ebook img

ERIC EJ853187: The Effects of Teaching Precurrent Behaviors on Children's Solution of Multiplication and Division Word Problems PDF

2009·0.19 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ853187: The Effects of Teaching Precurrent Behaviors on Children's Solution of Multiplication and Division Word Problems

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2009, 42, 361–367 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2009) THE EFFECTS OF TEACHING PRECURRENT BEHAVIORS ON CHILDREN’S SOLUTION OF MULTIPLICATION AND DIVISION WORD PROBLEMS HEATHER B. LEVINGSTON AND NANCY A. NEEF THEOHIOSTATEUNIVERSITY AND TRACI M. CIHON THECHICAGOSCHOOLOFPROFESSIONALPSYCHOLOGY Weexaminedtheeffectsofteachingovertprecurrentbehaviorsonthecurrentoperantofsolving multiplicationanddivisionwordproblems.Twostudentsweretaughtfourprecurrentbehaviors (identificationoflabel,operation,largernumbers,andsmallernumbers)inadifferentorder,in thecontextofamultiplebaselinedesign.Aftermeetingcriteriononthreeofthefourprecurrent skills,thestudents demonstrated thecurrent operantof correctproblem solutions. Theseskills generalized to novel problems. Correct current operant responses (solutions that matched answers revealed by coloring over the space with a special marker) maintained the precurrent behaviors inthe absence ofany other programmed reinforcement. DESCRIPTORS: mathematics,precurrent behaviors, problem solving, word problems _______________________________________________________________________________ The National Council of Teachers of tional Progress, 1992) indicate a need for Mathematics (1989, 2000, 2005) has for many methods of instruction that can enhance years recommended an increased focus on performance of all students, particularly given problem-solving tasks, typically taught through the emphasis on educating students with theuseofwordproblems.TheNationalCenter disabilities in inclusive classrooms (Owen & for Educational Statistics reported, however, Fuchs, 2002). Nevertheless, relatively little that only 36% of fourth graders in the United research has been reported on procedures for States were performing at or above the teaching word-problem solving compared to proficient level in identifying and appropriately teaching basic foundation and computation using information to solve word problems skills, and most of the research has targeted (Perie, Grigg, & Dion, 2005). Students with students with learning disabilities (see Xin & developmentaldisabilities(e.g.,autism)lageven Jitendra, 1999). further behind (Cawley, Parmar, Foley, Salm- From an operant perspective, the behaviors on, & Roy, 2001; Parmar, Cawley, & Frazita, necessary to solve mathematics word problems 1996). The difficulties with word problem can be classified as precurrents. Precurrent solving demonstrated by students across ability behaviors are those that increase the effective- and age levels (National Assessment of Educa- ness of a subsequent (current) behavior in obtainingareinforcer(Polson&Parsons,1994; Skinner, 1968). Once established, precurrent This study was part of a master’s thesis conducted by thefirstauthor.WethankRonDeMuesy,AmandaGuld, behaviors can be reinforced and maintained by and Kathleen Heron for their assistance with assessments the correct performance of the current behavior ofinterobserver agreement andprocedural integrity. (Parsons, 1976; Skinner). For example, the Address correspondence to Nancy A. Neef, School of PAES, College of Education and Human Ecology, The current behavior of getting to a particular OhioStateUniversity,PAESBldg.,17thAve.,Columbus, destination can reinforce and maintain the Ohio43210(e-mail: [email protected]). precurrent behavior of reading a map. Neef, doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-361 361 362 HEATHER B. LEVINGSTON et al. Nelles, Iwata, and Page (2003) taught four old typically developing girl who was enrolled precurrent behaviors for addition and subtrac- in a regular education summer school program. tion word problems (identifying the initial During a preexperimental skills assessment, value, change value, operation, and resulting both participants (a) calculated correct answers value) in a sequential manner to 2 young adults for at least 80% of multiplication problems with developmental disabilities. Once the pre- from 1 3 10 to 10 3 10, (b) calculated correct current behaviors were established, the number answers for at least 80% of division problems of correct problem solutions increased. These with a maximum dividend of 100, and (c) behaviors generalized to untaught problems. discriminated larger and smaller numbers from The current investigation replicated the Neef amongatleast80%ofpairsbetween1and100. etal.(2003)studyandextendedtheresearchon Nevertheless, both participants demonstrated instruction in mathematical problem solving in difficulty in solving word problems and were several ways. First, we systematically replicated nominated by their parents and a classroom theproceduresusedbyNeefetal.withyounger teacher for participation in the study. Teaching (elementaryaged)students,1withautismand1 sessionswereconductedindividuallybyanother without disabilities. Very few, if any, studies on teacher(firstauthor)atatableinthebackofthe teaching mathematical problem solving have participants’ classrooms. targeted students with autism, who often have Stimulus Materials intact computational skills but difficulty in discriminating the type of operation or ap- A bank of 300 multiplication and division proach to use to solve word problems (Min- word problems was created using the equations shew,Goldstein,Taylor,&Siegel,1994;Siegel, A 3 B 5 C and A 4 B 5 C derived from a Goldstein, & Minshew, 1996). Second, we review of third- and fourth-grade mathematics assessed the relation between precurrent and curricula (e.g., ‘‘Nathan’s wall is 12 feet wide. current behaviors with word problems that How many posters can he hang on his wall if involvedmultiplicationanddivisionoperations. each poster is 2 feet wide?’’). Variations were Almostalloftheinvestigationsofinstructionon created by substituting proper names, nouns, word-problem solving with students with verbs, and numbers. Multiplication problems disabilities have targeted addition and subtrac- had products of 100 or less, and division tion problems. Third, we implemented a self- problems had quotients of 10 or less without checking procedure to reduce the instructional remainders. Each worksheet had 10 problems demands on the teacher and to determine the drawn randomly from the bank. Each problem extent to which a matching answer (i.e., the was used only once during the study. Correct current operant of correct problem solution) numbers for the word-problem equations on was sufficient to maintain precurrent behaviors the training worksheets were written below the in the absence of other sources of reinforce- corresponding spaces with a color-changing ment. Finally, we assessed generalization to marker and were invisible until colored over novelwordproblemsintheabsenceofspacesas with a developing marker. stimulus prompts for the parts of the equation. Experimental Design A multiple baseline design across behaviors METHOD was used to examine theeffects of teaching four Participants and Setting precurrent behaviors (identification of label, Matt was a 10-year-old boy with a diagnosis operation, larger number, and smaller number) of autism who was enrolled in an inclusive on the current behavior (correct solution) of fourth-grade classroom. Maddix was a 10-year- word problems. Precurrent behaviors were PRECURRENT BEHAVIORS AND WORD PROBLEMS 363 Table 1 Prompts andCorrect andIncorrect Responses for Prompted Teaching Trials Identificationoflabel Prompt ‘‘Whatdotheywantyoutofind?Howshouldyoulabelyouranswer?Writeyouransweronthe line[whilepointingtotheline].’’ Correct Labelwrittenonthefinalline Incorrect Incorrectword,incorrectplacementofword,noresponsewithin10s Identificationofoperation Prompt ‘‘Doyouneedtomultiplytofind[howmuchorhowmany]altogetherordoyouneedto dividebybreakingthewholeintoparts?Writeyouranswerinthebox.’’ Correct Correctsymbolinbox Incorrect Incorrectsymbol;incorrectplacementofsymbol;noresponsewithin10s Identificationoflargernumber Prompt ‘‘Whichnumberisthelargest?Writeyouranswerontheline.’’ Correct Largestnumberonfirstline Incorrect Incorrectnumber;incorrectplacementofnumber;noresponsewithin10s Identificationofsmallernumber Prompt ‘‘Whichnumberisthesmallest?Writeyouranswerontheline.’’ Correct Smallestnumberonsecondline Incorrect Incorrectnumber;incorrectplacementofnumber;noresponsewithin10s trained in a different order for the 2 partici- except for the target precurrent behavior and pants. Continued baseline measurement of the the current behavior. The solution was pre- current operant permitted assessment of the written in invisible ink. The teacher read the effects of sequential acquisition of each pre- problem aloud and delivered the corresponding current behavior on the current operant. verbal prompt (Table 1). She then identified and modeled the correct response. This con- Teaching Procedure sisted of underlining and stating either the Typically,onetotwo10-to15-minteaching specificwordsintheproblemthatindicatedthe sessions were conducted 5 days per week solution’s label (identification of label), the throughouttheintervention.Teachingincluded operation (identification of operation), or the up to four phases, each corresponding to number (identification of larger number and instruction in a precurrent behavior that had smaller number) depending on the specific notyetbeenacquired.Instructionineachofthe precurrent behavior being taught. Next, the four phases (identification of label, operation, teacher wrote the correct response in its larger number, and smaller number, respective- corresponding space. She then solved the ly) occurred as needed and followed the same resulting number sentence and entered the procedure (described below). For each precur- current operant on the final answer space. rent behavior, participants were taught to write Finally, the teacher modeled a self-checking the response in the corresponding space of an procedure by coloring under the answer space equation below the word problem. Prompts for with a developing marker that revealed the eachprecurrentbehavior,alongwithdefinitions current operant (solution), and read the of correctand incorrect responses, are described resulting number sentence aloud. in Table 1. Following modeling, the teacher presented At the beginning of each phase, the teacher the participant with a worksheet that contained demonstrated the target precurrent behavior on 10 different word problems and corresponding a worksheet with five practice problems. equations with blank spaces for the precurrent Practice worksheets contained a word problem beingtaught,anypreviouslytaughtprecurrents, and an equation with all elements filled in and the current operant (i.e., the solution). The 364 HEATHER B. LEVINGSTON et al. participant was instructed to complete the plication or division) that would lead to the equation with a written response in the solution, the larger number referenced in the corresponding spaces for (a) the precurrent problem, the smaller number referenced in the behavior being taught, (b) all previously taught problem, and the solution to the equation. precurrent behaviors (e.g., identification of During baseline and intervention probes, par- label, operation, larger number, or smaller ticipants wrote their answers below the word number), and (c) the solution, and to then problem on lines and boxes that corresponded checkthesolutionusingthedevelopingmarker. toeachofthefourprecurrentbehaviorsandthe Teaching trials began with guided instruction current behavior. No consequences were deliv- during which the teacher presented the prompt ered for correct or incorrect responses. After all (Table 1) for the precurrent behavior being precurrent behaviors had met mastery criterion, trained. Previously taught precurrent behaviors a generalization probe was conducted in the were not prompted. After completion of the same manner, except that the worksheets did self-check procedure, the teacher prompted not contain lines for numbers and labels or a correction of any inaccurate responses, if box for the operation. necessary, by underlining the appropriate part of the word problem. Data Collection When the participant achieved criterion of at Data were collected on the written responses least 90% correct for the target precurrent during teaching trials and probes. Correct behavior, the subsequent session used un- responses were defined, respectively, as having prompted trials. The participant was given the thecorrectnumbersintheappropriateorderfor worksheet to complete and check independent- larger number and smaller number, the correct ly. When the participant met a criterion of at operation symbol separating the larger and least 90% correct for two consecutive sessions smaller numbers, and the correctly labeled with unpromptedtrials on the targetprecurrent solution following the equal sign. Labels were behavior, a probe was conducted (described countedascorrectiftheycorrespondedwiththe below). Teaching was then initiated on another noun represented in the math problem. Any precurrent behavior (unless probe data indicat- other response (or no response) was scored as ed an increasing trend, in which case probes incorrect. continued until performance was stable). If the participant did not meet criterion during a Interobserver Agreement session with unprompted trials, the teacher Two graduate students in special education returned to guided instruction with prompted independentlyscoredstudentresponseson31% trials until the participant again met criterion. of the training and probe sessions across all conditions for each participant. Point-by-point Probes interobserver agreement was calculated by Probes were conducted before training (base- dividing the number of agreements on each line), each time the student met training component of a problem by the number of criterion on a target precurrent behavior agreements plus disagreements and multiplying (intervention),andafterallprecurrentbehaviors by 100%. Mean agreement scores were 98.7% had been taught (generalization). During each or above (range, 80% to 100%) for both 20-min probe session, the teacher gave the trainingandprobesessionsforeachparticipant. participant a worksheet that contained 10 word problems. Completion of each word problem Procedural Integrity consisted of writing the object of the word Procedural integrity was assessed on 36% of problem (label), the correct operation (multi- alltrainingandprobesessionsbythreegraduate PRECURRENT BEHAVIORS AND WORD PROBLEMS 365 Figure 1. The percentage of correct responses during mathematics probes across baseline, intervention, and generalizationconditions for Matt (left)andMaddix (right). students in special education who had been RESULTS AND DISCUSSION trained to a criterion of at least 90% Figure 1 shows the percentage of correct agreement in scoring a sample videotaped precurrent and current responses during base- session using a procedural checklist of the line, intervention, and generalization probes behaviors outlined in the experimental meth- (dataforteachingsessionsareavailablefromthe ods. Procedural integrity was calculated by second author). For both participants, correct dividing the total number of steps performed responses for label, operation, and larger bytheteacherbythetotalnumberofstepsand number increased following training. Matt’s multiplying by 100%. The mean score was meanpercentagesofcorrectprecurrentrespons- 99.3% (range, 93% to 100%). es during baseline and intervention, respective- 366 HEATHER B. LEVINGSTON et al. ly,were13%and96%forlabel,52%and96% your work?’’; Naglieri & Gottling, 1995) that for operation, and 53% and 96% for larger havecharacterizedotherapproaches.Second,by number. Maddix’s mean percentages of correct teaching precurrent behaviors sequentially and precurrent responses during baseline and inter- probing performance as each was mastered, vention, respectively, were 60% and 90% for instruction was limited to those behaviors that operation, 15% and 98% for larger number, did not emerge as a result of training previous and 0% and 100% for label. Despite her low precurrents. Thus, once identification and baseline for larger and smaller numbers, she placement of the larger number in the equation often obtained a correct solution for multipli- wasmastered,studentswereabletodiscriminate cation problems that, unlike division problems, the smaller number, obviating the need for could be derived irrespective of the order of the training on that step, or on the solution. The two sets of numbers in the equation. For both self-checkingprocedure(coloringoverthespace participants, correct responding for the smaller with a disclosing marker to reveal the answers) number emerged following training on the enabled students to receive immediate feedback larger number and therefore was not trained. and reinforcement for correct responses while Following training on the first three precur- they worked independently, thereby further rents, correct solutions increased. Both partic- reducing demands on teacher time. Finally, ipants maintained high percentages of correct instruction resulted in generative problem responses on generalization probes (range, 90% solving (i.e., responding to novel stimuli in a to 100%). way that enabled solution of multiple types of The results replicate the findings of Neef et multiplication and division word problems). al. (2003) in that systematic instruction on Efficiency might be further enhanced by precurrent behaviors that represented the com- teaching the strategies in the context of group ponent parts of a problem was an effective instruction. method for establishing generative skills in the Problem solving involves an interresponse solution of arithmetic word problems. These relation in which the occurrence of precurrent results extend the generality of Neef et al. by behaviors makes the solution (current operant) demonstratingtheeffectivenessofthisapproach morelikely(Skinner,1953,1966,1968,1969); with a wider range of students (a student with the solution, in turn, may reinforce the autism and a typically developing student) and precurrent behaviors. Parsons (1976) and withwordproblemsthatinvolvedothertypesof Parsons, Taylor, and Joyce (1981) showed that operations (multiplication and division), which precurrent collateral behavior was maintained rarely have been targeted in research on when reinforcement was made contingent on a problem solving. Given the move toward subsequent current operant, and that the inclusion, there is a need for procedures that current operant decreased when collateral pre- are effective in addressing the goals of mathe- current behaviors were prohibited. In the maticsinstructionacross aspectrumof learners. present study, it was not possible (or desirable) In addition to being effective, instruction to reverse or prevent the precurrent behaviors should be efficient. The procedures used in the once they were acquired. However, precurrents current study were efficient in several respects. maintained at a high level of accuracy in the First, instruction was limited to the identifica- absence of any programmed reinforcement tion of the basic elements and operations other than producing the correct response involved in a problem without the use of (revealed by coloring over the space with a cognitive strategies that prompt self-reflection developing marker). This lends support to the (e.g., ‘‘What did you notice about how you did findings of Parsons and colleagues in demon- PRECURRENT BEHAVIORS AND WORD PROBLEMS 367 strating the relation between precurrent and Owen, R. L., & Fuchs, L. S. (2002). Mathematical problem-solving strategy instruction for third-grade currentbehaviors.Thecurrentstudysuggestsan students with learning disabilities. Remedial and approach that might be applied in future Special Education,23,268–278. research to teaching more complex discrimina- Parmar, R., Cawley, J., & Frazita, R. (1996). Word problem solving by students with and without mild tions (e.g., word problems that contain irrele- disabilities. Exceptional Children,62, 415–429. vant information) or advanced mathematical Parsons, J. A. (1976). Conditioning precurrent (problem skills (e.g., multistep problems or those with solving) behavior of children. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis,2,190–206. remainders) to many students. Parsons, J. A., Taylor, D. C., & Joyce, T. M. (1981). Precurrent self-prompting operants in children: ‘‘Remembering.’’ Journal of the Experimental Analysis REFERENCES of Behavior, 36,253–266. Perie, M., Grigg, W., & Dion, G. (2005). The nation’s Cawley,J.,Parmar,R.,Foley,T.E.,Salmon,S.,&Roy,S. report card: Mathematics 2005 (NCES 2006–453). (2001). Arithmetic performance of students: Impli- Washington,DC:U.S.GovernmentPrintingOffice. cations for standards and programming. Exceptional Polson, D. A. D., & Parsons, J. A. (1994). Precurrent Children,19, 124–142. contingencies: Behavior reinforced by altering rein- Minshew, N. J., Goldstein, G., Taylor, H. G., & Siegel, forcement probability for other behavior. Journal of D. J. (1994). Academic achievement in high- the Experimental AnalysisofBehavior, 61,427–439. functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical Siegel, D. J., Goldstein, G., & Minshew, N. J. (1996). andExperimental Neuropsychology,16,261–270. Designing instruction for the high-functioning autis- Naglieri, J. A., & Gottling, S. H. (1995). A study of tic individual. Journal of Developmental and Physical planning and mathematics instruction for students Disabilities,8,1–19. with learning disabilities. Psychological Reports, 76, Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New 1343–1354. York: FreePress. National Assessment of Educational Progress. (1992). Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem NAEP1992mathematicsreportcardforthenationand solving. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem solving: the states (Report No.23-ST02). Washington, DC: Research, method, and inquiry (pp. 225–257). New National Center for EducationalStatistics. York: Wiley. National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (1989). Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. New Curriculum and evaluation of standards for school York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. mathematics. Reston,VA:Author. Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2000). theoretical analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Curriculum and evaluation of standards for school Hall. mathematics. Reston,VA:Author. Xin, Y. P., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). The effects of National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2005). instruction in solving mathematical word problems Principlesandstandardsforschoolmathematics.Reston, forstudentswithlearningproblems:Ameta-analysis. VA:Author. The JournalofSpecial Education,32,207–225. Neef,N.A.,Nelles,D.,Iwata,B.A.,&Page,T.J.(2003). Analysis of precurrent skills in solving mathematics Received August20, 2007 story problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Final acceptanceMay 12,2008 36,21–33. Action Editor,Henry Roane

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.