackles during practice sessions and games. In another study, Smith and Ward (2006) report- We thank Peter Flynn for his contributions to the ed that a combination of public posting with study. verbal feedback and goal setting improved AddresscorrespondencetoJohnV.Stokes,5CherylDrive, Milton, Massachusetts 02186 (e-mail: johnvictorstokes@ blocking, route running, and releases of three yahoo.com) or James K. Luiselli, May Institute,41 Pacella wide receivers who also played on an NAIA Park Drive, Randolph, Massachusetts 02368 (e-mail: Division II football team. Although the inter- [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-463 ventions evaluated by Ward and Carnes and 463 464 JOHN V. STOKES et al. Smith and Ward included several components, coach during practice pass-blocking drills. The providing behavior-contingent feedback to en- performance of several of the athletes was also hance performance was common to both. measured during game conditions and a Allison and Ayllon (1980) also used behav- maintenance phase when they returned for a ioral coaching with five younger boys (ages 11 second season.Additionalevaluationincludeda and 12 years) on a citywide football team. The social validity assessment in which the athletes dependent measure was the percentage of rated the relative value of each of the interven- correctblocksexecutedcorrectlyduringpractice tion procedures. drills. Under baseline conditions, the football coach implemented typical procedures (praise, METHOD encouragement, correction), but blocking did Participants and Setting not improve. The behavioral intervention The participants were five students who involved the coach yelling ‘‘freeze’’ when a attendedapublichighschool.Theywereselected player blocked incorrectly, demonstrating how for the study because the coaching staff had the block should have been executed, and identified them as having the poorest pass- instructing the player to imitate what he had blocking skills from a pool of 15 offensive been shown. These procedures were effective in linemen on the varsity football team. Linemen improvingblockingwithalloftheboys.Similar who had started at least one game during the totheresearchbyWardandCarnes(2002)and previousseasonwereexcludedfromthestudy,as SmithandWard(2006),theAllisonandAyllon were two other linemen who had played study relied on performance feedback as a extensively at the varsity level. Our rationale for primary component of intervention. this selection process was to evaluate procedures Becauseitisnotlikelythatallprocedureswill withtheleastcompetentandexperiencedplayers. improve athletic performance of all athletes, Noneoftheparticipantshadmorethan5years different intervention methods should be in- offootballplayingexperience.Mattwasasenior, corporated in behavioral coaching research DanandLoganwerejuniors,andSteveandRuss (Martin, Vause, & Schwartzman, 2005). More- were sophomores. The mean age of the partic- over, few studies have assessed athletes’ prefer- ipants was 16.2 years (range, 15 to 17 years), ences for the various behavioral interventions with a mean height of 183 cm (range, 174 to that are available to coaches (Smith & Ward, 201cm)andmeanbodyweightof89kg(range, 2006). As noted by Kazdin (1977) and Wolf 79 to 100 kg). Other than being told that they (1978), consumer preference for different wouldbereceivingcoachinginpassblocking,the interventions is a useful social validity outcome participants were not informed about the measure.Finally,despitenearlythreedecadesof purpose of the study. The parents of each behavior-analytic applications in sport psychol- participant provided written informed consent. ogy, research has measured performance during The offensive line coach implemented the actual competitive athletic events infrequently measurement and intervention procedures (de- (Martin et al.). scribedbelow).Hewasabachelors-levelteacher The purpose of the present study was to with 2 years coaching experience at the high evaluate the effects of several behavioral coach- school level. His involvement in the study was ing intervention procedures on offensive line voluntary. pass-blocking skills of high school football athletes. The procedures included different Measurement methods for providing performance feedback Blocking was defined according to the 10- and were implemented by the offensive line steptaskanalysisshowninTable 1.Fivecollege BEHAVIORAL COACHING IN FOOTBALL 465 Table 1 Ten-Step Blocking TaskAnalysis Step Description 1 Stance Feetshoulder-widthapart,50%bendattheknees,buttocksdownoverheels,center-sidehand touchingtheground 2 Split Sidelinefootbackslightlywithtoetoheelofcenter-sidefoot 3 Firststep Makeplay-sidebucketstepwhilemaintainingbalance 4 Helmetcontact Helmetfacemaskmakescontactwithopponentatthetopofjerseynumber 5 Handplacement Pointerfingersat11:00o’clockand1:00o’clockwiththumbspointedtowardsopponent’s Adam’sapple 6 Handposition Handsmaintainedinsideopponent’sshoulderpadsandtorsoframe 7 Armextension Armsextendedoutwardbeyond45degrees 8 Hipfollowthrough Hipsrolledtowardsopponent’sbellybutton 9 Legdrive Maintaincontinuousrunninglegpumpuntilwhistlesounds 10 Handcontact Handsincontactwithopponent’sbodyuntilwhistlesounds offensive line coaches (acquaintances of the coach to record data by watching videotaped seniorauthor)weresurveyedbeforethestudyto pass-blocking drills from the previous season’s validate the steps selected in the task analysis. practices. Training continued until the senior Each step was listed in sequence on a recording author and the coach achieved 90% or greater form. The dependent measure was the percent- interobserver agreement for three consecutive age of steps executed correctly during a practice drills. Additional training consisted of the pass-blocking drill and league football games. senior author and coach observing offensive Measurement during the pass-blocking drill linemen perform pass-blocking drills during was conducted at weekly practice sessions. The actual practice sessions. This training was drill began with a participant assuming a three- completed when we achieved 90% or greater point stance (one hand and two feet in contact agreement for three consecutive drills. with the ground) at the 5-yard field stripe. An Observers measured pass blocking during orange traffic cone was placed approximately 5 games from videotapes using the previously yards behind the participant, with one member described task analysis form. A single game was of the coaching staff standing behind the cone. videotaped for Dan and Matt during the first A defensive lineman in a four-pointstance (two season and for Dan, Steve, and Russ during the hands and two feet in contact with the ground) second season. The offensive line coach record- waspositionedapproximately1 yardin front of ed the initial three (Dan, Matt, and Russ) or the participant. In response to the correct four (Steve) pass-blocking sequences from the offensive cadence, a participant had to block videotapes for each of these participants. the rushing defensive lineman within a 3-yard lateralboundary,preventinghimfromtouching Interobserver Agreement the orange cone. The pass-blocking drill During the study, 50% of the practice pass- continued until either 10 s elapsed or the blocking drills were videotaped for all partici- defensive lineman touched the orange cone, pants. We assessed interobserver agreement by whichever came first. having the senior author record pass-blocking The offensive line coach observed each execution on the 10-step task analysis form. participant during a single pass-blocking drill, These results were compared with the real-time recording a plus or a minus on the task analysis data that had been scored by the offensive line form next to each step that the participant coach. An agreement was tallied if both executed correctly and incorrectly, respectively. observers rated each step identically (correct Before the study, the senior author trained the execution or incorrect execution). Interobserver 466 JOHN V. STOKES et al. agreement was computed by dividing the during the pass-blocking drill, emphasizing number of agreements by the number of proper execution by reminding them about agreements plus disagreements and converting positional technique and staying focused. He the ratio to a percentage. Mean agreement was usually praised the participants when they 90% (range, 50% to 100%). We also assessed blocked correctly, for example, making a interobserver agreement during 50% of the statement such as ‘‘good work’’ or ‘‘That’s the videotaped games using the same method of waytohit!’’Occasionally,hedeliveredaslapon calculationdescribedforthepass-blockingdrill. the back of the helmet to acknowledge good Mean game agreement assessments were 88% performance.Inresponsetopoorexecution,the (range, 70% to 100%). Note that the two low coach typically responded negatively with a percentages of 50% and 70% were the only remark such as, ‘‘That stinks, you didn’t take a scores below 85% and were associated with one drop step,’’ sometimes followed by modeling participant (Russ). the desired pass-blocking behaviors. During the baseline phase, no other programmed conse- Normative Assessment quences were delivered. Before the study, the senior author and the Descriptive feedback. Baseline conditions re- offensive line coach recorded pass blocking of mained in effect except that the coach showed the three highest rated starting offensive the completed 10-step task analysis form to linemenfrom thepreviousseason. Werecorded each participant following the pass-blocking data on the 10-step task analysis form for the drill. He reviewed steps that participants initial pass-blocking opportunity by each line- executed correctly and incorrectly through a man during three videotaped games with high combination of instruction, modeling, and school opponents (interobserver agreement was physical prompting. Correct steps received assessed during 100% of the pass-blocking praise(e.g.,‘‘Great,keepitup!’’)andnonverbal sequences, and mean agreement was 87%, with approval (e.g., slapping a high five). The coach a range of 70% to 100%). Mean correct pass- responded to incorrect steps by explaining how blocking accuracy for the three linemen was they should have been executed, having a 80% (range, 70% to 90%). Therefore, we participantperformthestepsonetimecorrectly, adopted a range of 70% to 90% as our and giving verbal feedback for those steps (e.g., acceptable performance criterion. ‘‘That’s it, now you’ve got it!’’). Participants Intervention and Research Design were allowed to question the coach about how Intervention components were evaluated in a to further refine their performances. multiple baseline design across participants. Descriptive feedback plus video feedback. In Baseline, descriptive feedback, and descriptive addition to descriptive feedback, each partici- feedback plus video feedback phases were pantwatchedavideotapeofthepracticedrillhe implemented with all of the participants. Four had just completed. The videotape was shown of the participants also received teaching with to each participant immediately following the acoustical guidance (TAG). Procedures during drill. Together, the coach and participant rated baseline and intervention phases were imple- performance on the task analysis form during mented at a Wednesday practice session each their videotape observation. Praise and correc- week because that was the day typically tion from the coach, as described above, were scheduled for individual offensive skills drills. contingentoncorrectlyexecutedandincorrectly Baseline. Baseline procedures were those in executed steps, respectively. The video feedback effect before the study. Specifically, the offen- concluded by having each participant perform sive line coach instructed the participants the pass-blocking sequence by himself, one BEHAVIORAL COACHING IN FOOTBALL 467 time, without a rushing defensive lineman and When they achieved an acceptable level of without the coach’s praise or correction. performance during practice pass-blocking Teachingwithacousticalguidance.Fourofthe drills, we conducted measurement from video- five participants received the TAG condition tapes of a scheduled league game with a high following the descriptive plus video feedback school opponent. A different game was video- phase.One participantwasexcluded becausehe taped and measured with each participant (one started playing regularly in league games after game for Dan and Steve and two games for thedescriptiveplusvideofeedbackphasedueto Russ). aninjurysustainedbyoneoftheteam’sstarting Social Validity Assessment offensive linemen. With TAG, an audible At the conclusion of the first football season, stimulus such as a click or chirp is sounded to the participants were asked to rate their signify that a desired behavior has occurred satisfaction with the four phases they experi- (Pryor, 1999). TAG is similar to concurrent enced during the study (baseline, descriptive auditory feedback, an intervention used in feedback, descriptive feedback plus video feed- motor-skills learning (Konttinen, Mononen, back, and TAG). On a 1-page form, they Viitasalo, & Mets, 2004). During the TAG entered one of four numerical ratings for each phase, the coach informed each participant phase: 1 5 poor, 2 5 fair, 3 5 good, 4 5 about specific steps from the task analysis that excellent. Before presenting the form to the would be ‘‘tagged.’’ The coach selected steps participants, the senior author reminded them that each participant had failed to execute abouttheproceduresthatcomprisedeachphase consistently; these were Step 7 for Dan and of the study. Steve, Step 5 for Logan, and Steps 5, 6, and 7 for Russ. Immediately after observing a partic- ipant perform the step correctly, the coach RESULTS sounded a bullhorn that produced a siren Figure 1 shows the percentage of pass- lasting 1 s. All participants reported that they blocking steps executed correctly by each could hear the siren without difficulty. No participant during practice drills and games. additional feedback was given to the partici- Mean correct pass blocking was 40% and 50% pantsfollowingtheTAGpass-blockingdrillnor for Dan during the baseline and descriptive were putative positive (e.g., praise) or negative feedback phases, respectively. His performance backupreinforcers(e.g.,avoidanceofpracticing improved under the video feedback condition the drill again) associated with the TAG (M 5 82%). Targeting Step 7 of the task procedure. analysiswiththeTAGprocedurewasassociated Game performance. None of the intervention with 100% correct pass blocking during drills. procedures were implemented during the Mean blocking proficiency was 85% in game games. conditions. During the baseline at the start of Follow-up.Wemeasured thepass blockingof the second season, his mean correct pass Dan, Steve, and Russ when they returned for a blocking returned to low levels (M 5 45%). second football season. Each participant was Descriptive plus video feedback was effective in evaluated under baseline conditions, followed improving his pass blocking (M 5 80%), and by intervention procedures that had been this high proficiency was maintained during effective during the preceding season. Dan actual game observations (M 5 87%). and Steve received descriptive feedback plus Mean correct pass blocking was 47% and video feedback only, and Russ received descrip- 50% for Steve during the baseline and tive feedback plus video feedback and TAG. descriptive feedback conditions, respectively. 468 JOHN V. STOKES et al. Figure1. Percentageofpass-blockingstepsexecutedcorrectlyduringpracticedrillsandgamesacrosstwoseasonsof high school football. The shaded horizontal lines represent the normative pass-blocking performance range of starting varsitylinemen. Correct performance increased with the provi- quent in-game performance was strong (M 5 sion of video feedback (M 5 87%). Effects of 85%). the TAG procedure targeting Step 7 of the task Mean correct pass blocking was 43% for analysis are unclear; nevertheless, he achieved Logan during the baseline phase. His perfor- 100%correctin three ofsix pass-blocking drills mance improved initially with descriptive during the TAG phase. When assessed at the feedback but then decreased (M 5 62%). start of the second season, correct pass blocking Adding video feedback increased correct pass returned to low levels during baseline (M 5 blocking to a mean of 90%. Mean correct pass 55%). Reimplementing video feedback im- blocking was 95% with the TAG procedure in proved his performance (M 5 83%); subse- place for Step 5 of the task analysis. BEHAVIORAL COACHING IN FOOTBALL 469 Table 2 Social Validity Ratings ofBehavioral Coaching Tactics Participant Condition Dan Steve Logan Matt Russ M(SD) Baseline 1 1 1 1 1 1.0(0) Descriptivefeedback 2 2 3 2 2 2.2(0.45) Descriptivefeedbackplusvideofeedback 4 4 4 4 3 3.8(0.45) TAG 3 3 2 4 3.0(0.82) Mean correct pass blocking was 59% for ed for all of the participants who were assessed Matt during the baseline phase. Pass blocking during games. appeared to improve with both descriptive (M Table 2presentsthesocialvalidityresults.All 571%)andvideo(M584%)feedback.Mean five participants rated the baseline coaching blocking proficiency was 83% under game procedures as poor. They rated descriptive conditions. feedback as fair (80%) and good (20%), Russ demonstrated the most variability in descriptive feedback plus video feedback as pass-blocking execution during the initial good (20%) and excellent (80%), and TAG as baseline phase, ranging from 20% to 60% (M fair (25%), good (50%), and excellent (25%). 5 38%). His performance did not improve with descriptive feedback (M 5 41%), but did DISCUSSION improve with video feedback (M 5 66%). Descriptive feedback alone did not improve Further improvement occurred when the TAG pass blocking. The descriptive and video procedure was implemented for Step 5 (M 5 feedback condition was demonstrated to be 80%), Step 6 (M 5 82%), and Step 7 (M 5 effective in improving correct pass blocking for 88%) of the task analysis. The second season all five participants. When four of the five baseline assessment showed a return to low participants received TAG, their correct block- levels of correct pass blocking (M 5 27%). ing increased further, but the short assessment Descriptive and video feedback were associated phases, the increasing trends in the data in withimprovedperformance(M567%),which preceding conditions, and the aggregation of was maintained during games (M 5 65%). the data make it difficult to isolate the specific Because performance fell below the perfor- effects of TAG. For all of the participants, mance criterion, additional intervention was improved pass blocking matched a normative implemented (TAG of Step 7), yieldinga mean standard for more experienced linemen, and performance of 78%. His correct pass blocking this strong performance was evident in games. persistedduringasecondin-gamemeasurement Finally, participants rated the coaching condi- (M 5 77%). tion involving descriptive and video feedback At the close of our intervention, each most favorably. participant was able to pass block consistently Video feedback combined with descriptive within our normative criterion that was estab- feedbackwasconsistentlysuperiortodescriptive lished for starting offensive linemen. All feedbackaloneinimprovingpassblocking.This participants achieved this criterion during the outcome is consistent with similar research descriptive and video feedback phase; however, conducted with swimmers (Hazen et al., 1990). TAG allowed participants to exceed (Dan, One possible reason for the superiority of video Steve, Logan) or consistently stay within the feedback in the current study is that this criterion (Russ). Criterion performance persist- condition included model prompts in addition 470 JOHN V. STOKES et al. tothevocalpromptsinthedescriptivefeedback recommended as an effective training method condition, and these model prompts may be in football as well as in other sports. instrumental in developing proficient pass- We concentrated on pass blocking during blocking performances. Note, however, that weeklypracticedrillsinthisstudy.Performance the video feedback condition also required that during actual games was assessed with two the participants practice the entire 10-step participants during the first season and three blocking sequence one time after seeing them- participants who returned for a second season. selves on videotape. Therefore, the question These results demonstrated that the pass- remains as to whether video feedback without blocking proficiency achieved during practice this practice step would have had the same was maintained under game conditions. Obser- effect. vation of effective performance during actual TAG involves the behavior-contingent pre- game situations is a strength of the study. sentation of an audible marker (Pryor, 1999). However, conclusions about practice-to-game As noted previously,TAG is formally similar to performance must be tempered because the concurrent feedback by which an auditory gamesampleswerelimitedtothreetofourplays stimulus follows a target behavior that is during one to two games and because we did demonstrated under real-time conditions not conduct preintervention game assessments (Konttinen et al., 2004). In the present study, for any of the participants (Smith & Ward, the effects of TAG may have influenced 2006; Ward & Carnes, 2002). Furthermore, performance via different stimulus functions, not all of the participants’ pass-blocking such as (a) increasing the discriminative control performancewasmeasuredduringgames.These of antecedent stimuli (e.g., movements of the limitations should be addressed in future rushing lineman’s or the blocker’s own behav- behavioral coaching research concerned with ior) or (b) strengthening precursor behaviors in the effects of practice-based interventions on the blocking sequence (e.g., feet position or performance during competitive events. body angle). Furthermore, the auditory stimu- Using a behavioral consultation model (Kra- lus in our study, a siren, may have become a tochwill & Bergan, 1990), the senior author conditioned reinforcer by virtue of its pairing trained and monitored the coach’s implemen- with successful blocking and unprogrammed tation of intervention procedures and his positive attention from teammates and the measurement of their relative effectiveness coach. One advantage of TAG in behavioral throughout regularly scheduled football prac- coaching is that it can be implemented at the tices during this study. Interobserver agreement precise moment in practice situations that assessments revealed that the coach reliably require fluent execution of chained responses. recorded data throughout the evaluation. Un- For example, vocal prompting and feedback derstandingtherelationbetweencomponentsof from the coach may not influence behavior behavioral coaching and proficient pass-block- during the actual blocking sequence due to the ing performance under these authentic condi- noisy and physically challenging pass-blocking tions is another strength of our study. Future drill conditions. By contrast, coaches can research should measure intervention integrity effectively mark participant-specific behaviors of coaches to ensure that procedures are with the TAG procedure. Because strong implemented as described (Gresham, 1989) inferences regarding the efficacy of TAG are and to determine if different components are not possible in this study, further evaluations moreorlessdifficulttoimplementunderactual are needed to determine whether TAG can be coaching conditions. BEHAVIORAL COACHING IN FOOTBALL 471 The dependent measure in our study was the ments should incorporate dimensions of proce- percentage of pass-blocking steps executed dures used to improve performance that may correctly. However, our measurement system influence preference. These variables may did not show whether acquiring these steps include whether procedures build on existing corresponded with successful pass-blocking skillsor attempttoteachnovelskills,aresimple outcomes. For example, a successful or unsuc- orrelativelycomplex,ormakeplayersstandout cessful block could be defined as a participant in the presence of teammates rather than being preventing the rushing defensive lineman from taught more discreetly. In addition to deter- contacting the orange cone in practice or the mining the players’ values for behavioral quarterback in games. Or, another measure of coaching tactics, social validity assessment successful pass blocking could be the distance a should be conducted with the coaches who participant kept the rushing defensive lineman implementthemtoidentifythemostacceptable from the orange cone and the quarterback. training and practice procedures. Ultimately, improved pass blocking can be It is not uncommon to find poor postinter- measured against game indexes such as the vention maintenance associated with behavioral percentage of passes completed, total offensive intervention in sports. As an illustration, yards,pointsscored,andwins.Ourtaskanalysis Anderson and Kirkpatrick (2002) used several was selected to document whether intervention behavioral procedures to increase correct relay effectively taught young football athletes the tags by three members of a competitive inline basic skills necessary for proper pass-blocking roller speed skating team. Six months following execution (cf. Allison & Ayllon, 1980) but intervention, all of the skaters required addi- additional measures of successful blocking tional training because they did not perform outcomes should be incorporated in future relay tags at the frequency achieved with behavioral coaching research. intervention. Similarly, improved pass blocking Although few behavioral coaching studies was notmaintained forthethree participantsin have reported social validity outcomes (Roger- ourstudywhoreturnedforasecondseason.Itis son & Hrycaiko, 2002; Smith & Ward, 2006; likely that each participant’spass-blocking skills Swain & Jones, 1995), the social acceptability deteriorated without routine practice and of the various behavioral coaching tactics may ongoing behavioral coaching. Their perfor- be important to consider in addition to the mance improved immediately when descriptive efficacy of such tactics. Four of the five plus video feedback and TAG procedures were participantsinthecurrentstudypreferredvideo reimplemented. These results suggest that one feedback over the other procedures, and the way to promote maintenance is for coaches to fifth participant rated TAG most highly. conduct booster training sessions at the start of Consistent with other social validity research a new season. in which direct consumers’ values of interven- In summary, we found that a behavioral tions are assessed, our participants preferred the coaching intervention that incorporated vocal, conditionsunderwhichtheyweremosteffective visual,andinsomecasesacousticalperformance (e.g., Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, & Maglieri, 2005; feedback improved offensive line pass-blocking Heal, Hanley, & Layer, 2009). Of future skills of high school football athletes. The interest is whether the most effective coaching participants were able to perform at a level methods are those judged most favorably by comparabletomoreexperiencedteammatesand athletes and whether successful outcomes are did so proficiently outside the practice sessions. consistent with or independent of players’ They also rated most highly the intervention preferences. In addition, social validity assess- procedures that were effective for them. The 472 JOHN V. STOKES et al. resultsofthestudysupportpreviousfindingson Koop, S., & Martin, G. L. (1983). Evaluation of a coachingstrategytoreduceswimmingstrokeerrorsin the benefits of behavioral coaching in football beginning age-group swimmers. Journal of Applied (Allison&Ayllon,1980;Smith&Ward,2006; BehaviorAnalysis,16,447–460. Ward & Carnes, 2002) and an applied Kratochwill, T. R., & Bergan, J. R. (1990). Behavioral consultation in applied settings: An individual guide. behavior-analytic approach to athletic perfor- NewYork: Plenum. mance enhancement (Martin, Thompson, & Martin, G. L., & Hrycaiko, D. (Eds.). (1983). Behavior Regehr, 2004; Martin et al., 2005). modification and coaching: Principles, procedures, and research. Springfield, IL: CharlesCThomas. Martin, G. L., Thompson, K., & Regehr, K. (2004). REFERENCES Studies using single-subject designs in sport psychol- ogy: 30 years of research. The Behavior Analyst, 27, Allison,M.G.,&Ayllon,T.(1980).Behavioralcoaching 263–280. in the development of skills in football, gymnastics, Martin, G. L., Vause, T., & Schwartzman, L. (2005). and tennis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, Experimental studies of psychological interventions 297–314. with athletes in competitions: Why so few? Behavior Anderson, G., & Kirkpatrick, M. A. (2002). Variable Modification,29,616–641. effects of a behavioral treatment package on the Pryor, K. (1999). Don’t shoot the dog: The new art of performance of inline roller speed skaters. Journal of teachingandtraining.New York: Bantam. AppliedBehavior Analysis, 35,195–198. Rogerson, L. J., & Hrycaiko, D. W. (2002). Enhancing Buzas, H. P., & Ayllon, T. (1981). Differential competitive performance of ice hockey goaltenders reinforcement in coaching tennis skills. Behavior usingcenteringandself-talk.JournalofAppliedSport Modification,5, 372–385. Psychology,14,14–26. Gresham,F.(1989).Assessment oftreatment integrity in Shapiro,E.S.,&Shapiro,S.(1985).Behavioralcoaching school consultation and prereferral intervention. in the development of skills in track. Behavior SchoolPsychology Review,18, 37–50. Modification,9,211–224. Hanley,G.P.,Piazza,C.C.,Fisher,W.W.,&Maglieri, Smith,S.L.,&Ward,P.(2006).Behavioralinterventions K.M.(2005).Ontheeffectivenessofandpreference toimproveperformanceincollegiatefootball.Journal for punishment and extinction components of ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 39,385–391. function-based interventions. Journal of Applied Starkes,J.L.,&Ericsson,K.A.(2003).Expertperformance BehaviorAnalysis,38, 51–66. in sports: Advances in research on sport expertise. Hazen, A., Johnstone, C., Martin, G. L., & Srikames- Champaign, IL:Human Kinetics. waran,S.(1990).Avideotapingfeedbackpackagefor Swain, A., & Jones, G. (1995). Effects of goal-setting improvingskillsofyouthcompetitiveswimmers.The interventions on selected basketball skills: A single- Sport Psychologist,4,213–227. subject design. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Heal, N. A., Hanley, G. P., & Layer, S. A. (2009). An Sports,66,51–63. evaluation of the relative efficacy of and child Ward,P.,&Carnes,M.(2002).Effectsofpostingself-set preferenceforteachingstrategiesthatdifferinamount goals on collegiate football players’ skill execution of teacher directedness. Journal of Applied Behavior duringpracticeandgames.JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis,42,123–143. Analysis, 35,1–12. Hume,K.M.,Martin,G.L.,Gonzalez,P.,Cracklen,C., Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for & Genthon, S. (1985). A self-monitoring feed- subjective measurement or how applied behavior back package for improving freestyle figure skat- analysisisfindingitsheart.JournalofAppliedBehavior ing practice. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 333– Analysis, 11,203–214. 345. Wolko,K.L.,Hrycaiko,D.W.,&Martin,G.L.(1993). Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Assessing the clinical or applied A comparison of two self-management packages to importance of behavior change through social standard coaching for improving practice perfor- validation.Behavior Modification,1, 427–451. mance of gymnasts. Behavior Modification, 17, Konttinen, N., Mononen, K., Viitasalo, J., & Mets, 209–223. T. (2004). The effects of augmented auditory feedback on psychomotor skill learning in precision Received October 22,2008 shooting.JournalofSportandExercisePsychology,26, Final acceptance May18,2009 306–316. Action Editor,Gregory P. Hanley