JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2007, 40, 589–601 NUMBER4 (WINTER2007) AN APPLICATION OF THE MATCHING LAW TO SOCIAL DYNAMICS JOHN C. BORRERO, STEPHANY S. CRISOLO, QIUCHEN TU, WESTON A. RIELAND, NOE¨L A. ROSS, MONICA T. FRANCISCO, AND KENNY Y. YAMAMOTO UNIVERSITYOFTHEPACIFIC Using a procedure similar to the one described by Conger and Killeen (1974), we evaluated levelsofattendingfor25collegestudentswhoparticipatedineithera20-min(n512)or30- min (n 5 13) discussion on juvenile delinquency. Confederates delivered statements of agreement(e.g.,‘‘Iagreewiththatpoint’’)accordingtoindependentvariable-intervalschedules. Pooled results were evaluated using three generalized formulations of the matching law, and showedthatmatchingwasmorelikelyduringthefirst5 minofthediscussionthanduringthe last 5 min. Individual data for 7 of 9 participants were better described by the generalized response-rate matching equation than by the generalized time-allocation matching equation whenresponse allocation was characterizedin terms offrequencyrather than duration. DESCRIPTORS: choice, matching law, social dynamics, conversation, agreement, verbal behavior _______________________________________________________________________________ Response allocation under concurrent sched- Behavior-analytic research on choice has ules of reinforcement has been the subject of shown that there are a number of factors that appreciable basic and applied behavior-analytic may contribute to response allocation (e.g., research (Fisher & Mazur, 1997). This may be reinforcer quality, reinforcer magnitude), per- due in part to the ubiquitous nature of haps the most widely studied of which is concurrent schedules in the natural environ- reinforcerrate.Giventwoconcurrentlyavailable ment. As suggested by McDowell (1988, 1989) response alternatives (all other variables being and others, at any moment, organisms have equal),responseallocationhasbeenshowntobe the opportunity to engage in one of several a function of relative reinforcer rates associated concurrently available responses (e.g., read with each alternative. This frequently observed a newspaper, mow the lawn, or go for a run). linearrelationbetweenrelativeresponserateand Similarly, a pigeon may peck one of two relativereinforcer rate is known as the matching concurrently available keys, or engage in law, and was first described by Herrnstein grooming, and so on. Which one of two or (1961). Herrnstein assessed the responding of more available alternatives is selected describes pigeonswhenkeypeckingwasexposedtoaseries choice(Catania,1998),andbyobservingchoice of concurrent variable-interval (VI) schedules. and subsequent preference, we look next to the For example, one pair of schedules arranged variables that control response allocation. reinforcersontheleftkeyaccordingtoaVI75-s schedule while reinforcers on the right key were This experiment was completed as a senior project by thesecondandthirdauthorsunderthesupervisionofthe arranged according to a VI 270-s schedule. first. We thank Andrew Samaha for his assistance in Across all pairs of concurrent-schedule arrange- softwaredevelopment.WealsothankPaulineCabalesand ments, relative response allocation occurred in Hong Nguyen for their assistance in completing this project. Portions of this research were presented at the linear relation to relative reinforcer rates, when 25th meeting of the California Association for Behavior theappropriatechangeoverdelaywasincludedas Analysis in Burlingame. Monica Francisco is now at the partoftheprocedure.Theseresultswereassessed Universityof Kansas. in terms of the following equation: DirectcorrespondencetoJohnC.Borrero,whoisnow attheDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofMaryland R r Baltimore County, 1000 Hilltop Circle, Baltimore, 1 ~ 1 ; ð1Þ Maryland21250(e-mail: [email protected]). R z R r z r 1 2 1 2 doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.589–601 589 590 JOHN C. BORRERO et al. whereR andR representtherateofresponding obstructions). The b parameter describes altera- 1 2 totheleftandrightkeys,respectively,andr and tions in response allocation that are not 1 r represent the rate of reinforcement for accounted for by differences in relative re- 2 responses to the left and right keys, respectively. inforcer rates. In both nonhuman and human Equation 1 is often referred to as the pro- laboratories, bias may result from qualitatively portional matching equation or the strict different stimuli (e.g., food vs. water), differ- matching equation. ences in effort associated with the two response Based on certain deviations from strict alternatives (e.g., difficult math problems vs. matching as described by Herrnstein (1961), mastery level math problems), and seemingly Baum (1974) proposed what is now known as immutable preferences for one alternative the generalized matching equation, or more relative to the others (e.g., a strong preference formally, for the color green; Baum; Neef, Shade, & Miller, 1994). (cid:1) (cid:2) (cid:1) (cid:2) R r log 1 ~s log 1 zlogb; ð2Þ In some instances, behavior may be more R2 r2 appropriately captured in terms of its duration. For example, Baum and Rachlin (1969) where R , R , r , and r represent the same 1 2 1 2 assessed the behavior of pigeons in terms of features of the environment described in timespentononesideoranotherofamodified Equation 1. The s parameter describes sensitiv- operant chamber as a function of relative ity (the slope), and the b parameter describes reinforcer rates. Baum and Rachlin assessed response bias (the intercept). The generalized matching using the following equation: matching equation takes into account perfor- mance sensitivity (s) given a one-unit change in (cid:1)T (cid:2) (cid:1)r (cid:2) log 1 ~s log 1 zlogb; ð3Þ the relative reinforcer ratio. For example, if an T r 2 2 initial reinforcer ratio of 2:1 (e.g., VI 30 s VI 60 s; twice as much reinforcer availability on where T and T represent time allocated to 1 2 Response Alternative 1) were altered to a ratio ResponseAlternatives1and2,respectively,and of 3:1 (e.g., VI 30 s VI 90 s; three times as r and r are as described in Equations 1 and 2. 1 2 much reinforcer availability on Response Alter- In general, relative time allocation matched native1),one wouldexpect a concomitantone- relative reinforcer rates; however, considerable unit change in relative response allocation. bias was observed. Although an s parameter equal to unity (or Oliver, Hall, and Nixon (1999) assessed the log 1 5 0) occurs infrequently in the published aggressive and communicative behavior exhib- literature, s values of less than 1 are the most ited by a young boy in terms of its duration as frequently reported in the literature and are a function of the relative duration of reinforcer termed undermatching (a less than one-unit access associated with the two response alter- increase in relative response ratios given a one- natives (aggression and communication) using unit increase in relative reinforcer ratios; e.g., the following equation: Madden, Peden, & Yamaguchi, 2002). Mea- (cid:1) (cid:2) (cid:1) (cid:2) suresofsthatexceed1(e.g.,Aparicio,2001)are log T1 ~s log t1 zlogb; ð4Þ less frequently reported and are termed over- T t 2 2 matching (a greater than one-unit change in relative response ratios given a one-unit change whereT andT areasdescribedinEquation3, 1 2 inrelativereinforcerratios).Forexample,inthe and t and t represent duration of reinforce- 1 2 study by Aparicio, overmatching was associated ment associated with Response Alternatives 1 with increased response effort (e.g., climbing and 2, respectively. This seems to have been MATCHING AND SOCIAL DYNAMICS 591 a particularly appropriate model because ag- municative responses). Rather, the limits of the gression was shown to be sensitive to negative matching law remain a subject of ongoing reinforcement in the form of escape from behavior-analytic research. instructional demands. It is likely that the Although there have been numerous demon- duration of escape would be a more relevant strations of the matching relation with nonhu- reinforcement parameter than its rate. Also mans (responding for primary reinforcers), noteworthy is that the data used to assess the there have been considerably fewer studies matching relation were gathered in the partic- involving humans and attention as reinforce- ipant’s natural classroom environment during ment(e.g.,Pierce&Epling,1983).Likethekey interactions with his teacher. The results pecks of pigeons or the academic engagement reported by Oliver et al. were consistent with of students, communicative exchanges during matching and illustrated how the phenomenon conversation (i.e., verbal behavior, Skinner, could be modeled using parameters of re- 1957) should also be amenable to evaluations inforcement other than rate, as well as quanti- of matching. In one such study Snyder and fications of behavior in terms other than rate. Patterson(1995)evaluatedinteractionsbetween Equation 1 and its proportional variations socially aggressive boys and their mothers as have been applied infrequently in more recent well as boys who were not socially aggressive experimental research (relative to Equations 2, andtheirmothers.Theresearchersaccumulated 3, and 4), in part because of the increased 10 hr of interactions for each mother–child explanatory utility of the latter equations. dyad and then conceptualized maternal termi- Specifically, the generalized matching equations nation of the conflicts as the reinforcer for describe whether, and to what extent, behavior selection of various tactics (e.g., positive verbal, deviates from perfect matching. Further, the negative verbal) adopted by their children. generalized equations describe potential sources These data were then evaluated in terms of of these deviations (e.g., bias to one alternative the matching relation; results suggested that the or insensitivity to reinforcer rates). likelihood of a particular tactic was in part Although the response alternatives described determined by the likelihood of reinforcement by Herrnstein (1961) and Baum (1974) in- associated with each of those tactics, a conclu- volvedkeypecksexhibitedbypigeons,thework sion consistent with the matching law. ofOliveretal.(1999)andBorreroandVollmer In a similar study, Dishion, Spracklen, (2002) have demonstrated that relative mea- Andrews, and Patterson (1996) evaluated the sures of communication and problem behavior interactions of adolescent boys while focusing exhibited by individuals with developmental on the content of these discussions (e.g., disabilities can also be expressed in terms of the discussion of illegal behavior, discussions of matching relation. Other applications of the family and school that did not involve in- matching law(see Davison& McCarthy, 1988, appropriate activities, termed rule breaking and for an extensive review) have involved assess- normative, respectively) and the responses of ments of academic responding of individuals the listener (e.g., laughingor pausing). Datafor with and without developmental disabilities 181 dyads were assessed using Equation 1, and (Mace, McCurdy, & Quigley, 1990) and the researchers found that relative rates of rule collegiate (Vollmer & Bourret, 2000) and breaking were well accounted for by relative professional (Reed, Critchfield, & Martens, rates of laughing. Stated differently, Dishion et 2006) sports performance. Thus, the matching al. suggested that the content of naturally law is not limited by conceptualizations of occurring conversations between adolescents response alternatives (e.g., key pecks or com- was a function of the extent to which listeners 592 JOHN C. BORRERO et al. reinforced (laughed at) the conversation con- participants was assessed during seven 1-hr tent. sessions. For 2 of 6 participants, matching was The studies by Synder and Patterson (1995) observedduringthelast30 minofexperimental and Dishion et al. (1996) are particularly sessions (coefficients of determination ranged important because they speak to the durability from .45 to .69). Also noteworthy was the ofthematchingphenomenon(intermsofsocial observation that 3 of 6 participants allocated dynamics) when reinforcer rates are not exper- relatively more verbal behavior and time to the imentally manipulated.However,these analyses confederate who provided relatively less agree- involved between-groups comparisons; thus, ment. conclusions regarding matching at the level of Taken together, the results reported by the individual cannot be discerned from these Conger and Killeen (1974) and Pierce et al. results. However, the extent to which adults (1981) leave the matter of matching during differentially attend to, or engage with, others communicativeexchangesunresolvedwhendata in social exchanges has also been evaluated are evaluated at the level of the individual. The when reinforcer rates were experimentally present study was designed to replicate the programmed (Beardsley & McDowell, 1992). proceduresreportedbyCongerandKilleen,and CongerandKilleen(1974)evaluatedlevelsof later by Pierce et al., by evaluating levels of attending for 5 college students who were attending, given concurrent schedules of agree- invited to participate in a 30-min discussion. ment, for a larger sample of college students, using contemporary (Equations 2, 3, and 4) According to independently programmed con- variations of the matching equation (McDow- current VI schedules, confederates delivered ell, 2005). statementsofagreement(e.g.,‘‘Iagreewiththat point’’) following a participant’s comments. Data for the first and last 5 min of each session METHOD were then evaluated using a variation of Participants and Setting Equation 1 (a classic iteration of the matching Participants were 25 undergraduate college equation) and pooled for all participants. students who were enrolled in a small private Results showed that during the relatively brief universityandhadcompletednomorethantwo discussions, participants’ relative response allo- college-level psychology courses. The mean age cation (between each of the confederates) was of participants was 20.2 years, and each had well described by relative rates of agreement completed a mean of 3 years of college-level duringthelast5 minofthesession.Duringthe course work. Forty-eight percent of participants first5 minofthediscussion,however,nolinear were female. Prior to the experimental session, relation was observed. These data seemed to participants were asked to sign an informed support the notion that matching is a steady- consent form (approved by the university state phenomenon (e.g., Baum, 1974) that institutional review board) in a room near the requires some exposure to experimental con- experimental session room. Participants were tingencies before it occurs. told that contingent on their consent, they Pierce, Epling, and Greer (1981) extended would participate in a discussion to assess the work described by Conger and Killeen college students’ understanding of the factors (1974) by using Equations 2 and 3 (more contributing to juvenile delinquency. Two contemporary models of the matching relation) confederates also completed the informed andby conducting moreextended experimental consent process. Next, participants were re- sessions. Using procedures similar to those of mindedthatthediscussionwouldbevideotaped CongerandKilleen,theresponseallocationof6 (also included in the informed consent form). MATCHING AND SOCIAL DYNAMICS 593 After the consent form was signed, an experi- Procedure and Data Analysis menter directed the participant and the two While seated in the experimental setting, the confederates into the session room. Sessions moderatorreadthefollowinginstructionstothe were conducted in a laboratory that measured participants at the beginning of the discussion: approximately 5 m by 5 m. Materials included Before this session begins, we ask that you do not a desk, four chairs (one for the participant, one disclose any personal information about yourself for each confederate, and one for a moderator regarding any of the topics to be discussed. To responsible for evoking discussion), and posters further control for this, we also ask that all experiences shared be in third person such as ‘‘I on the walls (included to evoke discussion). know this person who …’’ This is to further insure Only 1 participant at a time was involved in your confidentiality. Thank you. a discussion session. The participant was Participants were asked not to disclose any strategically seated with his or her back to personal information regarding any of the a one-way mirror, with each confederate seated topics to be discussed. After the instructions, approximately 1.5 m to either side of the the moderator began the experimental session participant. The moderator was seated directly with the question, ‘‘What do you think are acrossfromtheparticipant(approximately1 m) some factors that influence juvenile delinquen- and facilitated conversation (described in more cy?’’ or a similarly phrased question. detail below). During the discussion, confederates emitted infrequent verbal responses that were not in Response Measurement and response to a statement made by the partici- Interobserver Agreement pant.Givenanextendedlullintheconversation Data were collected on comments made by (e.g., 30 s), the moderator attempted to evoke the participants (any vocal responses in refer- discussion using the following list of discussion ence to the topic); attending, defined as eye topics: contact between the participant and either confederate or the moderator; and orientation Factor 1: Drug and alcohol use of the body toward either confederate or the Factor 2: Peers and cliques moderator. Data were collected separately for Factor 3: Quality of school system or school environment attending to Confederate 1, Confederate 2, and Factor 4: Unfavorable home environment the moderator. Data were also collected on Factor 5: Subculture participation statements of approval delivered by each confederate (e.g., ‘‘I think you are right’’). Both Confederates participated in the conversation frequency and duration data were collected on (directed to the participant) (a) following handheld computers for each of the aforemen- a verbal response from a participant that (b) tionedmeasuresbybothprimaryandsecondary occurred after the VI interval associated with observers for 88% of all sessions. Duration of each confederate elapsed. Confederates were participant responses was scored given an signaled by colored flashlights operated by instance of attending and ended the moment experimenters on the opposite side of the one- attending to either confederate or the moder- way mirror. For example, Confederate 1 was ator ceased. Agreement coefficients were calcu- signaled by a blue light, and Confederate 2 was lated by dividing the smaller frequency (or signaled by a yellow light. The signal indicated durationin seconds) by thelarger frequency (or that a VI schedule for one confederate had duration in seconds) in each 10-s interval and elapsed, and that a statement of agreement multiplying by 100%. Mean agreement coeffi- could be delivered after a participant’s current cients for all measures exceeded 80% (range, or next response. The lights were not visible to 81% to 94%). the participant. It was possible to have a signal 594 JOHN C. BORRERO et al. delivered at a time when the opportunity to also evaluated response allocation in terms of deliveranagreeablestatementwasnotavailable. Equations 2 and 3 in an attempt to determine For example, a VI interval might have elapsed whether duration of response allocation or rate during a period in which the participant was ofresponseallocationwouldbebetterdescribed not talking. Thus, the confederate delayed by relative rates of statements of agreement. delivery of an agreeable statement until a par- ticipant’s next response. For this reason, RESULTS programmed and obtained reinforcer rates were Figure 1 depicts the results of the matching not identical. Confederates were instructed not analyses using Equations 2, 3, and 4. The left to attend (look at, orient toward, or speak) to column depicts results of the analyses during the participant under any other circumstances. the first 5 min of the discussion for all Between reinforcer deliveries, the confederates participants. The middle column depicts data either looked down, at each other, or at the according to each equation during the last moderator. 5 minofthediscussionforallparticipants.The Statements of agreement were delivered right column depicts data according to each according to independent VI schedules (i.e., equation during the entire 20- or 30-min VI 120 s VI 300 s for 20-min discussions, and discussion for all participants. VI 30 s VI 120 s for 30-min discussions). When evaluated as pooled data (across all Distributions of the VI schedules were de- participants) using Equation 2, results illustrate termined using the method described by undermatching (i.e., s , 1), with no notewor- FleshlerandHoffman (1962).Thefidelity with thy b values. Results using Equations 3 and 4 which confederates delivered statements of also were generally similar, in that coefficients agreement was not assessed and cannot be of determination were modest, but were larger discerned. Data were reported in either 5-min in all cases during the first 5 min of the components or for the entire session. For the discussion than in the last 5 min. One analysis 20-min discussions, the confederate who ini- (Equation 3, from the first 5 min of the tially delivered statements of agreement accord- discussion) produced a slope greater than 1 ing to the VI 120-s schedule switched to the VI and considerable bias (b 5 20.372). 300-sscheduleafter10 min,andtheconfederate Figure 2 depicts data for 4 participants who who initially delivered statements of agreement completed the 30-min discussion, in 5-min according to the VI 300-s schedule provided intervals. Data for individuals who completed statements of agreement according to the VI the 30-min discussion were selected for further 120-s schedule. During the 30-min sessions, analysis if there were at least five data points to confederatesswitchedschedulesafter10 min.At evaluate (i.e., the numerator or denominator theconclusionofthesession,theparticipantwas was nonzero for at least five 5-min intervals debriefed and was awarded extra credit in because the logarithm of zero is undefined). a required seminar for his or her participation. Data depicted in each row reflect analyses for Data for all participants were then evaluated individual participants. The left column depicts using Equations 2, 3, and 4. Data were results according to Equation 2, and the right expressed in several ways. First, data were columndepictsresultsaccordingtoEquation3. pooled for all participants and plotted during Equations2and3wereselectedtocomparethe thefirst 5 min of thediscussion, during the last time-allocation and response-rate variations of 5 min of the discussion, and during the entire thegeneralizedmatchingequationduringsocial 20- or 30-min discussion. Second, for partici- interaction.ForParticipants15,18,20,and17, pantswhocompletedthe30-mindiscussion,we Equation 2 produced relatively larger s param- MATCHING AND SOCIAL DYNAMICS 595 Figure1. Theleftcolumndepictspooleddataforallparticipantsduringthefirst5 minofthediscussionaccording toEquations2,3,and4(fromtoptobottom).Themiddlecolumndepictspooleddataforallparticipantsduringthelast 5minofthediscussionaccordingtoEquations2,3,and4(fromtoptobottom).Therightcolumndepictspooleddata forallparticipantsduringtheentire20-or30-mindiscussionaccordingtoEquations2,3,and4(fromtoptobottom). Dasheddiagonal lines representperfect matching, andsolid linesrepresent bestfit lines. eters(indicatingthattheyweremoresensitiveto Figure 3 depicts results for the remaining 5 changes in relative reinforcer ratios when participants who completed the 30-min discus- statements made by the participants were sion, in 5-min intervals. Results are similar to expressed in terms of relative rate of occur- those depicted in Figure 2, with two notable rence), smaller b parameters (indicating that exceptions. First, Participants 26 and 27 factors not captured by relative reinforcer ratios exhibited considerable bias to one of the two had a negligible impact on relative response confederates (b 5 20.648, 20.560, and b 5 allocation), and larger coefficients of determi- 0.286, 0.193, respectively, for Equations 2 and nation.Participants20and17producedperfect 3 and Participants 26 and 27). Based on the matching when expressed using Equation 2. availabledata,wecannotdeterminewhythese2 Note that three data points for Participant 20 participantsmighthavedemonstratedsuchbias. are plotted in coordinates (0, 0), and thus, only Second, Participants 27 and 14 were the 2 three data points are visible. participants for whom Equation 3 produced 596 JOHN C. BORRERO et al. Figure 2. Data depicted according to Equations 2 and 3 for 4 participants whose data were selected for further analysis. All data are for participants who completed the 30-min discussion. Dashed diagonal lines represent perfect matching, andsolidlines represent bestfit lines. larger coefficients of determination (relative to on the limited range of relative reinforcer rates Equation 2); however, the difference observed and not based on the model assessed. for Participant 14 was rather small. For For 7 of 9 participants who completed the Participant 27, these differences may be better 30-mindiscussion,Equation2providedabetter explained in terms of the spread in relative account of response allocation than did Equa- reinforcer ratios. That is, relative reinforcer tion 3, suggesting that characterizations of ratios were highly concentrated such that response allocation in terms of relative rate or evaluations of matching might be limited based duration may influence summaries based on MATCHING AND SOCIAL DYNAMICS 597 Figure 3. Data depicted according to Equations 2 and 3 for 5 participants whose data were selected for further analysis. All data are for participants who completed the 30-min discussion. Dashed diagonal lines represent perfect matching, andsolidlines representbest fitlines. 598 JOHN C. BORRERO et al. relative reinforcer rates. In other words, not the first 5 min than in the last 5 min of the only is selection of an appropriate model discussion), suggesting that participants in the important, but the way in which responding is currentinvestigationwereimmediatelysensitive characterized may also contribute to findings in to reinforcer rates. As noted previously, Conger support of the matching relation. andKilleenappliedavariationofEquation1(a proportional equation), which has been shown to be of less explanatory utility. Thus, if data DISCUSSION reported by Conger and Killeen were reex- The matching relation was assessed during pressed using generalized matching equations, experimentally arranged conversations among the findings may not be commensurate. A college students. To extend the classic work of second discrepancy lies in the rather low Conger and Killeen (1974) and Pierce et al. coefficients of determination obtained in the (1981), three iterations of the matching equa- current study (at the between-subjects level) tion were assessed at the between-subjects level. relative to those reported by Conger and Each equation was applied to capture relevant Killeen. However, this discrepancy may not be features of social dynamics (i.e., relative dura- unique to these two studies. Consider an tions of response allocation, relative rates of experiment in which 13 pigeons respond on response allocation, relative durations of agree- a pair of five VI schedules, during which each ment, and relative rates of agreement). At the pair of VI schedules is presented for 5 min. If between-subjects level, positive correlations one were to assume stable responding and then betweenrelativeresponseallocationandrelative select (at random) pairs of schedules and plot reinforcer rates were obtained, despite the way the data across subjects, low coefficients of in which behavior and statements of agreement determination would be expected. However, were expressed. This general finding is consis- when those data are reexpressed at the level of tentwiththosereportedbyCongerandKilleen. the individual subject, considerably higher Inadditiontothebetween-subjectsanalyses,the coefficients of determination would likely be matching relation was also assessed at the level obtained. Differences of this sort may even be of the individual, using Equations 2 and 3. expectedgivendifferencesintermsofsensitivity These two models were selected to determine whether rate or duration of response allocation and bias (as reflected in Figures 2 and 3). was better accounted for by relative rates of Therefore, although low coefficients of de- agreement. Results of these analyses were termination were obtained at the between- indicative of matching using one or both of subjectslevelinthecurrentinvestigation(which the models for allparticipants. These results are might be expected), improvements were ob- also consistent with those reported by Pierce et served at the level of the individual (which also al., although matching was observed for might be expected). relatively more participants in the current It is also possible that the 20-min discussions investigation than in the study by Pierce et al. in the current investigation did not allow The present findings also differ from those sufficient time for participants’ behavior to reportedinpriorwork.Forexample,whendata come under the control of the programmed were pooled, Conger and Killeen (1974) found schedules,andthus,influencedtheutilityofthe that closer approximations to matching were equations in describing the pooled data. observed during the last 5 min of the 30-min Matching is a steady-state phenomenon. In discussionthaninthefirst5 min.Resultsofthe fact, analyses conducted for the 20-min partic- present investigation found just the opposite ipants and the 30-min participants separately (i.e., closer approximations to matching during showedthatmoreofthevariancewasaccounted