JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2009, 42, 375–380 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2009) THREE VERSUS SIX REREADINGS OF PRACTICE PASSAGES SCOTT P. ARDOIN UNIVERSITYOFGEORGIA AND JESSICA C. WILLIAMS, CYNTHIA KLUBNIK, AND MICHAEL MCCALL UNIVERSITYOFSOUTHCAROLINA Althoughtheliteratureclearlydemonstratesthatrepeatedreadingsresultinimmediateeffectson students’ performance on the intervention materials as well as long-term benefits, data are less promising regarding its immediate generalization effects to similar materials. Using an alternating treatments design, the current study evaluated the effects of a multicomponent repeatedreadinginterventionongeneralizationpassagesafterstudentshadreadapassagethree versussixtimes.Resultsindicatedimprovementsinfluencyasaresultofbothinterventions,with slightlygreatermaintenanceeffectswhenstudentsweregivensixopportunitiestoreadpassages. DESCRIPTORS: generalization, reading fluency, repeatedreadings _______________________________________________________________________________ Reading fluency is a key component of high percentage of the same words. For effective reading instruction, because it is a instance, intervention failed to result in accept- necessary, although not sufficient, skill for able levels of generalization on generalization accomplishing the ultimate goal of comprehen- passagesfor3ofthe4studentsinonestudyand sion. One instructional practice for which there 2 of the 3 students in a second study (Daly, is substantial evidence in both the behavioral Martens, Dool, & Hintze, 1998; Daly, Mar- and the cognitive literature for improving tens, Hamler, Dool, & Eckert, 1999, respec- students’ reading fluency is repeated readings tively). Thus, it is not clear from these studies (RR). The studies that have examined multi- that providingintervention on thefirsthalfof a component RR interventions have demonstrat- story would have substantial, immediate, and ed repeatedly that providing students with beneficial effects for the student when reading modeling in the form of listening passage the latter half of the same story. preview, multiple opportunities to practice Failure of multicomponent RR interventions passages, corrective feedback, and contingencies to lead to substantial gains on generalization for improved reading can substantially increase passages led Ardoin et al. (2007) to evaluate students’ fluency on practice passages (e.g., alternative procedures to promote generaliza- Ardoin, McCall, & Klubnik, 2007; Eckert, tion. Ardoin et al. compared the effects of Ardoin, Daly, & Martens, 2002; Gortmaker, allowingstudentstoreadonepassagefourtimes Daly, McCurdy, Persampieri, & Hergenrader, and a multiple-exemplar condition in which 2007). Unfortunately, evidence from these students read two high-word-overlap passages studies indicates that observed effects on twice each. Results showed that although the practice passages do not necessarily result in multiple-exemplar condition provided repeated generalization effects on similar passages with a exposure to the same words in a different context, four readings of one passage allowed Address correspondence to Scott P. Ardoin, University greater generalization to new passages. This of Georgia, Department of Educational Psychology and suggested that an increased number of reread- Instructional Technology, 325L Aderhold Hall, Athens, ings may be the most important independent Georgia30622. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-375 variable for generalization to occur. The 375 376 SCOTT P. ARDOIN et al. purpose of the current study was, therefore, to 3 s. The median number of words read extend the literature by evaluating whether correctly in 1 min (WRCM) was used to doubling the treatment dose of a multicompo- determine each participant’s instructional nent RR intervention would lead to greater level. Shane and Roland received intervention generalization and maintenance effects on on first-grade passages because their WRCM similar generalization passages. Because we fell just below the recommended instructional wished to evaluate differences in dosage, it was level for first-grade materials (40 to 60 important to select a set number of repeated WRCM; Shapiro, 1996). Similarly, James’s readings, as opposed to having students reread reading of second-grade passages fell just each passage until they met a preestablished below the instructional level for second-grade criterion, as is sometimes done in the literature. materials (also 40 to 60 WRCM; Shapiro); RR procedures vary, with some studies requir- thus,hereceivedinterventiononsecond-grade ing a minimum of two readings and a passages. The survey-level assessment indicat- maximum of four readings (Therrien, Wick- ed that Seth’s performance on third-grade strom, & Jones, 2006) and others specifying material was just over the minimum WRCM three or four rereadings (Ardoin, Eckert, & for the third-grade level (70 to 100 WRCM; Pender, 2008; Daly et al., 1998; Manlanga, Shapiro) and fell far below the instructional 2003). Given this variability, we established the range for fourth-grade materials. Seth there- single dosage to be three rereadings. fore received intervention in third-grade materials. Thirteenrandomlyselectedpassagesfromthe METHOD first-, second-, and third-grade levels of the Participants were 4 boys who resided in and Silver Burdett Ginn (1991) basal reading series received their education at a residential facility were selected as practice passages. Generaliza- for troubled youth and had been identified as tion passages were developed by writing new being likely to benefit from a fluency-based passages that contained a large portion of the intervention (Shane was 11 years 8 months old words that were included in the practice andinfourthgrade,Sethwas12years6months passages. Passage overlap was calculated by old and in fifth grade, Roland was 7 years 5 dividing the total number of words that monthsoldandinsecondgrade,andJameswas appeared in both passages by the total number 7 years 10 months old and in second grade). of words in the generalization passage (Daly & Shane had been identified with an educational Martens, 1994). Practice passages ranged in disability (learning disabled, language). All length from 95 to 179 words across grades sessions were conducted at a desk placed in a (first-gradeM5115,range,95to154;second- quiet hallway outside the participants’ class- grade M 5 117, range, 90 to 153; third-grade rooms. One intervention session was conducted M 5 152, range, 124 to 179), and generaliza- per day. tion passages ranged in length from 83 to 161 across grades (first-grade M 5 99, range, 83 to Materials 128; second-grade M 5 98, range, 85 to 130; A survey-level assessment was conducted to third-grade M 5 102, range, 82 to 161). determine the appropriate instructional level at Percentage overlap between generalization and which to implement the intervention. This practice passages ranged from 78% to 95% involved administering sets of three passages at across grades (first-grade M 5 87%, range, successive levels of difficulty for 1 min while an 78% to 98%; second-grade M 5 86%, range, examiner recorded reading errors and provided 78% to 98%; third-grade M 5 89%, range, words to students on which they hesitated for 83% to 95%). THREE VERSUS SIX REREADINGS 377 Given the failure of readability formulas to thestudentrepeatthesyllableasmodeledbythe account for passage difficulty (Ardoin, Suldo, examiner, repeat the syllables at a faster pace, Witt, Aldrich, & McDonald, 2005), practice and finally blending the syllables so that they passages were assigned to conditions based on read the word at a normal pace (Ardoin et al., their level of difficulty, as determined by each 2007). student’s preassessment reading of the passages WRCM was assessed during each practice (Christ & Ardoin, 2009). Across 3 days, reading. During each reading, the experimenter students read 13 practice passages at their counted a word as read correct if it was instructional level. Students read each passage pronounced correctly given the context of the for 1 min without error correction. A student’s passage within 3 s or incorrect if pronounced WRCM on each passage was used to match incorrectly and not self-corrected within 3 s. passages according to difficulty. Then, one Althoughstudentswereallowedtoreadpractice passage from each pair was randomly assigned passages for 2 min, WRCM was assessed only to a condition. During this preassessment, during the first minute of reading. Students students earned a token for exceeding a were allowed to read the passage for 2 min predetermined score. Every two tokens were insteadofonly1 mintoincreasetheprobability exchangeable for a tangible reward. that they would be able to read the entire passage during each practice reading. Immedi- Procedure and Design ately following three or six rereadings (depend- Using a rapid reversal design, differences in ing on condition), the participant was prompt- students’ fluency on practice and generalization ed to read the corresponding generalization passages were evaluated as a function of two passage. One week later, the participant was multicomponent RR interventions. The inter- prompted to read the same generalization ventions differed only in whether students passage. Data were collected on WRCM as reread passages three (RR3) or six (RR6) times. described above. Participants read generaliza- Components of the interventions included (a) tion passages for only 1 min, and error- the passage being read to the student, (b) the correction procedures were not implemented. studentgiveneitherthreeorsixopportunitiesto Participants were given a token each time they read the passage aloud for 2 min, (c) the exceeded their preassessment WRCM score on studentbeingtoldaftereachreading howmany the corresponding practice passage and each words he read correctly in 1 min and incor- time they exceeded their WRCM on a rectly during the 2-min period, and (d) error- generalization passage from the first reading of correction procedures at the end of each the corresponding practice passage. Every two reading. Error-correction procedures involved tokens were exchangeable for a tangible reward phrase drill error correction and syllable (e.g., pencil, eraser). segmenting and blending. Phrase drill error correction involved the experimenter correctly Using the identical procedural checklists reading the word misread by the student, the employed by experimenters, independent ob- student correctly reading the word, and then servers listened to an audio recording of over reading the three- to five-word phrase contain- 33% of all session types to assess procedural ing the misread word. Syllable segmenting and integrity. Mean procedural integrity across blending were provided on words that students participantswas100%.Interobserveragreement misread in two separate readings of the practice was also examined by independent experiment- passage. Syllable segmenting and blending ers who listened to audiotapes of the same involved the experimenter modeling the indi- sessionsassessedforproceduralintegrity.Agree- vidualsyllables of the missed word, then having ment was calculated by dividing the number of 378 SCOTT P. ARDOIN et al. Figure1. Wordsreadcorrectlyin1min(WRCM)forShane,Seth,Roland,andJamesoneachsuccessivereadingof thepractice passages during theRR3andRR6conditions (represented bythedata points connected withlines). Data also illustrate students’ WRCM on corresponding generalization passages on intervention days (represented by the disconnected data points).The horizontallinesrepresent students’ mean preassessment WRCM onRR3(dashed line) andRR6(solidline) practice passages. words agreed on as correct and incorrect by the tion passages was not as high as the final total number of words read times 100%. Mean performancesonrespectivepracticepassages,it agreement across participants was 99% (range, was substantially higher than preassessment 83% to 100%). performanceonthepracticepassages.Withthe exception of three sessions for Roland, both interventions resulted in all participants read- RESULTS AND DISCUSSION ing all generalization passages in the instruc- Both multicomponent RR interventions tional range (40 to 60 WRCM), with Seth’s resultedinsubstantialincreasesinparticipants’ fluencyongeneralizationpassagesoccasionally WRCMonpracticepassagescomparedtotheir approaching mastery (.100 WRCM). Nota- preassessment performances on these passages bly, performances on the generalization pas- (seeFigure 1).Witheachsuccessivereadingof sagesadministeredimmediatelyafterinterven- a practice passage, participants’ WRCM on tion did not differ as a function of condition, that passage increased. Doubling the number suggesting that doubling the number of of reading opportunities usually resulted in reading opportunities did not provide greater students reading practice passages with greater generalization of fluent reading to new but fluency. Although performance on generaliza- similar passages. THREE VERSUS SIX REREADINGS 379 Table 1 Mean WordsReadCorrectly in1 Min(WRCM) on PracticePassages at Prereading, Across Greatest WRCM per Session,andAcrossLastReadingperSession;andMeanWRCMonHighWordOverlapPassagesonInterventionDay andat 1-WeekMaintenance Practicepassages Generalizationpassages GreatestWRCM Lastreading Prereading persession (Reading3or6) Interventionday Maintenance Shane RR3 37.4 81 80.6 52.8 72.2 RR6 38 98 98 57.6 77.2 Seth RR3 76.75 102.2 100.2 87.8 95 RR6 76.2 118.60 118.6 87.2 104.25 Roland RR3 27.33 58.17 58.17 47.83 44.17 RR6 27.66 66.17 66.17 46 49 James RR3 39.33 81.5 86.5 64 66.67 RR6 38.67 99.17 99.17 65.17 78 Shane’s, Seth’s, and James’s performances on words shared between practice and generaliza- generalization passages read 1 week after tion passages resulting in greater generalization intervention exceeded their performances on and maintenance effects. However, there are generalization passages administered immedi- many skills necessary for fluent reading (e.g., ately after intervention. Interestingly, in two phonemic awareness, sophisticated decoding cases (Shane and Seth), performances on skills), and more research on stimulus control generalization passages read 1 week after intheacquisitionofreadingfluencyisnecessary intervention approached performances on the before any conclusions can be drawn. corresponding practice passages on intervention A limitation inherent in this and other day (see Table 1). These results are consistent studies of repeated readings is the variability with those of Martens et al. (2007). Greater of passage difficulty within and across condi- fluency on generalization passages administered tions. Unfortunately, procedures do not cur- 1 week after intervention compared to imme- rently exist by which passage difficulty can be diatelyfollowinginterventionmaybeanartifact assessed to ensure that passages are exactly of fatigue after having already read practice equivalent in level of difficulty (Christ & passages multiple times. However, the upward Ardoin, 2009). A large portion of the variance trend in WRCM across sessions during RR3 in student performance within and across and RR6 suggests that students were not conditions could be a function of variation in fatigued. Therefore, it is unclear why perfor- passage difficulty. In an attempt to control mance at maintenance exceeded performance variability in the difficulty of passages, we on same-day generalization probes. preassessed passage difficulty to assign passages Interestingly, mean WRCM on generaliza- to the two intervention conditions. Unfortu- tion passages 1 week after intervention was nately, preassessment of passage difficulty was greater overall for the generalization passages conducted only on practice passages. One associated with the RR6 condition (Table 1). It cannot be certain of the equivalence between is possible that the RR6 condition promoted practice and generalization passages within the development of greater stimulus control for conditions or of the equivalence of generaliza- 380 SCOTT P. ARDOIN et al. tion passages across conditions. Although exact Daly,E.J.III,&Martens,B.K.(1994).Acomparisonof threeinterventionsforincreasingoralreadingperfor- equivalence between practice passages and mance: Application of the instructional hierarchy. corresponding generalization passages cannot JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 27,459–469. be assumed, an overlap of 78% to 98% of the Daly, E. J. III, Martens, B. K., Dool, E. J., & Hintze, J. M. (1998). Using brief functional analysis to select words between corresponding passages suggests interventions for oral reading. Journal of Behavioral a high degree of correspondence in passage Education,8, 203–218. difficulty. Other factors that could influence Daly,E.J.III,Martens,B.K.,Hamler,K.R.,Dool,E.J., passage difficulty (e.g., sentence length) were &Eckert,T.L.(1999).Abriefexperimentalanalysis for identifying instructional components needed to not controlled, however. Although the lack of implement oral reading fluency. Journal of Applied preassessment on the generalization passages BehaviorAnalysis,32,83–94. may be viewed as a limitation of the study, Eckert,T.L.,Ardoin,S.P.,Daly,E.J.III,&Martens,B. K. (2002). Improving oral reading fluency: A brief assessing passage difficulty on generalization experimental analysis of combining an antecedent passages would have resulted in the potential of intervention with consequences. Journal of Applied practice effects as an explanation for the gains BehaviorAnalysis,35,271–281. Gortmaker,V.J.,Daly,E.J.,McCurdy,M.,Persampieri, on generalization passages. Future studies could M.J.,&Hergenrader,M.(2007).Improvingreading compare generalization performance both with outcomesforchildrenwithlearningdisabilities:Using and without a preassessment of generalization briefexperimentalanalysistodevelopparent-tutoring interventions.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,40, passagestoallowtheevaluationoftheimpactof 203–221. preassessment on performance when generaliza- Manlanga, P. (2003). Using repeated readings and error tion passages are read. correction to build reading fluency with at risk elementarystudents.JournalofPrecisionTeachingand Celeration, 19(2),19–27. REFERENCES Martens,B.K.,Eckert,T.L.,Begeny,J.C.,Lewandowski, L. J., DiGennaro, F. D., Montarello, S. A., et al. Ardoin, S. P., Eckert, T. L., & Pender, C. A. S. (2008). (2007).Effectsofafluency-building programonthe Promotinggeneralizationofreading:Acomparisonof reading performance of low-achieving second and twofluency-basedinterventionsforimprovinggeneral thirdgradestudents.JournalofBehavioralEducation, education students’ oral reading rate. Journal of 16,39–54. Behavioral Education,17,237–252. Shapiro,E.S.(1996).Academicskillsproblems.NewYork: Ardoin, S. P., McCall, M., & Klubnik, C. (2007). Guilford. Promoting generalization of oral reading fluency: Silver Burdett Ginn. (1991). World of reading. Morris- Providing drill versus practice opportunities. Journal town,NJ:Author. ofBehavioral Education, 16,55–70. Therrien, W. J., Wickstrom, K., & Jones, K. (2006). Ardoin, S. P., Suldo, S. M., Witt, J. C., Aldrich, S., & Effect of a combined repeated reading and question McDonald, E. (2005). Accuracy of readability generation intervention on reading achievement. estimates’ predictions of CBM performance. School Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21(2), Psychology Quarterly,20,1–22. 89–97. Christ, T. J., & Ardoin, S. P. (2009). Curriculum-based measurement of oral reading: Passages equivalence Received October 8,2007 andprobe-setdevelopment.JournalofSchoolPsychol- Final acceptance October15,2008 ogy,47,55–75. Action Editor,Stephanie Peterson