JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2007, 40, 249–261 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2007) EVALUATING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN’S PREFERENCES FOR MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEMS DURING INSTRUCTION NICOLE A. HEAL AND GREGORY P. HANLEY UNIVERSITYOFKANSAS Preschool teachers rely on several strategies for motivating children to participate in learning activities.Inthecurrentstudy,weevaluatedtheeffectivenessofandpreferenceforthreeteaching contexts in which embedded, sequential, or no programmed reinforcement was arranged. The embeddedcontextincludedhighlypreferredteachingmaterials,thesequentialcontextincluded highlypreferrededibleitemsforcorrectresponding,andacontrolcontextincludedneither.In addition,anexclusivelyplay-orientedactivitywasincludedasafourthoptiontodetermineifone of the direct teaching contexts could compete with a relatively unstructured and exclusively child-led activity. All participants preferred the sequential context (use of high-quality consequences) over the embedded context (use of high-quality teaching materials), 2 of the 4 participants preferred some motivational system to none at all, and the play area was selected overallvariantsoftheinstructionalcontextsduringthemajorityoftrials.Wefoundeithernoor smalldifferencesincorrectrespondinginthedifferentinstructionalcontexts;however,ratesof undesirable behavior were highest in the least preferred interaction area for 3 of the 4 participants. Implications for the design of effective and preferred teaching environments for young children arediscussed. DESCRIPTORS: choice, concurrent-chains arrangement, embedded reinforcement, in- struction,preschoolers, sequentialreinforcement _______________________________________________________________________________ Determining children’s preferences for dif- selection of practices, such as relative efficacy or ferent classroom contexts is important because, implementation effort, are similar across prac- inadditiontoefficacymeasures,preferencedata tices. may influence the adoption of classroom Anumberofmethodshavebeendevelopedfor practices. The acceptability of behavioral inter- determiningyoungchildren’spreferencesamong ventions has often been determined by inviting food, toys, activities, and curriculum materials teachers or caregivers to complete rating scales (DeLeon & Iwata, 1996; Fisher et al., 1992; or questionnaires (Miltenberger, 1990). How- Foster-Johnson,Ferro,&Dunlap,1994;Roane, ever, there are distinct advantages to determin- Vollmer,Ringdahl,&Marcus,1998).Inspiteof ing the acceptability of different teaching these advancements, procedures for accurately practices with the learners themselves. For determining preschoolers’ preferences for class- example, Dunlap et al. (1994) showed that room practices or teaching strategies have not incorporating a child’s preferences into in- been described in the research literature. A structional contexts resulted in less off-task general strategy for directly determining prefer- and disruptive behavior. Information about ences for protracted events or behavioral inter- children’s preferences may be most important ventions has, however, been described in several when factors that would typically influence the studies (Hanley, Iwata, & Lindberg, 1999; Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, Contrucci, & Maglieri, Theresearchpresentedinthisarticlewascompletedin 1997;Hanley,Piazza,Fisher,&Maglieri,2005), partialfulfillmentofthesisrequirementsbythefirstauthor and these procedures may be applicable for atthe University ofKansas. RequestsforreprintsshouldbeaddressedtoGregoryP. determining young children’s preferences for Hanley, Applied Behavioral Science Department, 4023 classroom practices. Common features in these Dole Center, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas studies were the use of (a) modified concurrent 66045(e-mail: [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.59-05 chains arrangements with (b) individuals who 249 250 NICOLE A. HEAL and GREGORY P. HANLEY could not readily express their biases due to teaching materials that areattractive (e.g.,color- (c) severe language impairments or the complex ful,iconic,multidimensional)andthoughttobe nature of the assessed events. For example, generally preferred by children are used. Thus, Hanleyetal.(1997,2005)determinedchildren’s childrenmayautomaticallyderivereinforcement preferences among different function-based simply by manipulating instructional materials. treatments for problem behavior by recording By contrast, preferred stimuli are systematically each child’s selections in initial links of concur- identified and then used as consequences for rent chains in which different treatments were correct responding with the sequential motiva- presented in terminal links. In initial links, tional system. Therefore, in addition to the children pressed colored switches located on manner in which putative reinforcers are de- a table outside the treatment room. Pressing livered (embedded in materials or provided by a switch resulted in exposure to a treatment a teacher after a correct response), the type and, procedure inside the room, and each switch was presumably, quality of reinforcers differ across associated with a different treatment. Following motivational systems. Although both systems repeatedexposuretothisarrangement,children’s have been evaluated in preschool classrooms responding in the initial link differentiated, (Bryant&Budd,1982;Driscoll&Nagel,1999; suggesting preferences among the treatments. Gordon & Williams-Browne, 2000; Warren, In the current study, we applied a similar Rogers-Warren, & Baer, 1976; Zanolli & concurrent-chains arrangement to determine Daggett, 1998), their relative effectiveness or preschoolers’ preferences for different teacher- valuetothelearnershasnotyetbeendetermined. led instructional practices. In the current study, either embedded or Two general strategies for motivating chil- sequential reinforcement was implemented in dren to participate in teacher-led instruction two contexts. Thus, the teaching contexts varied havebeendescribed.First,reinforcershavebeen along two primary dimensions: the type and timing of reinforcement. We also included an embedded within the task itself (Essa, 2003; exclusivelyplay-orientedactivitydevoidofteach- Herr & Libby, 1990; Mayesky, 1998). Second, er instructions and an instructional context reinforcers have been delivered following com- devoid of either motivational system in our pletion of the task (Daly, Martens, Hamler, evaluation. Thus, four contexts were included as Dool,&Eckert,1999;Freeland&Noell,1999; terminal links in our concurrent-chains arrange- Rincover & Newsom, 1985). The relative ment: instructions with embedded reinforce- efficacy of embedded and sequential reinforce- ment,instructionswithsequentialreinforcement, ment has been evaluated during skills training instructions only (control), and no instructions with adults with severe disabilities (Thompson (play). We compared measures of children’s & Iwata, 2000) and interventions aimed at correct responding and undesirable behavior increasing children’s consumption of nonpre- while they experienced the different teaching ferred foods (Kern & Marder, 1996). By conditions to determine their relative effective- keeping the type of reinforcement constant ness. We determined children’s preferences across conditions and manipulating only the among the four classroom contexts by allowing manner in which reinforcement was delivered, and measuring their selections of each context. these studies provided strong evidence for the superiority of the embedded procedure. METHOD However, sequential and embedded motiva- tional systems differ in other ways when in- PARTICIPANTSAND SETTING tegrated within teacher-led instructional activi- The participants attended a full-day inclusive ties. With embedded motivational systems, preschool program that served typically and PREFERENCES FOR MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEMS 251 atypically developing children ranging from 3.5 50%ofallsessionsacrossallparticipants(range, to 5 years of age. Charlie was 5 years old with 50% to 90%). Agreement was scored when a diagnosis of nonspecified developmental de- both observers recorded the same card selection lay. James, Amy, and Anne were typically within a trial; coefficients were calculated as developing4-year-olds.Inadditiontoobtaining described previously. Agreement was 100% informed consent from parents, assent was across all participants. obtained from the participants each day prior Performance data also were collected during to conducting sessions. Children were selected the interaction area preference assessment. A for participation based on their and the first mean task-completion score was generated by author’s availability. assigning points for correct responding to vocal Observations took place in a large unoccu- (3 points), model (2 points), or physical (1 pied classroom (12 m by 7 m) that contained point) prompts within each instructional trial. child-sized tables and chairs, toys, and teaching Undesirable behavior was defined as vocal materials. The four terminal-link interaction attempts to leave the area (e.g., ‘‘Did the timer areas were located along one wall, three of gooff yet?’’or‘‘Iwantto gobackoverthere.’’), which contained a table, chair, and teaching and physical or vocal avoidance of instructions materials. There was a shelf with nine toys that (e.g., pushing materials away or putting his were often available in the children’s classroom or her head down on the table). All terminal- (e.g.,woodenblocks,dinosaurs,andconnecting link interactions were videotaped and later blocks) in the fourth interaction area. A scored in 10-s intervals using handheld com- different-colored laminated card (60 cm by puters for Charlie and James. Paper-and-pencil 75 cm) was placed on the wall in each of the measures were used to record performance data interaction areas. On the opposite wall, there in 20-s intervals during sessions for Amy and was a table containing four smaller colored Anne. A second observer recorded data simul- cards (15 cm by 10 cm). taneously but independently during at least 28% of all sessions across participants (range, RESPONSE MEASUREMENTAND 28% to 70%). For Charlie and James, agree- INTEROBSERVER AGREEMENT ment percentages were calculated by dividing A second observer simultaneously but in- the smaller number of responses recorded dependently recorded data on children’s selec- in each interval by the larger number of tions during 100% of task material and responses and multiplying the result by 100%. consumable preference assessments. Interob- Interobserver agreement was 95% (session server agreement was calculated by dividing range, 68% to 100%) for correct responding the number of agreements by the number of and 99% (session range, 73% to 100%) for agreements plus disagreements and multiplying undesirable behaviors, respectively. Agreement by 100%. An agreement was scored when both was scored when both observers recorded the observers scored the same child selection within same responses in each 20-s interval for Amy a trial. Agreement was 100% across all task and Anne. Agreement was calculated by di- materialandconsumablepreferenceassessments viding the number of agreements by the for all children. number of agreements plus disagreements and Duringthe interaction area preference assess- multiplying by 100%. Interobserver agreement ment, observers recorded card selections, which was 99% (session range, 87% to 100%) for were defined as the first contact of the correct responding and 97% (session range, participant’s hand on a card. Interobserver 67% to 100%) for undesirable behaviors, agreement data were collected during at least respectively. 252 NICOLE A. HEAL and GREGORY P. HANLEY Table 1 Description ofTask Materials inSets 1and2 Skill Set1teachingmaterials Set2teachingmaterials A Counting:1–20 Multicoloredcountingvehicles(cars,trucks,airplanes, 2cmby2cmnaturalwoodenblocks andbuses) B Patterning:Color Multicoloredpegbearswithyellowfoampegboard 5cmby5cmmulticoloredconstruction paper C Patterning:Shape Orangeshapeswithfacesonthefrontgluedtoclothes Whiteconstructionpapershapes hangers D Patterning:Size RedandgreenMartians(bigandlittle)gluedtofoam 8cmby8cmand15cmby15cmwhite pieces constructionpaper E Writing:Lettersa 20cmby28cmlaminatedconstructionpaperwith 20cmby28cmwhiteconstructionpaper F Writing:Shapes decorativeedgesandweterasemarkers andpencils G Matching:Numbers Greenfishwithglitteronthefinsgluedtoa2cm 5cmby5cmwhiteconstructionpaper, H Pointing:Numbers by2cmblueblock,numberslocatedonthebody numberswritteninblackink I Naming:Numbers ofthefishoryellowstarfishwithglitteronthebow gluedtoa2cmby2cmyellowblock,numbers locatedonthebodyofthestarfish J Pointing:Lettersb 8cmby15cmyellowlaminatedcardswithbluedogs 5cmby5cmwhiteconstructionpaper, K Ordering:Lettersb onthefrontgluedtofoampieces,letterslocated letterswritteninblackink onthebodyofthedog L Matching:Lettersa 8cmby15cmpurplelaminatedcardswithorange 5cmby5cmwhiteconstructionpaper, M Pointing:Lettersa catsonthefrontgluedtofoampieces,letters letterswritteninblackink N Naming:Lettersa locatedonthebodyofthecator8cmby10cm O Ordering:Lettersa orangelaminatedcardswithgreenfrogsonthefront gluedtowoodenblocks,letterslocatedonthebody ofthefrog P Ordering:Name 8cmby15cmbluelaminatedcardswithgreen 5cmby5cmwhiteconstructionpaper, andredMartiansonthefrontgluedtogreen letterswritteninblackink Popsiclesticks,letterslocatedonthebodyofthe Martian Q Pointing: Bluebutterflieswithglitteronbody,greenwormswith 2cmby10cmwhiteconstructionpaper, Sightwords glitterontail,yellowsunflowerswithglitterontops sightwordswritteninblackink gluedtoPopsiclesticks,sightwordslocatedonthe frontofthepicture aUppercaseletters. bLowercaseletters. PROCEDURE academic task, the two sets of materials were Task Materials Preference Assessment placed on the table, and the child was asked, Academic tasks were chosen according to ‘‘Do you want to work on [task name] with each child’s skill level, as determined from their these materials or these materials?’’ The teacher individualized classroom curricula. For exam- paused for approximately 2 s and then slid the ple, if the child had mastered matching child’s chair closer to the table. The child chose numbers 1 through 20, then the next skill, a set by touching the materials. Following pointing to numbers 1 through 20, would have a child selection, one academic instruction been used in the assessment. Two sets of relevant to the teaching materials was delivered. teaching materials were created for each aca- The order of task presentation was random and demic task. One set consisted of colorful, counterbalanced, and this sequence of events iconic, multidimensional materials; the other continued until both sets of teaching materials consisted of simple black-and-white materials for each academic task were presented once. (Table 1). A paired-item preference assessment Two sessions were conducted if selections (Fisheretal.,1992)wasconductedwiththetwo appeared stable; five sessions were conducted sets of teaching materials to identify the more if different materials were selected across the and less preferred sets of materials. For each initial two assessments. The most preferred set PREFERENCES FOR MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEMS 253 of teaching materials was used in the embedded control for James. The color assignments were context, and the least preferred set of teaching held constant throughout the interaction area materialswasusedinthesequentialandcontrol preference assessment for each child. contexts. Interaction area exposure. Exposure sessions were conducted to allow the child to experience Consumable Items Preference Assessment the interaction areas in the terminal links and Ten consumable items deemed acceptable by learn the associations between colored cards in parentsandteacherswereselectedforeachchild the initial links and the corresponding terminal and included in a paired-item preference links. Standing behind the child in the initial- assessment (Fisher et al., 1992). Each item link area, the teacher vocally and physically was paired with every other item once and were guided the child to touch one of the four cards presented to the child on separate plates; and then provided access to the correlated area approach responses (touching one of the two for 5 min. The selection of each session’s plates) were scored. After a plate had been academic task was counterbalanced and ran- touched,thechildwasgivenwiththeitem.The domly determined for each child, and the same order of stimulus presentations was random. A academic task was used across instructional hierarchy of preference among the items was contexts within each session. In addition, the obtained for each child, and the three items the order of link selections was randomly deter- child preferred most were used as consequences mined and counterbalanced. forcorrectrespondinginthesequentialcontext. Instructions were delivered approximately once every 20 to 30 s in each instructional Interaction Area Preference Assessment context. A three-step prompting procedure was Description of interaction areas. Based on the used to deliver instructions, and consequences results of the preference assessments, three (e.g., praise or praise plus consumable item) instructional contexts were designed. Highly wereprovidedfollowingcorrectresponsestothe preferred teaching materials were present and vocal or model prompt. There were no praise was delivered following correct respond- differential consequences for undesirablebehav- ing in the embedded context, less preferred ior. To provide similar amounts of teacher teaching materials were present and highly attention in all four interaction areas, attention preferred consumable items plus praise were was provided every 20 to 30 s in the play area delivered following correct responding in the for Amy and Anne. Teacher attention was sequential context, and less preferred teaching nearly continuous in the play area for Charlie materials were present and praise was delivered following correct responding in the control and James. After the child experienced each context. Finally, various toys that were typically interaction area once, the exposure session was available in the preschool classroom were terminated. Four exposure sessions were initial- present in the play area. The play area was ly conducted with each participant; one addi- included in the preference assessment to de- tional exposure session was conducted immedi- termine the conditions under which a child ately prior to the second interaction area might select a teacher-led instructional activity preference assessment. over an exclusively child-led play activity. Choice sessions. Choice sessions were identical Laminated colored cards (60 cm by 75 cm) to the exposure sessions, except that the child correspondingtoeachconditionwerepostedon made independent selections in the initial link, the wall of each interaction area. For example, and 3-min access to one of the four interaction a red card was correlated with play, yellow with areaswasarrangedintheterminallinks.During sequential,bluewithembedded,andgreenwith thechoicesessions,theteacherstoodbehindthe 254 NICOLE A. HEAL and GREGORY P. HANLEY child and provided the instruction, ‘‘Touch delivered all instructions in a neutral tone of a card.’’ If the child did not respond, the same voice, and the quality of the social praise instruction was repeated twice until 1 min had delivered by the teacher was similar across all passed. Sessions were conducted once daily and interaction areas. Finally, for each child, the were terminated after a child made eight same academic task was used across the in- selections or refused to make a selection within structional contexts within each session. 1 min of a choice opportunity (this happened once for Charlie and once for James). EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN Restrictionrules.Tworestrictionrulesallowed Aconcurrent-chainsarrangementwasusedto the identification of a preference hierarchy evaluate preference for the instructional con- among the four interaction areas (Hanley, texts and play area. Multielement and reversal Iwata, Lindberg, & Conners, 2003). First, if designs were used to evaluate relations between aninteractionareawasselectedon60%ormore the interaction areas and terminal-link behav- trials for two consecutive sessions or for two of iors (i.e., correct responding and undesirable three sessions, the card correlated with that area behaviors). was removed from the choice array. Second, if two interaction areas were alternately selected RESULTS for60%ormoretrialsacrossfoursessions,then Materials Preference Assessment cards correlated with both of the areas were The materials preference assessments identi- removed. After an initial preference hierarchy fied preferred materials for each participant. was obtained, a second interaction area prefer- Charlie showed a relative preference for Set 1 enceassessmentwasconductedtodeterminethe (colorful, iconic, multidimensional) over Set 2 reliability of the hierarchy with 3 of the 4 (black-and-white) teaching materials for 15 of children. All four interaction areas were again the 16 academic tasks. James showed an available to be selected, an exposure session was exclusive preference for Set 1 materials. Skills conducted, and the children were given the were selected for inclusion in the interaction opportunity to make independent selections. area preference assessment based on three Prevention of bias. The following variables criteria: (a) There were highly discrepant were held constant throughout the study to selection percentages between Sets 1 and 2, ensure that interaction area selections were (b) the initial skill in a developmental sequence a function of the independent variables. The was selected over a later skill (e.g., pointing to teacher provided no differential consequences numbers was selected over naming numbers beyond access to the different interaction areas because we generally taught the former prior to for selections in the initial link. The colored the latter), and (c) the skill had not been cards in the initial link were sequenced mastered in the classroom at the onset of the randomly at the start of each session. After interaction area assessment (e.g., patterning was each selection, the card located at the far left not used with James because he had previously end was rotated to the far right end. The acquired this skill in the classroom). Therefore, position of the interaction areas also was counting, patterning by color, writing shapes, sequenced randomly for each session. One and pointing to numbers were used in Charlie’s teacher conducted all sessions throughout the interaction area preference assessment; count- evaluation. The same teacher also was available ing, writing letters, naming numbers, and in each interaction area to prevent the partic- naming upper case letters were the four tasks ipant from choosing an interaction area to gain included in James’ interaction area preference access to a particular teacher. The teacher assessment. PREFERENCES FOR MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEMS 255 Only two academic tasks were included in selections to the play area. When the play area Amy’s and Anne’s materials preference assess- was removed, a preference for the embedded ments because each had already mastered many context emerged relative to the control context. of the academic tasks included in Charlie’s and The preference assessment was then repeated James’ assessments. Amy showed an exclusive with Charlie to determine whether the prefer- preference for the Set 1 materials, and Anne ence hierarchy would be replicated. Although selected the Set 1 teaching materials on 80% of results of the second assessment showed that trials. Naming letters and pointing to sight Charlie initially chose play in lieu of all words were alternately used in Amy’s and instructional contexts, the hierarchy among Anne’s interaction area preference assessments. the instructional contexts remained intact in In sum, the selected Set 1 materials were more that the sequential context was preferred to the highly preferred than the selected Set 2 embedded context, and both were selected in materialsforalltasksforallchildren.Therefore, lieu of the control context. Therefore, Charlie Set 1 materials were included in the embedded preferred the sequential motivational system instructionalcontexts;Set2materialswereused and, at leastinitially,demonstrated apreference in the sequential and control contexts. for an instructional context with the sequential motivational system over an exclusively play- Consumable Items Preference Assessment oriented activity. Theconsumableitemspreferenceassessments James allocated the majority of his choices to identified three highly preferred items for each the play area when all four options were participant(M&MsH,KitKatBitesH,andGold available (Figure 1). After the play area was FishH for Charlie; SkittlesH, Gold FishH, and restricted, he allocated his selections relatively jelly beans for James; stickers, Teddy Gra- evenly between the sequential and embedded hamsH,andFrootLoopsHforAmy;andKitKat contexts. In the second assessment, the prefer- BitesH, Froot LoopsH, and Fruit SmilesH for ence patterns were replicated (note the similar- Anne). These items were presented as conse- ity in the data paths from the first and second quencesforcorrectrespondinginthesequential assessments).Insum,Jamespreferredplaytoall context. variants of instruction, and he showed a prefer- ence for the use of motivational systems relative Interaction Area Preference Assessment to their absence (i.e., control), but he did not Initial links (preference data). The results of show a strong preference for either of the the interaction area preference assessments are motivational systems. shown in Figures 1 and 2. These figures show Initially, Anne’s selections were variable cumulative trial-by-trial data. Rows of data (Figure 2); however, a preference for the play represent selections towards a single interaction area eventually emerged. When only the three area; columns of panels represent data from instructional contexts were available, Anne single assessments. Restriction of an interaction allocated almost all of her selections to the area is indicated when the data path stops on sequential context. After the sequential context each panel. Charlie initially allocated the majorityofhischoicestothesequentialcontext, was removed from the array, she alternated her as indicated by the immediate upward trend in selections between the two remaining instruc- selections for the sequential context and the tional contexts. The patterns in the second initial flat data paths for the other contexts. assessment were somewhat similar to the first; Whenthesequentialcontext wasremovedfrom however, Anne often selected the sequential the array and three interaction areas were context instead of the play area. When the available, Charlie allocated almost all of his sequentialcontextwasrestricted,Anneallocated 256 NICOLE A. HEAL and GREGORY P. HANLEY Figure 1. Cumulative number of initial-link selections during the first and second interaction area preference assessments for Charlie (first andsecondcolumns) andJames (third andfourthcolumns). all of her selections to the play area. When the Amy because she declined further participation. play area was restricted, Anne selected the In sum, Amy showed a preference for the play embedded context. area over all of the instructional contexts. Amy initially showed variability in her Among the instructional contexts, Amy pre- selections (Figure 2), but a preference for the ferred the sequential context. play area emerged. When the play context was ThetoppanelofFigure 3displaysasummary restricted, she almost exclusively selected the of all children’s interaction area preference sequentialcontext.Whenthesequentialcontext assessments and shows the percentage of was restricted, she selected the embedded over selections for each interaction area given the the control context. The interaction area opportunity to select the interaction area. preference assessment was not repeated with Overall selection percentages indicate that all PREFERENCES FOR MOTIVATIONAL SYSTEMS 257 Figure 2. Cumulative number of initial-link selections during the first and second interaction area preference assessments for Anne (first andsecondcolumns) andAmy (thirdcolumn). children preferred the play area to instructional lowest in the control context for all children. contexts, all children preferred the sequential Anne and Amy required less prompting to contexttotheembeddedcontext,and2ofthe4 complete the academic tasks relative to Charlie children preferred the use of some motivational and James, which was not surprising consider- system to none at all. ing these participants’ more extensive histories Terminal link (performance data). Only very with the preschool classroom curriculum. small differences were observed in mean task- The percentage of intervals with undesirable completion scores (Figure 3) across instruction- behaviors is also shown in Figure 3. Charlie al contexts for all children. Nevertheless, scores engaged in undesirable behaviors during 0%, were the highest in the sequential context and 6%, 20%, and 23% of the intervals in the play, 258 NICOLE A. HEAL and GREGORY P. HANLEY at similarly low levels across all interaction areas; therefore, there was no strong correlation between undesirable behavior and the value of the interaction areas. DISCUSSION Weadaptedaconcurrent-chainsarrangement that has been used to identify children’s preferences for behavioral interventions to de- termine preschool children’s preferences for common classroom practices. The 4 children preferred unstructured play to teacher-led in- structional contexts that involved different motivational systems. When the play context was unavailable, all children preferred the sequential to the embedded motivational sys- tem, and 2 of the 4 children preferred the embedded motivational system to none at all. This study was primarily concerned with comparing teaching conditions that are com- monly found in preschool classrooms. Early childhood textbooks (Eliason & Jenkins, 1986; Graves, Gargiulo, Sluder, & Holmes, 1996; Roopnarine & Johnson, 1993) indicate that potentially reinforcing teaching materials are frequently integrated into early childhood classroom activities, as are enhanced conse- Figure 3. Summaries of initial-link responding (pref- quences(edibleitems,stickers,trinkets,etc.)for erence indexes; top), mean task-completion scores (mid- correct responding. Thus, we selected contexts dle), and the percentage of intervals with undesirable with high ecological validity; however, we behaviors (bottom) for all participants during the in- teraction areapreference assessments. sacrificed some experimental integrity by vary- ing two elements across the sequential and sequential, embedded, and control interaction embedded contexts (i.e., both reinforcer type areas, respectively. For Charlie, there was and the manner in which the reinforcers were a negative correlation between the value of the delivered changed across contexts). Thus, by interaction area (evident in the top panel of evaluating children’s preferences for practices Figure 3) and undesirable behavior (i.e., the common to preschool classrooms, we may have likelihood of undesirable behavior decreased as demonstrated the rather obvious outcome that thepreference valueincreased).Thiscorrelation children prefer candy and stickers to material also was apparent with Anne and James, who reinforcers(i.e.,thenatureofthedeliveryofthe engaged in undesirable behaviors in 0%, 0.3%, reinforcement may not have influenced prefer- 7%, and 18% and 0%, 6%, 10%, and 12% of ences). Nevertheless, because all children pre- the intervals in the play, sequential, embedded, ferred the sequential context, we recommend and control interaction areas, respectively. By that teachers systematically identify preferred contrast, Amy engaged in undesirable behaviors items that both teachers and parents find