JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2006, 39, 341–349 NUMBER3 (FALL2006) ACTIVE PROMPTING TO DECREASE CELL PHONE USE AND INCREASE SEAT BELT USE WHILE DRIVING MICHAEL CLAYTON AND BRIDGETT HELMS JACKSONVILLESTATEUNIVERSITY AND CATHY SIMPSON HEALTHSERVICESCENTER,INC.,BIRMINGHAM Automobilecrashesaretheleadingcauseofdeathforthoseaged3to33,with43,005(118per day) Americans killed in 2002 alone. Seat belt use reduces the risk of serious injury in an accident, and refraining from using a cell phone while driving reduces the risk of an accident. Cellphoneusewhiledrivingincreasesaccidentrates,andleadsto2,600U.S.fatalitieseachyear. Anactivepromptingprocedurewasemployedtoincreaseseatbeltuseanddecreasecellphone useamongdriversexitingauniversityparkinglot.Amultiplebaselinewithreversaldesignwas usedtoevaluatethepresentationoftwosigns:‘‘PleaseHangUp,ICare’’and‘‘PleaseBuckleUp, ICare.’’Theproportionofdriverswhocompliedwiththeseatbeltpromptwashighandinline withpreviousresearch.Theproportionofdriverswhohunguptheircellphonesinresponseto thepromptwasaboutequaltothatoftheseatbeltprompt.Aprocedurethatreducescellphone use amongautomobile drivers is asignificant contribution tothe behavioral safetyliterature. DESCRIPTORS: activeprompting, behavioral safety, cell phones, seat belts,flashfor life _______________________________________________________________________________ In the U.S., vehicle crashes are the leading The use of seat belts can significantly reduce causeofdeathforthoseaged3to33 years,with the risk of automobile injuries and fatalities. 43,005 (118 per day) Americans killed in 2002 Prompted by the injury-reduction and life- (themostrecentyearforwhichdataareavailable; saving effects of seat belt use, legislators National Highway Traffic Safety Administra- mandated the inclusion of seat belts in all tion). Worldwide, vehicle accidents result in U.S. automobiles sold since 1968 (Stowell & 1.2 million deaths per year, behind only child- Bryant, 1978). The challenge for public health hood infections and AIDS as a cause of death officials has been getting Americans to use the among people aged 5 to 30 years (Krug, 2003). seat belts that are now available in virtually The use of safety belts by drivers and front-seat every vehicle on the road (Geller, Casali, & passengers reduces fatalities by 45% for those in Johnson, 1980). This problem is compounded cars and by 60% for those in light trucks by selective recruitment, such that the riskiest (Kahane, 2000). Simply being in a moving drivers are also those least likely to wear seat automobile is a dangerous proposition, but belts (Evans, 1996; Van Houten & Malenfant, there are many things that drivers do (or do 1988). Most automobile industry attempts to not do) that increase the risk of injury or death. prompt seat belt use have relied on negative reinforcement. For example, lights, bells, and We thank Frances Archuleta, Ryan Reed, Rene Burt, buzzers sound until the seat belt is fastened. Barry Davenport, Jennifer Simpson, and Jana Vallejo for Research has shown that many drivers simply their assistance with data collection, and Trudie Guffey disable the systems (Geller et al., 1980), but andthelate Cathi Downing for inspiration. Address correspondence to Michael Clayton, who is these negative reinforcement approaches have now at Youngstown State University, One University remained in use by the automobile industry. Plaza, Youngstown, Ohio 44555 (e-mail: mcclayton@ Punitive measures, such as fines, are in effect in ysu.edu). doi:10.1901/jaba.2006.153-04 every state. 341 342 MICHAEL CLAYTON et al. Geller et al. (1980) first identified seat belt stopped at traffic lights. The sign read ‘‘Please use as an important dependent variable in Buckle Up—I Care’’ and was flipped over to appliedbehavioranalysis.Theyfoundthat62% present ‘‘Thank You’’ when drivers complied. of available seat belt interlocks or unlimited Thyer, Geller, Williams, and Purcell (1987) buzzer systems had been disabled, but that all used the same prompt, but included students drivers with working systems wore seat belts. standing outside a university parking lot to Geller et al. recommended avoiding the pitfalls prompt drivers. Active prompts also have been of punishment and negative reinforcement by used to increase compliance with stop signs instead focusing on positively reinforcing seat (Austin, Hackett, Gravina, & Lebbon, 2006; belt use. Van Houten & Retting, 2001). Consistent with this suggestion, seat belt use Subsequent research removed the human has been addressed with informational cam- component and simply placed stationary signs paigns that manipulate antecedent variables by outside parking lots (Berry, Geller, Calef, & providing performance feedback and often Calef, 1992) or provided a stationary textual include some type of active enforcement prompt (‘‘Buckle Up, Avoid Hospitals’’) in (Hagenzieker, 1991; Williams, Wells, McCartt, a retirement community (Cox, Cox, & Cox, & Preusser, 2000). For example, Wells, Mal- 2000).Othershaveremovedthetextualprompt enfant, Williams, and Van Houten (2000) altogether and provided a vocal prompt de- posted feedback signs at mall and shopping livered bygrocery clerks(‘‘Have a niceday, and center exits displaying rates pf seat belt use by remembertowearyoursafetybeltforasaferide patrons,distributedpostersandflyersproviding home’’)toincreaseseatbeltuse(Austin,Alvero, information about the benefits of the ‘‘click it & Olson, 1998; Engerman, Austin, & Bailey, or ticket’’ program, and urged patrons to use 1997). theirseatbelts.Thisprocedureresultedina10% Cell phone use is a comparatively recent increase in seat belt use among drivers and phenomenonthatalsoaffectsautomobilesafety. passengers (see also Malenfant, Wells, Van At present, about 50% of Americans use a cell Houten, & Williams, 1996; Pasto` & Baker, phone, and about 85% of those use one while 2001). Several researchers also have introduced driving(InsuranceInformationInstitute,2004). ‘‘promise’’ cards in which drivers agree to wear In addition to taking their eyes of the road belts in the future (Clark et al., 1999; Geller & while dialing, drivers can become so engrossed Lehman, 1991; Ludwig & Geller, 1991). Such in conversation that their ability to concentrate feedbackprocedureshaveincreasedseatbeltuse on driving is severely impaired. Currently, by 2% to 20%. about 6% of automobile accidents are caused Amoreeffectiveprocedureforincreasingseat by cell phone use (Lissy, Cohen, Park, & belt use has focused on both active and passive Graham, 2000). promptingofdrivers’behavior(Geller,Bruff,& Automobile safety data on cell phone use is Nimmer, 1985). Passive prompts are posted on limited but suggests an overlooked problem. A an inanimate object and remain there for the driver’s useof a cell phone upto 10 min before duration, whereas active prompts include hu- a crash is associated with a fourfold increased man facilitation of the prompt. In general, likelihood of crashing, and risk is raised prompting procedures typically suggest that the irrespective of whether a hands-free device is driver ‘‘buckle up’’ and have increased seat belt used (McEvoy et al., 2005). Impairment has usefrom12%to35%.Gelleretal.usedafront- been found to be at about the same level as seat passenger presenting a textual prompt to someonedrivingwitha0.08bloodalcohollevel occupants of adjacent vehicles while both were (Redelmeier&Tibshirani,1997).Comparedto ACTIVE PROMPTING 343 drivers who do not use cell phones, drivers the parking lot per hour during data-collection talkingoncellphonesmisstwiceasmanytraffic sessions over the course of the study. One signals, are more likely to swerve into the next student delivered the treatments, and two lane (46%), tailgate (23%), have close calls students collected independent observations of (18%),andrunredlights(10%)(Wilson,Fang, thetargetbehaviorsduringallsessions.Students & Wiggins, 2003). The use of a hands-free stood on the passenger side (as indicated in phone results in an equal level of distraction, Figure 1 by the figure holding a clipboard). but the radio, passenger conversation, and During the second season of the study, two books on tape do not interfere with driving to additional observers were stationed about one the same extent (Strayer, Drews, & Johnston, block away (secondary setting) from the 2003; Strayer & Johnston, 2001). primary setting, so that they could observe the Punitiveconsequenceshavebeenputinplace subsequent behaviors of left-turners from the in many countries. Drivers caught using a cell primary location. phone in England, Wales, or Scotland are fined £30, and drivers in New York, New Jersey, and Materials Washington D.C. are being fined as well The materials used in this study included (although hands-free phones remain legal). In a data-collection sheet (available from the first 2005, Colorado, Delaware, Maryland, and author), two walkie-talkies, and a two-sided Tennessee banned cell phone use by young poster that served as both a prompt and drivers (Copeland, 2005). Resistance from the a potential conditioned reinforcer. The poster cell phone industry and the difficulty of was white posterboard (51 cm by 61 cm) with banning all driver distractions (eating, applying black lettering about 7.6 cm in height. During make-up, reading, etc.) make further punitive the first intervention (prompting sign for cell measures less desirable. phone use), one side of the poster read ‘‘Please The current study attempted to increase seat Hang Up—I Care’’ and the other side read belt use and decrease cell phone use while ‘‘Thank You.’’ During the second intervention driving. The method was modeled after Thyer, (prompting sign for seat belt use), the poster Geller, Williams,and Purcell’s (1987)interven- read ‘‘Please Buckle Up—I Care’’ and ‘‘Thank tion to increase seat belt use by drivers exiting You.’’ a university parking lot. Our literature review Procedure found no attempts to alter cell phone use in The two treatments were delivered using automobiles, but it seemed feasible to decrease a multiple baseline across behaviors (with cell phone use while driving by using a pro- reversal) design. The entire study took place cedure that has increased seat belt use in over 2 years, with the interventions occurring previous studies. each spring and lasting for 6 to 7 weeks. Observer pairs in the primary setting re- METHOD corded seat belt and cell phone use of drivers Setting and Participants exiting the parking lot. If the driver had a cell The current study was conducted at a four- phoneineitherhand,thatqualifiedasengaging way intersection at a medium-sized state in the target behavior. Observers also recorded university located in the southeastern United whether the driver buckled up or hung up in States. The focus of the intervention was on response to the prompt (defined as completing a two-lane road exiting a large parking lot used the behavior any time between when the driver by faculty, staff, and students (Figure 1). approached the observers and passed through Approximately 110 vehicles, on average, exited the intersection). The sessions were conducted 344 MICHAEL CLAYTON et al. Figure1. Theintersection where the studywasconducted. every weekday from 11:00 a.m. until 12:00 During the second season of the study, p.m. Interobserver agreement was calculated by observers in a secondary location observed dividing the number of agreements by the specific vehicles as instructed by the observers number of agreements plus disagreements and in the first setting. Their task was to observe multiplying by 100%. Interobserver agreement whether the driver in a targeted vehicle was was assessed on 100% of sessions and was wearing a seat belt or using a cell phone. consistently over 95%. Communication was accomplished using a pair Duringtheinterventions,astudentdisplayed of walkie-talkies. These observers sat on a wall thepostertodriversexitingtheparkinglot.The (1.3 mhigh)3 mfromtheroadwayfacingeach prompt was held at chest height (120 cm to other to give the appearance of engaging in 155 cm above the ground) for the duration of conversation. Interobserver agreement also was the driver’s presence. The distance between the assessed on 100% of these sessions and was prompting sign and the automobile ranged consistently over 95%. from 1 to 3 m, depending on which lane the When a driver was observed buckling up or vehicle was in. All drivers were prompted with hanging up in response to the prompt in the the ‘‘Please …’’ side, and drivers who complied first setting, the observers waited until the car were then shown the ‘‘Thank You’’ side of the turned left and then communicated the color poster for approximately 10 s. and type of vehicle to the observers in the ACTIVE PROMPTING 345 Figure 2. Proportion ofdrivers using cell phones andhanging up, anddrivers wearingseat beltsandbuckling up during the study. The broken phase line at the midpoint indicates when the second season began, about 1 year later. Opencircles:proportionofdriversusingacellphoneuponapproachingtheobservers(top)andproportionofdrivers wearingaseatbelt(bottom).Closedcircles:proportionofdrivershangingupuponapproachingtheobservers(top)and proportion who fastened their seat belts (bottom). Open circles represent a change in behavior over time, and closed circlesrepresent an immediate response tothe displayedprompt. second setting. This occurred only during the use averaged 6%. During prompting for cell second season of the study and only for drivers phone use, use remained about the same, but turning left. Left-turners were chosen because the proportion of drivers hanging up their cell most (M 5 64%) vehicles turned left, and phones whenpromptedtodosoincreased. Seat limits on student resources did not allow belt use during this phase rose as well and observers in all three possible directions. averaged 56%. The presence of the prompter and observers implied monitoring, and because nonuse of a seat belt is an unlawful behavior, RESULTS AND DISCUSSION drivers may have buckled up to avoid a pre- The proportion of drivers who engaged in sumedaversiveconsequence.Duringprompting prompted behaviors is shown in Figure 2. The for seat belt use, cell phone use averaged about proportionofdriversobservedhangingupacell 7%, and hang-ups dropped to zero. Seat belt phone or buckling their seat belt is separate useaveraged78%duringthisphase.Twoweeks from those drivers observed using a cell phone after the last data point of prompting for seat or wearing a seat belt. belt use, a 1-week reversal was conducted. Seat During the first baseline, the percentage of belt use dropped to an average of 64%, and cell seatbeltuseaveragedabout44%andcellphone phone use remained about the same. 346 MICHAEL CLAYTON et al. Figure3. Proportionofdriversusingacellphoneorwearingaseatbeltbothbeforeandaftertheinterventions.The brokenphase line atthe midpoint indicates whenthe secondseasonbegan, about 1year later. Theresultsforthesecondseasonofthestudy hang up when they approached the prompt. closely resembled those of the first season, These rates approximate what previous re- except that the proportion of cell phone users searchers have found when using similar who hung up after being prompted to do so interventions for other driving behaviors. Fur- increased over the first season’s proportion. ther,whencomparingbaselinetopromptingfor Also, the observers in the second setting seat belts during the first season, there was collected data on the proportion of drivers a 34% increase in seat belt use. During the usingcellphonesandseatbeltsafterhangingup second season compliance was 25%. or buckling up in the first setting. The Figure 3 shows the proportion of drivers percentage of drivers using cell phones one using seatbelts andcellphones,bothbeforethe block after hanging up averaged 36%, whereas intervention began and after it concluded, the percentage of drivers who buckled their seat during both seasons of the study. During belts following the prompt and then unbuckled Season 1, cell phone use declined 9% from its one block away was zero. initialrate,andduringSeason2itdeclined3%. Thedata shownin Figure 2indicatethatcell Seat belt use increased 46% in the first season phone use was relatively low. The inclusion of and 13% in the second season. data on drivers who hung up in response to the With respect to seat belt use, the effect of prompt shows an average rate of compliance of prompting seemed to carry over into Season 2. 28%duringthefirstseasonand33%duringthe Seat belt use during baseline was over 40% and second season. At no other time did drivers increased to over 60% at the end of Season 1. ACTIVE PROMPTING 347 Baseline percentages during Season 2 were a result of more drivers acquiring cell phones, higher, suggesting possible carryover from although there is no way to substantiate this Season 1. Although the baseline proportion of possibility. seat belt use was higher in Season 2, the An additional innovation, the use of ob- intervention resulted in about the same effect servers in the second postprompt setting, as Season 1. Some drivers, for whatever occurred during Season 2. This procedure reason, may be more resistant to using seat allowed analysis of the longer term effects of belts. The ceiling effect seen in Figure 3 the intervention. Although initial compliance suggests that active prompting did not reach rates with the ‘‘hang up’’ prompts were these drivers. promising, it seemed likely that drivers might have put the other party on hold and then GENERAL DISCUSSION resumedtheirconversationmomentarily.Direct Overall, the findings of the present study observation indicated that about a third of support the use of an active prompting in- drivers did so one block later. Conversely, no tervention to increase drivers’ seat belt use and drivers were observed to remove their seat belts decrease cell phone use. Seat belt use increased after buckling up. markedly from baseline, with about a third of There are several issues pertaining to the unbuckled drivers buckling up when prompted current study that should be addressed. First, across the 2 years of the study. Similar success the rate of cell phone use during the study was was shown for a condition that prompted relatively low (6% initially). Currently, it is drivers to hang up cell phones. Postprompting estimatedthatabout10%ofdriversontheroad observationssuggestedthatbehaviorchangewas at any one time are using some type of phone highly durable for seat belt use, with no drivers (Glassbrenner, 2005). Future work in more unbuckling a block away from the initial urban environments is needed to further prompting site. Conversely, about a third of evaluate the procedure while minimizing this drivers had reengaged in cell phone use within floor effect. a block of the initial prompt and hang-up Asecondissuerelatestothenatureofthetwo compliance, suggesting differential effectiveness behaviors (cell phone and seat belt use). It is of the intervention across behaviors. legaltotalkonacellphonewhiledrivinginthe The current study used two procedural stateinwhichthestudywasconducted,butitis innovations. First, data were collected on not legal to drive without a seat belt. It also is compliance with the textual prompts. This reasonable to assume that drivers have had allowed for differentiation between behaviors considerably more practice buckling up and that were immediate responses to the prompt have received more education with respect to andacquisitionofsafebehaviorsovertime.The the safety benefits of seat belts. percentage of drivers buckling up or hanging Finally, the present procedure may increase up shows responding to the prompts, whereas the risk to drivers in another way. When the percentage of drivers engaging in the prompted to buckle up or hang up, drivers target behaviors shows acquisition. Behavior frequentlybegantofumblearoundfortheirseat change was more obvious in the case of seat belts or attempted to hang up their cell phones. belts, for which both immediate responses and These are risky behaviors that could lead to acquisition over time seemed to occur. Cell accidents. Fortunately, driving speed when phone use, on the other hand, appeared to be leaving a parking lot and negotiating a four- sensitive to the prompts, but the percentage of way intersection tends to be relatively slow, but users did not decline. This may have been the risk is real. 348 MICHAEL CLAYTON et al. Anecdotal evidence suggests that the social Copeland, L. (2005, June 2). States to drivers: Hang up, validity of these interventions was high. Many look out. USA Today, Retrieved June 2, 2005, from http://usatoday.com driversgavea‘‘thumbsup’’orapolitewaveand Cox,B.S.,Cox,A.B.,&Cox,D.J.(2000).Motivating smile, usually in response to the cell phone signagepromptssafetybeltuseamongdriversexiting prompt. There also were many ‘‘seat belt senior communities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33,635–638. salutes’’ (using a hand to lift the belt from the Engerman, J. A., Austin, J., & Bailey, J. S. (1997). body and gesture to the observers that they are Prompting patron safety belt use at a supermarket. wearingtheir seatbelts) performedbydrivers as JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 30,577–579. they passed the prompter. Over the course of Evans, L. (1996). Safety-belt effectiveness: The influence of crash severity and selective recruitment. Accident the study approximately 6,500 vehicles were Analysis& Prevention,28,423–433. prompted,withonlyonerudegestureobserved. Geller, E. S., Bruff, C. D., & Nimmer, J. G. (1985). Several aspects of the current intervention ‘‘Flash for life’’: Community-based prompting for safety belt promotion. Journal of Applied Behavior make it attractive for future safety programs. Analysis, 18,309–314. Theprocedureiseffective,easytocarryout,and Geller,E.S.,Casali,J.G.,&Johnson,R.P.(1980).Seat very low cost when volunteers are available. belt usage: A potential target for applied behavior Given the statistics on automobile accidents, analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 669–675. the procedure is worth far more than it costs Geller, E. S., & Lehman, G. R. (1991). The buckle-up to implement. Students enjoyed being outside promise card: A versatile intervention for large-scale and interacting with drivers, and the drivers behaviorchange.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis, 24, 91–94. did not seem to mind the intrusion. In future Glassbrenner, D. (2005). Driver cell phone use in 2005— applications, it would be easy and rewarding Overall results, Research Note DOT HS 809 967. for various collegiate groups (teams, sororities, Retrieved January 11, 2006, from http://www.nhtsa. clubs) to perform similar active prompting dot.gov Hagenzieker, M. P. (1991). Enforcement or incentives? interventions around campus focusing on Promotingsafetybeltuseamongmilitarypersonnelin seat belts, cell phones, stop signs, turn signals, theNetherlands.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis, and so on. The combined low cost, easy 24, 23–30. Insurance Information Institute. (2004, March). Cell presentation, and high effectiveness and phones and driving, Retrieved March 13, 2004, from acceptability of the procedure suggest that it http://www.iii.org shouldbecarriedoutmoreoftenandcouldsave Kahane, C. J. (2000). Fatality reduction by safety belts lives. for front-seat occupants of cars and light trucks: Updated and expanded estimates based on 1986– 1999 FARS data. (NHTSA Report No.DOT HS REFERENCES 809199) Krug, E. (2003). Road traffic injuries. World Health Austin,J.,Alvero,A.M.,&Olson,R.(1998).Prompting Organization, Retrieved July 5, 2006, from http:// patron safety belt use at a restaurant. Journal of www.who.int/world-health-day/2004/en/traffic_facts_ AppliedBehavior Analysis, 31,655–657. en.pdf Austin,J.,Hackett,S.,Gravina,N.,&Lebbon,A.(2006). Lissy, K., Cohen, J., Park, M., & Graham, J. D. (2000). The effects of prompting and feedback on drivers’ Cellular phones and driving: Weighing the risks stopping at stop signs. Journal of Applied Behavior and benefits. Harvard Center for Risk Analysis, 8(6), Analysis,39,117–121. 1–6. Berry, T. D., Geller, E. S., Calef, R. S., & Calef, R. A. Ludwig, T. D., & Geller, E. S. (1991). Improving the (1992). Moderating effects of social assistance on driving practices of pizza deliverers: Response verbal interventions to promote safety belt use: An generalization and moderating effects of driving analysis ofweak plys. Environment and Behavior,24, history. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 653–669. 31–44. Clark,M.J.,Schmitz,S.,Conrad,A.,Estes,C.,Healy,M. Malenfant,L.,Wells,J.K.,VanHouten,R.,&Williams, M., & Hiltibidal, J. (1999). The effects of an A. F. (1996). The use of feedback signs to increase intervention campaign to enhance seat belt use on observeddaytimeseatbeltuseintwocitiesinNorth campus. Journal of American College Health, 47(6), Carolina. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 28(6), 277–281. 771–777. ACTIVE PROMPTING 349 McEvoy, S. P., Stevenson, M. R., McCartt, A. T., Thyer, B. A., Geller, E. S., Williams, M., & Purcell, E. Woodward,M.,Haworth,C.,&Palamara, P.,etal. (1987). Community-based ‘‘flashing’’ to increase (2005, July 12). Role of mobile phones in motor safety belt use. Journal of Experimental Education, vehicle crashes resulting in hospital attendance: A 55,155–159. case-crossover study. British Medical Journal, Van Houten, R., & Malenfant, L. (1988). The effects doi:10.1136/bmj.38537.397512.55 of nighttime seat belt enforcement on seat belt National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. (n.d.). use by tavern patrons: A preliminary analysis. Retrieved July 11, 2005, from http://nhtsa.gov/ Journal ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 21,271–276. people/crash/LCOD/index.htm Van Houten, R., & Retting, R. A. (2001). Increasing Pasto`, L., & Baker, A. G. (2001). Evaluation of a brief motorist compliance and caution at stop signs. intervention for increasing seat belt use on a college Journal ofAppliedBehavior Analysis, 34, 185–193. campus.Behavior Modification,25(3),471–486. Wells, J. K., Malenfant, L., Williams, A. F., & Van Redelmeier,D.A.,&Tibshirani,R.J.(1997).Association Houten,R.(2000).Useofacommunityprogramto between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle increase seatbeltuse amongshoppingcenter patrons collisions. New England Journal of Medicine, 336(7), in Charlotte, North Carolina. Journal of Safety 453–458. Research,31(2),93–99. Stowell,C.,&Bryant,J.(1978).Safetybeltusage:Surveyof Williams,A.F.,Wells,J.K.,McCartt,A.T.,&Preusser, the traffic population. Technical Report DOT H56- D. F. (2000). ‘‘Buckle Up NOW!’’ An enforcement 01340.Washington, DC:Kirschner Associates, Inc. program to achieve high belt use. Journal of Safety Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., & Johnston, W. A. (2003). Research,31(4),195–201. Cellphone-inducedfailuresofvisualattentionduring Wilson, J., Fang, M., & Wiggins, S. (2003). Collision simulated driving. JournalofExperimental Psychology: andviolationinvolvementofdriverswhousecellular Applied, 9(1),23–32. telephones. Traffic InjuryPrevention,4(1), 45–52. Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction:Dual-taskstudiesofsimulateddrivingand Received November 4,2004 conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Final acceptanceFebruary 8,2006 Science, 12(6),462–466. Action Editor,Ron VanHouten