ebook img

ERIC EJ894801: What Future Teachers Think about Brain Research PDF

2009·0.06 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ894801: What Future Teachers Think about Brain Research

Thoughts about Brain Research 39 What Future Teachers Think About Brain Research Debby Zambo and Ron Zambo Arizona State University As information becomes available, teachers of educational psychology must be ready to accept or resist change. This is especially true when it comes to the brain because as technologies advance, information about it is seeping into our lives. Deciding if, how much, and when this information fits into educational psychology coursework can be difficult. In this survey study, the authors gathered data from students (n = 215) enrolled in a professional teacher education program in the southwestern United States. The study targeted the opinions of future teachers in regard to the usefulness of brain research, the importance of learning about the brain, and concerns about the applicability of brain research. The results indicate future teachers think brain research is useful and valuable for educators. Participants were neutral about potential concerns. Education advances as new information believe findings from neuroscience should becomes available, and those who are be kept in biology; they have no practical teaching educational psychology must be application for them. Given the dichotomy ready to accept or resist change. This is between our students’ interests and the need especially important related to information for instructors of educational psychology to about the brain. As researchers learn more incorporate or eliminate brain research in and more about brain development, their course, we wonder just where and if chemistry and structure theories of learning brain research fits into the lives of our and motivation are being altered, confirmed, students as future educators. As instructors and changed. As technology advances, more of undergraduates, we are struggling to and more educational psychology textbooks understand if, and how much, information are referencing or containing entire chapters from brain research should be incorporated dedicated to the brain, how it develops and into the courses we teach. learns, and the things that motivate it to succeed. However, paradigm shifts are not Our dilemma is timely because of the always easy and deciding if, how much, and avalanche of new information being brought how this information fits into educational into educational psychology textbooks. In psychology coursework can be difficult the 1990s, the “decade of the brain” new especially if students desire it or are technologies allowed scientists to pry open skeptical of its use. From our experience the “black box” of the brain to see in vivo working with future teachers, we have what was happening as an individual learned noticed that some students are motivated to or performed a task (Restak, 2001; Willis, learn all they can about the brain. They 2006). With improved technology, brain avidly read the information on the brain in images were able to show the dramatic their textbook and seek Web sites and physical changes that occur in the brain with videos about the brain because they believe environmental stimulation and the dark, neuroscience will provide insights and unlit areas that are present when a brain is strategies they can transform into classroom deprived of the stimulation and affection it use. In contrast, we see other future teachers needs (Perry, 2007). New technologies are who remain skeptical. They invest little time leading to an understanding of how the brain learning about the brain because they functions as it processes information and Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 40 why some individuals find learning so called to work with other professionals. difficult (Shaywitz, 2005; Shaywitz, These professionals (e.g., physicians, Shaywitz, & Fletcher, 1990). As a result of therapists, audiologists, etc.) learn about the this new and growing body of knowledge, brain, and to communicate teachers need the some researchers are calling for a paradigm same language or vocabulary. Information shift in education, comparable in magnitude about the brain will enable teachers to work to the shift from a behavioralist orientation with other professionals and generate to a cognitive perspective in the 1960s and effective strategies based on students’ 1970s (Byrnes & Fox, 1998). Many believe individual needs. It will also make teachers that the findings from brain research can be more critical consumers of information transformed into practical strategies teachers because they will be able to discern fallacies can use to improve their teaching, and these from facts, and effective treatments from are creeping into coursework and textbooks. unrealistic promises. Some researchers, like Geake and However, there are others who have Cooper (2003), believed that the findings reservations about the potential usefulness from neuroscience are so important they of brain research in education. For example, need to be part of teacher education and in Educational Psychology, Anita Wolfolk professional development. These researchers (2007) cautioned her readers to be wary of argued that teachers, psychologists, and brain research because even though much neuroscientists need to work together. These has been written suggestions are often researchers brought teachers into the mix oversimplified. Likewise, in Educational because they believed that they will be the Psychology: Developing Learners, Jeannie ones who transform findings from Ormrod stated that brain physiology does neuroscience into reality. In their words, not explain how best to foster learning and teachers will always be interested in gaining cognitive development. Ormrod noted that an understanding of the multitude of factors educators enthusiastic about brain-based that govern the learning of their charges. learning are likely to be talking about what Such teacher professional development, we psychologists have known for years. This suggest, should embrace an understanding of way of thinking is echoed by long-time development of cognitive neuroscience. critic of neuroscience in the classroom, John Therefore we propose that education adapt T. Bruer (1999). Bruer noted that even an interactive bio-psycho-social model, though neuroscience provides information which can only come if educationists engage about how the brain operates, much of what neuroscientists in dialogue to share each is being sold to teachers is really little more other’s professional knowledge. (p. 11) than a repackaging of 1970s cognitive science. Bruer (1999) suggested that even Similarly, Byrnes and Fox (1998) and though neuroscience provides images of the Zull (2002) purported that if teachers learn brain at work, those images tell teachers about neurons, synapses, and brain little about behavior or effective strategies. chemistry they will be able to use that knowledge to teach effectively. Others like Others worry about the profits being Berninger and Richards (2003) believed made. Judy Willis (2006), a neuroscientist teachers not only need to know about the and middle school teacher, warned that brain but that they have the right to know many of the products being marked as brain about the brain because they are the ones enrichment actually misinterpret or Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 41 overextend findings from brain research. Regardless of the views of the experts Likewise, learning expert Jill Stamm (2007) and textbook writers, our experience shows noted that many of the promises being made that brain research is making its way into by companies selling toys, DVDs, and educational psychology textbooks and class computer games to accelerate brain discussions. Yet, when we did a search for development are not realistic because they information about students’ thoughts about are based on making a profit and not on the neuroscience, or the beliefs of practicing knowledge of neuroscientists who teachers, no information could be found. To understand firsthand the facts and fallacies begin to fill in these gaps and help us to about brain function (p. xi). Furthermore, understand where brain research might fit with the availability of the Internet there are into our courses, we created a questionnaire many Web sites of questionable validity to solicit the opinions of the preservice related to the brain. For example, if you teachers in our courses. Our research Google left brain + right brain you will find questions were (a) What sources do future Web sites that tell you which side of your teachers use to get information about the brain is dominant simply responding to brain and where might they go in the future statements like, “I wear a watch.” and to get more information? (b) Do future “Some people think I’m psychic.” Although teachers believe that information about the dominance is a result of brain structure, brain will lead to better instruction? (c) Do determining one’s dominant hemisphere by future teachers believe that learning about responding to simple statements is the brain should be a part of teacher questionable. In addition, there are Web education? and (d) How concerned are sites that claim they can raise intelligence future teachers about the use of brain scores, cure attention deficits, and make research in education? babies more motivated to learn. In these cases, information from neuroscience is Method being overextended, and this is especially Participation in the project was troubling when parents and teachers of voluntary and anonymous. Participants children with disabilities put their faith in completed the questionnaire at the end of a unfounded causes or cures. class session in an educational psychology, assessment, or child development course in In Constructing Autism, Nadesan (2005) which they were enrolled. Course reported her concerns about making life- instructors, none of whom were connected altering decisions about children and their with this research study, distributed and learning from images, electrical patterns, collected the questionnaires. There were no and analyses of brain chemistry. Nadesan points toward the course grade or any other believed brains are complex structures with compensation offered as an incentive. A variations and that variations challenge the scripted statement was used to introduce the very notion of using brain scans and other study and explain procedures. After this technologies. Drawing lines as to which introduction, students were given and asked brain is defective gets messy because two to read a letter that explained potential risks brains that are both considered normal (or of participation and their rights. The abnormal) can look and function similarly students who decided not to participate in and differently. the project were asked to leave the room. If students agreed to participate, then they were handed a questionnaire to complete. In Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 42 an effort to uncover the individual opinions enrolled in an undergraduate teacher of the participants, participants were not education program at a state university in allowed to talk to or discuss items with their the southwest region of the United States. colleagues during the administration. The Within the college of education, participants questionnaires were returned to the were enrolled in various programs such as instructor immediately after their early childhood education (26, 12%), completion. Although participation was elementary education (59, 27%), secondary voluntary, everyone who was asked to education (40, 19%), special education (40, participate in the study did. Possibly, this is 19%), and bilingual education (34, 16%). an indication of these future teachers’ We were unable to determine the placement interest in brain research. In addition, the of 16 (8%) participants because they high level of participation eliminated the supplied no response to this demographic possibility that participants self-selected, an question. Of the 215 who completed the issue that comes to bear on the validity of questionnaire, 167 (78%) were female and many survey studies. 48 (22%) were male. The majority of participants were relatively young, 157 Participants (73%) were between 20 and 29 years of age The participants were all full-time and 163 (76%) were White. Demographic students, in their junior year in college, information is provided in Table 1. Table 1. Demographic Information of Participants Current placement No. % Early childhood education 26 12.1 Elementary education 59 27.4 Secondary education 40 18.6 Special education 40 18.6 Bilingual education 34 15.8 No response 16 7.5 Ethnicity American Indian or Alaskan Native 4 2 Asian or Pacific Islander 7 3 Biracial or Bicultural 1 1 Black or African American 4 2 Hispanic or Latino 31 14 White–Caucasian 163 76 Other 4 2 Missing 1 1 Age (in years) Under 20 1 1 20–29 157 73 30–39 30 14 40–49 20 9 Over 50 7 3 Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 43 Gender Female 167 78 Male 48 22 Development of the Questionnaire The items were grouped under the three The questionnaire included two overarching themes being investigated, later items to gather information about these referred to as subscales: (a) Information future teachers’ sources of information about the brain can lead to better instruction about the brain. To spark their thinking (better), (b) learning about the brain should about where they typically had found become a part of teacher preparation information about the brain, we listed (prepare), and (c) there are potential potential sources of information (i.e., problems with the application of brain Internet, television, radio, magazines, research to education (concerns). The items journals, courses, workshops), and were formatted with a 6-point Likert scale. participants marked all that applied. The Participants indicated their level of listed sources were based on our agreement with each statement as strongly observations of where the general public and agree, agree, mildly agree, mildly disagree, educators might see information about the disagree, or strongly disagree. brain. An additional choice, “other,” provided the participants the opportunity to Data from the 22 items in regard to include additional, unlisted sources. A questions two, three, and four were entered parallel item asked participants to predict into SPSS with 1 (strongly disagree) and 6 what sources they would use in the future to (strongly agree). Reliability of the 22 access information about the brain. Likert-type items was computed by using coefficient alpha. The 22 items had a The remainder of the questionnaire reliability of .72 with reliabilities of the comprised items based on the research cited subscales at .84 (better), .80 (prepare), and in the introduction of this manuscript. For .52 (concerns). example, the idea posed by Berninger and Richards (2003) that learning about the brain Results offers educators a common language to Information Sources communicate was the basis for, “Learning In regard to the first question, “What about the brain gives teachers the sources do future teachers use to get vocabulary they need to communicate information about the brain and where might effectively with other professionals like they go in the future to get more doctors and psychologists.” Other items information?” participants indicated that were written on the opposing view and they relied on multiple sources. The mean express what could be concerns, for number of sources participants reported example, Bruer’s (1999, 2006) ideas as to using in the past and projected using in the the true applicability and usefulness of brain future were both 3.0. Although the mean research for educators became, “There are number of sources remained the same, the too many bold statements about brain rankings of sources changed. For example, research and its implications for education.” the Internet and television were the most popular sources in the past, whereas, the Internet and journals were the most popular Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 44 sources expected to be used in the future. percentage of use of all sources for both past Sources listed in the “other” category were and future. colleagues and books. See Table 2 for the Table 2. Sources of Information by Percentage of Participants Source Past sources (%) Future sources (%) Internet 64.2 73.5 Television 52.6 18.1 Journals 47.9 67.9 Courses 49.3 47.9 Magazine 41.4 27.0 Workshop 13.5 30.7 Inservice 9.3 14.0 Radio 11.6 3.7 Other 7.4 3.3 Not interested 3.7 2.3 Subscales and Participants’ Perceptions The percentages of participants Means and standard deviations were agreeing with each statement were also calculated for each subscale. Because the computed and can be found in Table 3. One questionnaire had a 6-point Likert scale we item from better was agreed to by over 95% considered means greater than 3.5 (the of the participants: “Learning about the midpoint on the scale) an indication of brain will help teachers understand learning agreement, means below 3.5 an indication of differences.” Three items from the subscale disagreement, and their distance from 3.5 an prepare had over 95% agreement: (a) indication of their strength of agreement or “Information from brain research should be disagreement. Group means and standard used to improve instruction.” (b) “It is deviations on each of the three subscales important for teachers to be informed about were 4.69 (0.76) for better, 4.63 (0.71) for current brain research.” (c) “It is important prepare, and 3.56 (0.65) for concerns. A to translate findings from brain research to series of paired sample t tests (p < .01) practical teaching strategies.” No items from revealed that the means for better and the subscale concerns had agreement above prepare were not significantly different, 95%. The item with which there was the however, they were both significantly higher least agreement (25%) was from concerns, than the mean for concerns. that is, “Brain research is conducted in clinical situations so findings should not be applied to children in our schools.” Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 45 Table 3. Percentage of Agreement With Questionnaire Items Subscale name and item % Better Learning about the brain will help teachers understand learning differences. 96.3 I believe findings from brain research will lead to a better education for all students. 94.9 I believe brain research will lead to better identification of students with special 93.5 needs. Learning about the brain will help teachers design programs to optimize learning. 91.1 Teachers need to know about how the brain functions to understand teaching and 82.8 learning. Teachers should be expected to consider the brain when they prepare lessons. 80.5 Prepare Information from brain research should be used to improve instruction. 97.7 It is important for teachers to be informed about current brain research. 97.2 It is important to translate findings from brain research to practical teaching 96.8 strategies. Learning about the brain is not too complicated for teachers. 92.1 Learning about brain functions has little to offer teachers. 89.3 School psychologists receive training about the brain and so should teachers. 83.2 Learning about the brain should be included in both the biological sciences and in 82.8 teacher preparation. Learning about the brain gives teachers the vocabulary they need to communicate 78.1 effectively with other professionals like doctors and psychologists. Teacher education is incomplete without information about the brain. 69.8 I believe learning about brain research is beyond the scope of what teachers should 68.8 be expected to learn. Concerns There is misunderstanding among educators about how brain research applies to 76.7 education. I am concerned that brain research will be used to decide who will receive 61.4 educational enrichment. There are too many bold statements about brain research and its implications for 58.6 education. I am concerned that the use of brain research will create an additional financial 57.7 burden on schools. I am concerned that in the future, brain research will be used to decide who will 46.6 receive special education services. Brain research is conducted in clinical situations so findings should not be applied to 25.1 children in our schools. Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 46 About 75% of the participants were brain research we should incorporate into White women between the ages of 20 and our courses. We wondered where future 29. To determine variations between this teachers get information about the brain and majority demographic and the remaining what they thought about the use and 25% of the participants, we used multiple applicability of brain research for education. analyses of variance (MANOVAs) to We found participants in this study use an determine the effect of ethnicity (White, average of three sources to acquire non-White), gender (male, female), and age information about the brain and would likely (20–29, over 29) on the three subscales. No continue to rely on three sources in the significant differences were found for any of future. The use of multiple sources, both in the three factors, indicating that the sample the present and future, indicates that these was homogeneous and that the findings are individuals, like many, appear to be applicable to all demographics included in interested in brain research. It seems as the study. though the “decade of the brain” is filtering into the lives of both preservice and Further, about 50% of the sample practicing educators. indicated a course as a past source of information about the brain. We wondered if When asked about sources they have educators who had learned about the brain typically used in the past, 64% of the from a formal source (i.e., a course) might participants indicated that they relied on the have more positive beliefs in regard to brain Internet, also the most frequently listed research in education. MANOVA used to source for future information (74%). determine the effect of having taken a Considering the participants’ ages and course as a source of information about the educational levels, along with the times in brain (took a course, did not take a course) which we live, it seems reasonable that the on the three subscales revealed significant Internet was the most frequently indicated differences between the two groups, F(3, source of information. There is an incredible 211) = 8.13, p < .01. Analyses of variance amount of information about brain research (ANOVA) on each dependent variable were and education on the World Wide Web. A conducted as follow-up tests. The ANOVA Google search for brain + education results was significant for prepare, F(1, 213) = in millions of hits, for example, PowerBrain 11.61, p < .01, with means of 4.80 and 4.47 Education and the Education Connection. for those who took a course about the brain compared with those who did not. The The second most common past source results for better and concerns were not was television (53%). Public television and significant. cable channels offer programs on health and Discussion wellness that many times focus on the brain, As instructors we became aware of the learning, or both. A Google search of PBS + debates concerning the relevance of brain brain reveals PBS programs, such as, Secret research for educators, were seeing more Life of the Brain; The Brain Fitness and more information about the brain being Program, Frontline: Inside the Teenage brought into educational psychology Brain; and Change Your Brain, Change textbooks, and began witnessing varied Your Life. In addition, brain research is levels of enthusiasm from our students about becoming a common topic in the news and brain research. This experience led us to as a result, television may be an incidental wonder if, and how much, information from source of information, not sought out but Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 47 present in a program being watched for Follow-up MANOVA results provided other reasons. However, the proportion of further insight. The effect of having taken a those reporting that they would use course as a past source of information had a television as a future source of information significant effect on the prepare subscale. was only 18%. A similar decline is seen in Participants who had taken a course agreed the use of magazines and radio. significantly more (4.80) that information about the brain should be included in teacher In regard to the three subscales, the education than those who had not (4.47). future teachers felt somewhat strong agreement (4.69) that learning about the Implications for Teachers of brain could make them better teachers, Educational Psychology especially in regard to learning differences. Understanding future teachers use the There was also somewhat strong agreement Internet as a primary source of information for the subscale of prepare (4.63). These about the brain could be of value for those future teachers believe that their preparation interested in their training and this includes should include information about the brain, those teaching educational psychology. The especially practical strategies based on brain brain is a complex organ and simple, quick research, because they will improve their understandings provided on the Internet do instruction. Just as Berninger and Richards not help them understand how the brain (2003) believed teachers have a right to learns or struggles to learn. Therefore it is learn about the brain, it seems these important that future teachers be introduced participants believed the same. They and directed to reputable Web sites with believed information about the brain should valid information. Examples of these include be made available as part of the coursework Brain Connection they receive. (www.brainconnection.com) and the Dana Alliance for Brain Initiatives The participants’ beliefs in regard to (www.dana.org/braincenter). These groups concerns were neutral (3.56). These future are working alongside neuroscientists to teachers neither agreed nor disagreed about translate what is being found into practical misunderstanding or misinterpreting ideas. We propose that if future teachers are information from brain research. They were given this information they will think less inclined to be concerned about the use critically about what they are being sold and of brain research to diagnose children and what they are doing in their classrooms. provide enrichment. These participants have Using Web sites like these, along with neutral concerns that brain research is being information in educational psychology overstated or that its use will cause textbooks and theories like information additional financial burdens on schools. The processing, could provide the solid item that was agreed to most strongly in this information students need. subscale was, “There is misunderstanding among educators about how brain research The participants in this study believe applies to education” (77%). This may be an they are capable of understanding how the indication of these future teachers’ brain learns and that this should be part of awareness that there is much information the education and professional development available that is either misunderstood or they receive. They want information so that false. they may provide good instruction based on science and research. A second implication Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2 Thoughts about Brain Research 48 is that future teachers need to be taught how As instructors we recognize there is a lot to take valid information and transfer it into to teach in a short amount of time and that what they do with children. New discoveries standards often drive our work. However, about the brain and how it learns are being there is no denying that information from made every day. These discoveries are neuroscience is seeping into our daily lives, challenging our beliefs and enriching what the lives of our students, and into the we know about teaching, learning, courses we teach. This study raised issues motivation, and human behavior. It seems about information sources and perceptions reasonable for future teachers to receive this of future teachers, and this is important latest information in courses that help them because teachers hold the learning of our not only to learn the information from brain nation’s children in their hands. Preservice research but also to transform it into teachers should receive the best and most practice. Many educational psychology up-to-date information available, and they textbooks contain good facts, and these should be taught to be critical of unreliable courses would be a good place for this information being sold for profit. Even if information. Ways to blend ideas from standards do not contain this information cognitive science, development, and they do propose best practices and critical motivation with neuroscience would provide thinking. Information from this study may up-to-date information and help students help those concerned with professional discern facts from fallacies. development, especially those who teach educational psychology courses. References Berninger, V. W., & Richards, T. L. (2003). education? Westminister Studies in Brain literacy for educators and Education, 26, 7–20. psychologists. San Diego, CA: Nadesan, M. H. (2005). Constructing autism Academic Press. unraveling the ‘truth’ and understanding the social. New York: Bruer, J. T. (1999). In search of . . . brain- Routledge. based education. Phi Delta Kappan, Ormrod, J.E. (2008). Educational 80(9), 648–657. psychology: Developing learners (6th Bruer, J. T. (2006). Points of view: On the ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill implications of neuroscience Prentice Hall. research for science teaching and Perry, B. (2007). The boy who was raised as learning: Are there any? A skeptical a dog and other stories from a child theme and variations: The primacy psychiatrist’s notebook: What of psychology in the science of traumatized children can teach us learning. CBE-Life Sciences about loss, love, and healing. New Education, 5, 104–110. York: Basic Books. Byrnes, J., & Fox, N. (1998). The Restak, R. (2001). The secret life of the educational relevance of research in brain. Washington, DC: Joseph cognitive neuroscience. Educational Henry Press. Psychology Review, 10, 297–342. Shaywitz, S. (2005). Overcoming dyslexia. Geake, J., & Cooper, P. (2003). Cognitive New York: Vintage. neuroscience: Implications for Spring, 2009 Teaching Educational Psychology 5:2

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.