ebook img

ERIC EJ798589: Assessing Potency of High- and Low-Preference Reinforcers with Respect to Response Rate and Response Patterns PDF

2008·0.47 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ798589: Assessing Potency of High- and Low-Preference Reinforcers with Respect to Response Rate and Response Patterns

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2008, 41, 177–188 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2008) ASSESSING POTENCY OF HIGH- AND LOW-PREFERENCE REINFORCERS WITH RESPECT TO RESPONSE RATE AND RESPONSE PATTERNS BECKY PENROD, MICHELE D. WALLACE, AND EDWIN J. DYER UNIVERSITYOFNEVADA,RENO Previousresearchhassuggestedthattheavailabilityofhigh-preferencestimulimayoverridethe reinforcingefficacyofconcurrentlyavailablelow-preferencestimuliunderrelativelylowschedule requirements (e.g., fixed-ratio 1 schedule). It is unknown if similar effects would be obtained underhigherschedulerequirements.Thus,thecurrentstudycomparedhigh-preferenceandlow- preferencereinforcersunderprogressivelyincreasingschedulerequirements.Resultsfor3ofthe 4 participants indicated that high-preference stimuli maintained responding under higher schedulerequirementsrelativetolow-preferencestimuli.For1participant,high-preferenceand low-preference stimuli were demonstrated to be equally effective reinforcers under increasing schedule requirements. Implications with respect to rate of performance and response patterns arediscussed. DESCRIPTORS: positivereinforcement,preferenceassessments,progressiveratio,response effort _______________________________________________________________________________ A number of researchers have examined reinforcers than those that are identified as less methods for assessing stimulus preferences and preferred (e.g., chosen less than 20% of deciding the degree to which such preferences presentations). are predictive of the reinforcing effects of those Although stimuli identified as highly pre- stimuli (see Cannella, O’Reilly, & Lancioni, ferred (HP) have been shown to be more 2005, for a review). Various preference assess- effective reinforcers when compared to those ment methods differ in terms of the manner in that are less preferred (LP; e.g., Fisher et al., which the stimuli are presented, with stimuli 1992; Roane et al., 1998), recent investigations presented individually (Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, have evaluated the reinforcing efficacy of LP Iwata, & Page, 1985) or in pairs (Fisher et al., stimuli in the absence of HP stimuli more 1992), or with multiple stimuli presented closely. Roscoe, Iwata, and Kahng (1999) concurrently (DeLeon & Iwata, 1996; Roane, evaluated the reinforcing effectiveness of HP Vollmer, Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1998; Windsor, and LP stimuli identified through two prefer- Piche, & Locke, 1994). In general, preference ence assessments (based on the methods assessments produce a relative ranking of described by Pace et al., 1985, and Fisher et stimuluspreferencesthatisbasedontherelative al.). The relative reinforcing effects of HP and amount of time with which the stimuli were LP stimuli were evaluated in both single- and manipulated or the number of times one concurrent-operants arrangements. In the sin- stimulus was chosen relative to other stimuli. gle-operant arrangement only the LP stimulus An implication of such rankings is that stimuli was available, whereas the HP and LP stimuli that are more highly preferred (e.g., chosen on wereavailablesimultaneouslyintheconcurrent- 80% of presentations) are more effective operantsarrangement.ResultsindicatedthatLP stimuliwerenotaseffectiveasHPstimuliwhen Address correspondence to Michele D. Wallace, California State University, Los Angeles, Charter College both were available (i.e., concurrent-operants of Education, Division of Special Education and Coun- arrangement). However, when LP stimuli were seling,KHC1064,LosAngeles,California90032(e-mail: evaluatedinasingle-operantarrangement,these [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-177 stimuli produced increases in responding that 177 178 BECKY PENROD et al. were similar to levels observed with HP stimuli requirement completed; Hodos, 1961). Using in the concurrent arrangement. That is, when stimuli that were preferred similarly based on evaluated in a single-operant paradigm, the LP the results of a preference assessment (Fisher et stimuli were as effective as HP stimuli in terms al., 1992), Roane et al. showed that one of reinforcement efficacy, which suggested that stimulus was associated with greater response the concurrent availability of the HP stimuli persistence than the other under increasing masked the potential reinforcement effects schedule requirements for all participants. associated with LP stimuli. These findings suggested that although two It should be noted that in the Roscoe et al. reinforcers may be equally effective when (1999)investigationthereinforcingeffectsofthe assessed under low schedule requirements LPstimuliwereassessedusingafixed-ratio(FR) (e.g., FR 1), they may not be equally effective 1 schedule of reinforcement only. Thus, it is when assessed under increasing schedule re- unknown whetherLP stimuli wouldfunction as quirements. effective reinforcers under increasing schedule Given the results of previous research, which requirements. Previous research has shown that suggests that HP and LP stimuli may be equal two reinforcers may produce similar levels of in terms of reinforcer efficacy when delivered respondingunderlowschedulerequirementsbut underdenseschedules(e.g.,FR1;Roscoeetal., different levels of responding as schedule 1999) and the finding that two stimuli may be requirements increase (DeLeon, Iwata, Goh, & associatedwithdifferentresponseoutputsunder Worsdell, 1997; Roane, Lerman, & Vorndran, increasing schedule requirements (e.g., DeLeon 2001; Tustin, 1994). For example, Tustin et al., 1997; Roane et al., 2001; Tustin, 1994), evaluatedpreferenceforattentionandcombined the purpose of the current study was to visual and auditory stimulation under various determinetheextenttowhichLPstimuliwould schedule requirements in a single-operant ar- function as reinforcers under increasing sched- rangement. Response rates were similar when ule requirements. reinforcement was delivered continuously (i.e., on an FR 1 schedule); however, when schedule METHOD requirements increased (e.g., FR 10), one stimulus was associated with higher levels of Participants and Setting responding. Similar results were obtained by Four children participated in this study. Sam DeLeonetal.,whoshowedthatwhentwoedible was a 5-year-old boy who had been diagnosed stimuli were concurrently available, a greater with autism, Cedar was a 5-year-old girl who preference for one item over the other was had been diagnosed with autism, Elliot was an observedastheschedulerequirementsincreased. 8-year-old boy who had been diagnosed with Roane et al. (2001) evaluated variations in Asperger disorder, and Aden was a 5-year-old preference between two reinforcers under boy who had been diagnosed with attention increasing schedule requirements by assessing deficit hyperactivity disorder. All sessions were two highly ranked stimuli under progressive- conducted in a room (3.7 m by 4.6 m) that ratio(PR)schedules.InaPRschedule,response contained a table, chairs, and other materials requirements increase within the course of a that varied depending on the experimental single session, thus allowing for a relatively condition in effect (described below). rapid evaluation of reinforcing efficacy under increasing response requirements. Within PR Response Measurement and schedules, relative reinforcement effects are Interobserver Agreement identified by comparing the obtained break The target response for each participant was point for each stimulus (i.e., the last schedule chosen based on their individual skill deficits. ASSESSING REINFORCER POTENCY 179 Two different responses were chosen for each using a block-by-block method. Specifically, participant to evaluate the reinforcing efficacy the number of 10-s intervals during which oftheLPorHPstimuli.Thetargetresponsefor observers disagreed (on the occurrence or Sam was visually tracking a moving object (i.e., nonoccurrence of the response) was subtracted a circular blue card in the HP condition or a from the total number of intervals. For each of triangulargreencardintheLPcondition).This thedisagreementintervals,thesmallerfrequen- response was defined as Sam’s hand touching cycountwithinthe intervalwas dividedbythe any part of the card after the experimenter larger frequency count within the interval to changed its location. The target response for yield a partial agreement measure for each Cedar consisted of picking up either one red interval. These values then were added to the bean (HP condition) or one kidney bean (LP numberofintervalsduringwhichtheobservers condition)withherthumbandindexfingerand agreed. This number was divided by the total placing the bean into a small hole (approxi- number of intervals in each session and mately the size of a dime) cut into the top of a multiplied by 100% to yield a percentage of container. For Elliot the target response con- agreement. sistedofpickingupeitheronekidneybean(HP A second observer simultaneously but inde- condition) or one red bean (LP condition) with pendently collected data during 50% of all a pair of tweezers, using his thumb and index sessions. Mean interobserver agreement for finger as if holding a pencil, and placing the target responses was 94% for Sam (range, bean into a plastic container. Aden’s target 82% to 100%), 95% for Cedar (range, 88% response was tracing either the first (HP to 100%), 95% for Aden (range, 78% to condition) or the second half (LP condition) 100%), and 94% for Elliot (range, 83% to of the alphabet by moving a pencil along the 100%). Mean agreement for reinforcer delivery dotted lines of each letter, such that when he was 91% for Sam (range, 76% to 100%), 97% was finished, the traced letter correctly resem- forCedar(range,88%to100%),94%forAden bled the appropriate form of the letter. Sam, (range, 85% to 100%), and 94% for Elliot Cedar, and Elliot had no history with the (range, 77% to 100%). responses that we selected. Consequently, these 3 participants received preexperimental training Preference Assessment using either a least-to-most prompting hierar- Two preference assessments were conducted chy (Sam and Elliot) or stimulus fading to determine each participant’s preference for (Cedar). Aden had practiced tracing the 10fooditems.Beforeconductingthepreference alphabet in his regular schooling and demon- assessments, each food item was sampled bythe strated similar precision with the first and participants. For all participants, the preference second halves of the alphabet during preexperi- assessments were conducted in a specific order mental observations. (described below), and all preference assess- Inadditiontotheabovementionedresponses, ments were conducted at least 2 hr before or data also were collected on reinforcer delivery, after meals. Foods assessed during the prefer- which was defined as the experimenter placing ence assessments were available to the partici- an edible reinforcer directly in front of the pants only during the experimental sessions. participant(Elliot,Cedar,andAden)ordirectly Single-stimulus method. For the single-stimu- into the participant’s mouth (Sam). lus (SS) preference assessment, the procedures Handheld computers equipped with Observe described by Pace et al.(1985) were followed in software were used to collect data on the which each item was presented singly, 10 times frequency of the target responses and reinforcer each,inacounterbalancedorderoverthecourse delivery.Interobserveragreementwascalculated of five sessions. The stimulus was placed on a 180 BECKY PENROD et al. plate approximately 0.7 m in front of the Reinforcer Assessment participant. If the participant reached for the Baseline. During baseline, materials needed item,heorshewasallowedtoconsumetheitem. forengagingintheresponseineithertheHPor If 5 s elapsed without the participant reaching the LP condition, depending on which condi- for the item, the experimenter prompted the tion followed, were placed in front of the participant to pick up the food and consume it. participant, and there were no programmed Thetrialwasthenrepeated.Ifagaintherewasno consequences for the emission of the target response,theexperimenterremovedtheitemand response. Throughout this and all subsequent presented the next food. The SS assessment was conditions, the therapist told Aden that he conducted first for all participants. could respond if he wanted to but that he did Paired-choice method. A paired-choice (PC) nothavetorespond.Sam,Cedar,andElliotdid preference assessment also was conducted for not receive any verbal prompts throughout the each participant following the procedures study. All baseline sessions were 10 min in described by Fisher et al. (1992). During this duration, and two to four sessions were assessment, each of the same 10 foods used in conducted per day. the SS assessment was paired once with every FR 1 conditions. During the FR 1/HP other food in a counterbalanced order. Specif- condition, the therapist delivered the HP item ically, two food items were placed on separate to the participant on an FR 1 schedule plates next to each other approximately 0.7 m following the emission of the target response. in front of the participant. The participant was The FR 1/LP condition was identical to the FR allowed access to whichever food he or she 1/HP condition, with the exception that the reached for, and the food that was not selected therapist delivered the LP item to the partici- was removed. Attempts to reach for both foods pant following each response emitted on an FR at once were blocked, and the plates were 1 schedule. All FR 1 sessions were 10 min in withdrawn briefly and re-presented. If the duration. During the FR 1 conditions, two to participant did not reach for either food, the four sessions were conducted daily. experimenter prompted the participant to PR conditions. During the PR-HP condition, sample both items, and the trial was repeated. thetherapistdeliveredtheHPitemfollowingthe If again the participant did not reach for either completionofaprogressivelyincreasingresponse food,theexperimenterremovedbothitemsand requirement. Specifically, the therapist imple- presented the next set of foods. mented a PR schedule based on the procedures Based on the results of the preference described by Roane et al. (2001) in which the assessments, HP and LP stimuli were identified therapist delivered the HP item on an FR 1 for each participant. The HP stimulus was the schedule,whichthenincreasedtoFR2,andthen food item approached most frequently during to FR 3, and continued in this fashion until no both assessments. Specifically, stimuli chosen responding occurred for 3 min in any given between 80% and 100% of trials during both session. The therapist conducted two trials for the SS and PC assessments were identified as eachschedulerequirementtopreventrapidratio the HP items. The LP stimuli were those items strain (as described by Roane et al.). for which there was the largest difference The PR-LP condition was identical to the between the two assessments. Specifically, PR-HP condition, with the exception that the stimuli chosen between 80% and 100% of therapist presented the LP item rather than the trials during the SS assessment but less than HP item contingent on responding using a PR 40% of trials during the PC assessment were schedule. The same PR schedule was in effect selected as LP items. for both the HP and LP stimuli. ASSESSING REINFORCER POTENCY 181 Basedonthe3-minterminationcriterion,the stimuli. The cumulative number of responses duration of sessions during the PR conditions across the PR-HP and PR-LP conditions was varied as a function of response persistence. determined by adding the total number of There was no limit imposed on the duration of responses from the first session to the total each session; however, no session continued for number of responses from the second session more than 1 hr for any participant. Across and so on. participants, the mean session durations were 24 min and 17 min during the PR-HP condi- RESULTS tion and PR-LP condition, respectively. Based Preference Assessments on time constraints, one PR session was conducted per day for each participant. Results from the preference assessments are depicted in Figure 1. HP stimuli (i.e., stimuli Experimental Design chosen frequently during both assessments) Theexperimental conditionsdescribed above were peanut butter balls for Sam, cheese were arranged in a multiple baseline design crackers for Cedar, rice crispy treats for Elliot, across participants. The HP and LP conditions andchocolatechipcookiesforAden.LPstimuli were introduced in a counterbalanced order (i.e., stimuli chosen frequently during the SS across participants. That is, 2 participants were preference assessment but rarely during the PC exposed to the HP conditions first followed by preference assessment) were pretzels for Sam, the LP conditions, and the other 2 participants licoriceforCedar,cheesecrackersforElliot,and were exposed to the LP conditions followed by raisins for Aden. the HP conditions. Reinforcer Assessment Data Analysis Figure 2 depicts the results of the reinforcer The relative reinforcement effects of HP and assessments. During baseline, Sam rarely en- LP stimuli were evaluated by comparing gagedinthetargetresponse(M50.4responses response rates associated with each stimulus perminute).DuringtheFR1/LPandtheFR1/ across conditions and the average break point HP conditions, rates of responding increased associated with each stimulus under the PR relative to baseline. Rates of responding were schedules. A response rate for each session was slightly higher in the FR 1/LP condition (M 5 determined by dividing the total number of 5.0)thanintheFR1/HPcondition(M53.1). responses by the session duration (in minutes) During the PR-LP condition, rates of respond- to yield the number of responses per minute. ing were lower (M 5 1.9) than during the PR- Based on differences between the types of HP condition (M 5 5.7). During the PR-LP schedules in effect, the response rates in the condition, the obtained break points for each two FR conditions and those obtained in the sessionwereasfollows:FR5,FR7,FR8,FR6, two PR conditions were compared separately. FR 3, FR 6, FR 6; with an average break point During the PR conditions, the average break of FR 6. The obtained break point for each point for each stimulus was determined by session of the PR-HP condition was as follows: adding the obtained break points (i.e., the last FR 13, FR 18, FR 16, FR 6, and FR 13, and schedulerequirementcompleted)acrosssessions the average break point was FR 13. for the HP and LP items independently and Similar results were obtained for Cedar. then dividing this value by the total number of During baseline, Cedar rarely engaged in the sessions. In addition, cumulative records were response (M 5 0.2 responses per minute). generated to determine if different patterns of Similar levels of responding were observed in responding were associated with HP and LP boththeFR1/LPandFR1/HPconditions(Ms 182 BECKY PENROD et al. Figure1. PercentageofedibleitemschosenduringtheSSandPCpreferenceassessmentsforSam,Cedar,Elliot,and Aden.The single asterisk denotes theHPstimuli, andthe double asterisk denotes the LPstimuli. ASSESSING REINFORCER POTENCY 183 Figure2. ResponsesperminuteofthetargetresponseforSam,Cedar,Aden,andElliotduringbaseline,FR1,and PRsessions withHPandLPreinforcers. 184 BECKY PENROD et al. 5 3.3 and 2.5 responses per minute, respec- varied greatly across the PR-HP and PR-LP tively). Lower rates of responding occurred in conditions. To illustrate, for Sam the total thePR-LPcondition(M51.2)thaninthePR- session duration was approximately 219 min, HP condition (M 5 2.3). The average break with a total of 377 responses across seven point during the PR-LP condition was FR 2, sessions during the PR-LP condition, as and the break point for each session was FR 2, opposed to a total duration of approximately FR4,FR1,FR5,FR1,andFR1.Theaverage 183 min, with a total of 1,083 responses across break point during the PR-HP condition for five sessions during the PR-HP condition. Cedar was FR 4, and the break points for each Similar patterns were observed for Cedar (i.e., sessionwereFR5,FR2,FR6,FR4,andFR4. 48 min of total session time and 73 total During baseline, Aden rarely engaged in the responses in the PR-LP condition, 57 min of target response (M 5 0.8 responses per total session time and 141 responses in the PR- minute). During the FR 1/HP and the FR 1/ HPcondition),Aden(i.e.,38totalminutesand LP conditions, rates of responding increased 118 total responses in the PR-LP condition, relative to baseline and were lower in the FR 1/ 114totalminutesand259responsesinthePR- HP condition (M 5 3.9) than in the FR 1/LP HP condition), and Elliot (i.e., 157 total condition (M 5 5.0). Rates of responding were minutes and 546 total responses in the PR-LP slightly lower in the PR-HP condition (M 5 condition, 264 total minutesand 643 responses 2.2) than in the PR-LP condition (M 5 3.1). in the PR-HP condition). Thus, across partic- The average break point was FR 6 for the PR- ipants the means for the PR-LP condition were HP condition, and the obtained break points 116 minand279responses(M52.4responses for each session were FR 7, FR 6, FR 4, FR 8, per minute) and were 155 min and 532 FR 4, and FR 4. The obtained break points for responses (M 5 3.4) for the PR-HP condition. each session during the PR-LP condition were FR 4, FR 6, FR 3, and FR 3, and the average Cumulative Responses break point was FR 4. Cumulative records allow a direct inspection For Elliot, baseline rates of responding were of rate and changes in rate otherwise not low (M 5 0.9 responses per minute). During possiblewithothertypes ofgraphs.Specifically, theFR1/HPandtheFR1/LPconditions,rates relative rates of responding can be determined increased relative to baseline and were higher in by visually comparing the slopes of two the FR 1/HP condition (M 5 5.6) than in the performances; if the slope of one performance FR 1/LP condition (M 5 1.7). During the PR- is steeper than the other, this indicates a higher HP condition, rates of responding were some- response rate. In addition, cumulative records what lower (M 5 2.5) than those observed in reveal patterns of responding (i.e., break and the PR-LP condition (M 5 3.0). The break run) not shown by a typical line graph. points for each session during the PR-HP The top panel of Figure 3 depicts the condition were FR 8, FR 7, FR 12, FR 6, FR cumulative number of responses across all 5,FR9,FR4,FR5,andFR8,andtheaverage sessions during the PR-LP and PR-HP condi- break point was FR 7. The average break point tions for Sam. During the PR-HP condition, during the PR-LP condition was FR 7, and the Sam consistently responded across the sessions break points for each session were FR 6, FR 4, and the slope of the line was steep, but during FR 12, FR 10, FR 11, FR 2, FR 5, and FR 5. the PR-LP condition, responding was accom- It should be noted that although similar paniedbymorebreaksandthustheslopeofthe response rates and break points were observed line was not as steep. Even though the pattern under the PR conditions for most participants, of responding was fairly stable during both the session length and overall response output PR-HP and the PR-LP conditions, it should be ASSESSING REINFORCER POTENCY 185 Figure3. CumulativenumberofresponsesacrosssessionsduringthePR-HPandPR-LPconditionsforSam,Cedar, Aden,andElliot. 186 BECKY PENROD et al. noted that more responding occurred in a tion than under the PR-LP condition for 3 of shorter period of time and in fewer sessions the 4 participants (Sam, Cedar, and Aden). For during the PR-HP condition. these participants, the average break point was The second panel of Figure 3 depicts the lower in the LP condition than in the HP same data for Cedar. During the PR-HP condition;however,ingeneral,thedifferencein condition, Cedar consistently responded across break points was minimal. Specifically, for 2 the PR-HP sessions and responding was participants (Cedar and Aden) the difference in accompanied by more breaks in the PR-LP break points was within two responses of one condition.Again,agreaternumberofresponses another (e.g., FR4 in the PR-LP condition and was observed in fewer sessions during the PR- FR 6 in the PR-HP condition for Aden), and HP condition. for 1 participant (Elliot), the average break The third panel of Figure 3 depicts the same point was identical in both PR conditions. data for Aden. During the PR-HP condition, Aden consistently responded and the slope of DISCUSSION the line was steep. Although initially the slope Results of the current study replicated those of the line during the PR-LP condition of Roscoe et al. (1999) by demonstrating that resembled the slope during the PR-HP condi- LP stimuli maintained responding during FR 1 tion, pausing was observed after Session 3, schedules of reinforcement for all participants. which is denoted by divergence in the slopes of Moreover, the current results extended those the two lines thereafter. obtainedbyRoscoeetal.,inthattheLPstimuli The bottom panel of Figure 3 depicts the used in the current investigation also main- same data for Elliot. Similar patterns of tained responding under PR schedules of responding were observed during both the reinforcement. It is noteworthy that, although PR-LP and PR-HP conditions, in that the LP stimuli functioned as reinforcers under both slopes of the lines were similar, suggesting that FR 1 and PR schedules, the LP stimuli were the HP and LP stimuli were equally effective in generally not as effective as the HP stimuli in maintainingrespondingunderincreasingsched- ule requirements. terms of response persistence under increasing PR schedules. Nevertheless, the current results In summary, results indicated that baseline suggestthatLPstimulimayfunctionaseffective rates of responding were low for all 4 participants. When an FR 1 schedule was in reinforcers in a variety of contexts. effect, responding increased relative to baseline Roaneetal.(2001)demonstratedthathigher in both the LP and HP conditions for all rates of responding were associated with one of participants. For Sam and Aden, higher rates of two stimuli under increasing schedule require- responding were observed under the FR-LP ments, suggesting that even though two stimuli conditionthanundertheFR-HPcondition;for may be equally effective reinforcers for low- Elliot, higher rates of responding were observed effort responses, one stimulus may be more undertheFR-HPconditionthanundertheFR- effective than the other for high-effort respons- LP condition. Cedar was the only participant es.ThecurrentresultssupportthoseofRoaneet for whom similar rates of responding were al., in that both LP and HP stimuli were observed under boththe LP and HP conditions similarly effective under low schedule require- when an FR 1 schedule was in effect. When a ments, yet under higher schedule requirements PR schedule was implemented, responding one stimulus (the HP item) was generally the maintained under both the LP and HP more effective reinforcer. The current results conditions for all participants; however, more also highlight the use of PR schedules when responding occurred under the PR-HP condi- conductingreinforcerassessments.PRschedules

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.