ebook img

ERIC EJ808981: The Effects of Tact and Listener Training on the Emergence of Bidirectional Intraverbal Relations PDF

2008·0.13 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ808981: The Effects of Tact and Listener Training on the Emergence of Bidirectional Intraverbal Relations

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2008, 41, 411–415 NUMBER3 (FALL2008) THE EFFECTS OF TACT AND LISTENER TRAINING ON THE EMERGENCE OF BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBAL RELATIONS ´ ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR, ALMA RU´N OLAFSDO´TTIR, AND BERGLIND ARADO´TTIR UNIVERSITYOFAKUREYRI We evaluated the effects of 2 types of training on the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations with 4 typically developing children. Tact training involved reinforcing foreign- languagevocalizationsinthepresenceofvisualstimuli,andlistenertraininginvolvedreinforcing selectionsofvisual stimulifollowing vocalpresentations offoreign-language words. Intraverbal relations were tested by instructing participants to vocalize native-language equivalents of foreign-language words or vice versa. Both types of training produced increases in intraverbal responding, butthe emergent relations werenotalways bidirectional. DESCRIPTORS: emergent relations, foreign-language instruction, intraverbals, listener behavior,tacts _______________________________________________________________________________ Skinner (1957) described the intraverbal multiple tact relations; that is, verbal responses relation as a verbal response under the control in the presence of nonverbal stimuli (Skinner, of an antecedent verbal stimulus that does not 1957). Miguel et al. additionally trained the have point-to-point correspondence with the selectionofnonverbalstimuliinthepresenceof responseproduct.Intheappliedverbalbehavior verbal stimuli, sometimes termed listener rela- literature (e.g., Sundberg & Partington, 1998), tions, as did Petursdottir, Carr, Lechago, and this term has been used to describe a variety of Almason (2008). Across the three studies, tact educationally relevant outcomes of language and listener training had minimal effects on training, such as answering social questions, typically developing preschoolers’ vocal intra- reciting the alphabet, singing songs, telling verbal responding, suggesting that for young stories, and describing stimuli in their absence. children, activities such as labeling and identi- It has been argued that because academic and fying stimuli in picture books and videos may social behavior relies heavily on intraverbal not always suffice to expand intraverbal reper- repertoires, early childhood education should toires. However, all three studies investigated emphasize their establishment (Partington & training in the context of teaching categoriza- Bailey, 1993; Sundberg & Michael, 2001). tion skills. Having acquired relations between A few studies have investigated the extent to objects and their object names and category which young children may derive novel in- names, the participants were tested for the traverbal relations from the reinforcement of ability to emit either multiple object names other relations. Two studies (Miguel, Peturs- givenacategoryname(Migueletal.;Partington dottir, & Carr, 2005; Partington & Bailey, & Bailey) or the same category name given 1993) evaluated the the effects of training multiple object names (Petursdottir et al.). Thus, the relations were hierarchical (i.e., The second and third authors’ contribution should be ordered groupings within a system, as in the considered equal. We thank Mark Sundberg for helpful case of utensils [category] and knife [object comments. AddresscorrespondencetoAnnaIngeborgPetursdottir, within the category]). The present study was who is now at the Department of Psychology, Texas intended to provide a preliminary evaluation of Christian University, Box 298920, Fort Worth, Texas the effects of tact and listener training on the 76129(e-mail: [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-411 emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations 411 412 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al. inthecontextofalesscomplex,nonhierarchical delivered social praise contingent on correct task structure. To this effect, 4 children were responses in both types of training. If the child taught a small foreign-language vocabulary in made an incorrect response, the experimenter which a one-to-one relation existed between prompted a correct response vocally (tact native-language and foreign-language nouns. training) or by pointing (listener training). If the child did not make a response, the experimenter waited 10 s and then prompted METHOD a correct response. Training continued until Participants, Setting, and Materials 100% correct responding was achieved in three Ida, Emil, Lena, and Karen were 5 years old, consecutive trial blocks. spoke Icelandic as a native language, did not Intraverbal testing. We used a multiple speakasecondlanguage,andparticipatedinthe baseline design across stimulus sets to evaluate study with their parents’ permission. Sessions the effects of tact training (Ida and Emil) or lasted 20 to 30 min and were conducted three listener training (Karen and Lena) on the to five times a week over a period of 2 to 4 acquisition of two types of intraverbal respond- weeks. Sessions took place in an empty ing:Icelandic-SpanishintraverbalsandSpanish- preschool classroom, where the child and Icelandic intraverbals. Intraverbals were tested experimenter sat across from each other at a in baseline and following each training phase. child-sized table. Visual stimuli consisted of Each trial block consisted of six Icelandic- laminated color photographs (8 cm by 10 cm) Spanish and six Spanish-Icelandic trials. Trial ofsixanimalspicturedinanaturalenvironment presentation order varied across blocks. The and six fruits pictured against a white back- experimenter initiated a Spanish-Icelandic trial ground. Prior to the experiment, the children bypresentingtheIcelandicequivalentof‘‘What couldname(i.e.,tact)allofthevisualstimuliin does[Spanishname]mean?’’Acorrectresponse Icelandic and vocally imitate their Spanish was defined as vocalizing the Icelandic equiva- names. lent of the Spanish name. The experimenter Procedure initiated an Icelandic-Spanish trial by present- Tact and listener training. Emil and Ida ing the Icelandic equivalent of ‘‘What is received tact training, and Lena and Karen [Icelandic name] in Spanish?’’ A correct received listener training. Each child received response was defined as vocalizing the Spanish training sequentially on two six-stimulus sets equivalent of the Icelandic name. The experi- (fruitsand animals). Training wasconducted in menterdidnotpresentvisualstimuliduringtest six-trial blocks. On a tact training trial, the trials and did not deliver any consequences experimenter placed a visual stimulus on the following correct or incorrect responses. table in front of the child and asked the Data collection and interobserver agreement. Icelandic equivalentof ‘‘What is this [animal or During testing and training, the experimenter fruit]calledinSpanish?’’Acorrectresponsewas recordedcorrectandincorrectresponsesondata defined as vocalizing the Spanish name of the sheets. The experimenter recorded a correct animal or fruit displayed on the picture. On a response when the child emitted the target listener training trial, the experimenter placed vocalization or selected the target visual stimu- three visual stimuli (all animals or all fruits) on lus and recorded an incorrect response if the the table and asked the Icelandic equivalent of child vocalized the name of or selected a ‘‘Which [animal or fruit] is called [Spanish different stimulus or did not respond within name]?’’ A correct response was defined as 10 s of trial initiation. The experimenter scored touching the target stimulus. The experimenter the child’s first response if the child vocalized BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBALS 413 multiple fruit or animal names. A second resulted in only partial acquisition of the observer independently collected data on at intraverbal relations. least 40% of all testing and training trials for Both types of training appeared to produce each child. An agreement was defined as both greater effects on intraverbal responding than observers recording a correct response or both those observed in prior research on emergent recording an incorrect response. Otherwise, a vocal intraverbal relations (Miguel et al., 2005; disagreement was scored. Point-by-point inter- Partington & Bailey, 1993; Petursdottir et al., observer agreement for each trial block was 2008). This might suggest that young children calculated by dividing the number of agree- readily derive vocal intraverbal relations when ments by the sum of agreements and disagree- those relations are not hierarchically structured, ments and converting this ratio to a percentage. whereas a hierarchical structure might interfere During testing, mean interobserver agreement withtheiremergence.However,theparticipants was 100% for Emil, Ida, and Lena and 99% inthepresentstudywereslightlyolderorinthe (range, 92% to 100%) for Karen. Mean upper age range of those in earlier studies, and agreement during training exceeded 97% for as a result, developmental differences cannot be each child. ruled out. Although tact training appeared to have a stronger effect on the intraverbal RESULTS AND DISCUSSION repertoire than did listener training, these data should be interpreted with caution, because no Figure 1showsintraverbaltestingdataforall within-subject evaluation was conducted to participants. Correct intraverbals rarely oc- comparetheeffectsofthetwotypesoftraining. curred in baseline, but stable increases were In addition, the tact-trained children were observed following both types of training. The exposed to more training trials (M 5 53.7 trial tact-trained participants (Emil and Ida) per- formed with high accuracy (overall .90% blocks) than the listener-trained children (M 5 33.8 trial blocks) before they reached the correct) in Icelandic-Spanish trials on both stimulus sets. In addition, their performance in training criterion (acquisition data are available Spanish-Icelandic trials was highly accurate from the first author). (overall .90% correct) on the animals set, One finding of interest, however, is that butEmil’sperformance waslessaccurateonthe training did not necessarily result in equal fruits set. The listener-trained participants accuracy on both of the tested relations. When (Lena and Karen) did not reach .90% overall Lena was vocally presented with Spanish words levels of accuracy in either testing condition; following listener training, she emitted their however, Lena responded with 100% accuracy Icelandic equivalents with fairly high accuracy, in Spanish-Icelandic trials during the last two but when she was vocally presented with those blocks of testing on the animals set. Lena’s same Icelandicwords,sherarelyemittedcorrect performance in Icelandic-Spanish trials was Spanish responses. Also, following tact training consistently less accurate than her performance on his first stimulus set, Emil’s intraverbal in Spanish-Icelandic trials, whereas Karen responding was more accurate in Spanish than performed with similar accuracy in both in his native Icelandic language. These findings conditions. In sum, tact training reliably suggest that emergent intraverbal relations are resulted in the acquisition of an Icelandic- not necessarily bidirectional, along with the Spanish intraverbal repertoire and somewhat possibilitythatthetrainingofdifferentrelations less reliably in the acquisition of Spanish- (e.g., tacts vs. listener relations) may differen- Icelandic intraverbals. The effects of listener tiallyaffectwhichtypeofunidirectionalrelation training were more variable and typically emerges.Thisfindingmayberelatedtotherole 414 ANNA INGEBORG PETURSDOTTIR et al. Figure 1. The number of correct responses on Icelandic-Spanish and Spanish-Icelandic intraverbal test trials in baseline andfollowing the completion oftacttraining (toptwo panels) andlistener training (bottomtwo panels). BIDIRECTIONAL INTRAVERBALS 415 of the novel Spanish words as either responses by the visual stimulus on tact trials and by the or discriminative stimuli (SDs) during training relevant vocal stimulus (Icelandic fruit or and testing. Tact training directly established animal name) on Icelandic-Spanish trials. the Spanishvocalizations required on Icelandic- Exclusive intraverbal control by ‘‘Spanish’’ Spanishtesttrials butdidnotestablishstimulus would result in one of the six responses being control over any kind of responding by the correct by chance (assuming a repertoire of the Spanish names presented in Spanish-Icelandic six Spanish responses in the set) and therefore trials. Listener training, by contrast, established cannot explain why tacts were acquired to stimulus control by Spanish words over selec- criterion and Icelandic-Spanish intraverbals tion responses, but no Spanish vocalizations were typically emitted with 100% accuracy. were required. Keeping this in mind, a verbal More likely, instructions on test trials func- behavioranalysis(Skinner, 1957)mightsuggest tioned as contextual stimuli that prompted the that if during training, the children were to tact childtovocalizesomenameineitherSpanishor the visual stimuli in Icelandic prior to selecting Icelandic. them or tacting them in Spanish, those tacts alone could suffice to establish the intraverbal REFERENCES relationthatsharedacommonresponseformor a common SD with the trained relation. By Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the contrast, additional processes would be neces- Experimental AnalysisofBehavior,65, 185–241. sary for the emergence of the opposite relation. Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005). Althoughwedid not monitorthe occurrence of Theeffectsofmultiple-tactandreceptive-discrimina- tion training on the acquisition of intraverbal Icelandic tacts, it is possible that they occurred behavior.TheAnalysisofVerbalBehavior,21,27–41. eitherovertly orcovertly.Futureresearchmight Partington, J. W., & Bailey, J. S. (1993). Teaching examine further the role of existing tacts in the intraverbal behavior to preschool children. The Analysis ofVerbal Behavior,11,9–18. indirect establishment of novel intraverbal Petursdottir,A.I.,Carr,J.E.,Lechago,S.A.,&Almason, relations and explore practical and conceptual S. M. (2008). An evaluation of intraverbal training (cf. Horne & Lowe, 1996) implications of andlistenertrainingforteachingcategorizationskills. unidirectional versus bidirectional intraverbals. Journal ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 41, 53–68. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: It should be noted that the similarity of the Copley. instruction used on tact training trials and Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of Icelandic-Spanish test trials may have facilitated Skinner’sanalysisofverbalbehaviorforchildrenwith autism. BehaviorModification,25,698–724. responding on test trials as a result of the verbal Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching stimulus ‘‘Spanish’’ acquiring some degree of languagetochildrenwithautismorotherdevelopmental control over Spanish-language vocalizations. disabilities.PleasantHill,CA:BehaviorAnalysts,Inc. However, because this stimulus was present on Received March 13,2007 all tact and Icelandic-Spanish trials, the specific Final acceptanceJuly 24,2007 form of the response must have been controlled Action Editor,Bridget Taylor

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.