ebook img

ERIC EJ623184: The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children with Prelingual Deafness. PDF

13 Pages·2000·0.11 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ623184: The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children with Prelingual Deafness.

Journal of Early Intervention http://jei.sagepub.com The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness Ann M. McKinley and Steven F. Warren Journal of Early Intervention 2000; 23; 252 DOI: 10.1177/10538151000230040501 The online version of this article can be found at: http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/23/4/252 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children Additional services and information for Journal of Early Intervention can be found at: Email Alerts: http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://jei.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Citations (this article cites 31 articles hosted on the SAGE Journals Online and HighWire Press platforms): http://jei.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/23/4/252 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. JournalofEarlyIntervention,2000 Vol.23,No.4,252–263 Copyright2000bytheDivisionofEarlyChildhood,CouncilforExceptionalChildren The Effectiveness of Cochlear Implants for Children With Prelingual Deafness ANN M. McKINLEY Vanderbilt University STEVEN F. WARREN University of Kansas No longer considered an experimental procedure, cochlear implantation in young children with profound hearing loss has become almost commonplace. This paper reviews the efficacy of cochlear implantation in children with prelingual deafness. First, a brief introduction to cochlear implants is provided. Second, research regarding the effects of cochlear implantation in children with prelingual deafness on speech perception, speech production, and language development is reviewed. Finally, implications for early childhood educators are discussed. The development of cochlear implants is one fessionals view deafness as a condition to be of the most significant interdisciplinary bio- diagnosed and treated, the Deaf community medical achievements to date (Clark, Cowen, seesdeafnessasadifference,notadeficit.Ac- & Dowell, 1997). This therapeutic break- cording to Lane and Bahan (1998), Deafpeo- through has been the subject of widespread ple have a language and are a cultural minor- media attention in recent years as its effects ity, and the world of deafness is distinctive, have been increasingly reported in scientific rewarding, and worth preserving. journals (e.g., Balkany, 1993; Bonn, 1998; Many deaf advocates consider cochlearim- Osberger, Maso, & Sam, 1993). As technolo- plants as genocidal to the Deaf culture (Lane &Bahan,1998).Theyarguethatimplantation gy continues to improve,publicawarenessin- ofchildrenconflictswiththerightoftheDeaf creases, and the age of implantation lowers, cultural minority to exist and flourish. Al- young children with cochlearimplantsarebe- though these issues have been raised, this pa- coming common in various types of early per is written simply to evaluate the perfor- childhood educational settings. manceofchildrenwhohavereceivedcochlear This paper reviews the empirical evidence implants.WedonotwishtodiscredittheDeaf of the efficacy of cochlear implantation in community or the potential value of manual children with prelingual deafness. We ac- communication. Instead, we recognize that knowledgethatitisalmostimpossibletowrite there are parents of deaf children who desire a paper on this topic without addressing the that their child learn verbal communication. controversy surrounding the use of cochlear With this in mind, wehaveprovidedacritical implants for children. Cochlear implant pro- review of the research literature, examining fessionals have met tremendous opposition the effectiveness of cochlear implants on the from the Deaf community. Members of the development of verbal communication skills Deafcommunitydifferentiatetheterm‘‘deaf’’ in children with prelingual deafness. In addi- that is used to describe a pathology, from the tion, we addressed issues that early childhood term ‘‘Deaf,’’ which denotes a culture and a educators, who come in contact with children sense of pride. Whereas many medical pro- with cochlear implants, might face. 252 JEI, 2000, 23:4 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. Figure 1. Internal and external components as worn by the cochlear implant recipient. A Primer on Cochlear Implants ternal components consist of a microphone, a A cochlear implant is an electronic device speech processor, and a transmitter, which providing auditory information to individuals sends the signal to the internal components who have severe to profound sensorineural (see Figure 2). Cochlear implant surgery is hearing loss in both ears and receive limited usually a 2- to 3-hour procedure, performed or no benefit from conventional hearing aids. by an otolaryngologist (ear, nose, and throat Typically, these individuals have pure-tone surgeon). The child typically spends 1 night thresholdsof100dBHLorgreater.Theorigin inthehospitalfollowingcochlearimplantsur- of profound deafness is usually damaged or gery. Approximately 4 weeks following sur- diminished hair cells in the inner ear (Hen- gery, the child returns to the implant center derson, Salvi, Boettcher, & Clock, 1994). Be- for the initial stimulation of the device. This cause of the amount of hair cell damage, initial ‘‘tune-up’’ session usually takes 2 to 3 merelyamplifyingsounddoesnotenablepeo- days depending on the child’s age and level ple with profound deafness to process sound. ofcooperation.Anaudiologiststimulateseach Unlike conventional hearing aids, which de- oftheimplantedelectrodesanddeterminesthe liver amplified sound to the ear, a cochlear child’s threshold and comfort levels. Once implantbypassesdamagedhaircellsandstim- threshold levels and comfort levels have been ulates the auditory nerve directly withelectri- obtained for each of the electrodes, a map of cal current.Cochlearimplantshavebeenused these levels is created and programmed onto in research trials with adult patients since the thechild’sspeechprocessor.Initially,thechild mid-1970s but did not receive Food andDrug will have to make frequent visits to the audi- Administration (FDA) approval for clinical ologist to fine-tune the map. After that, ad- use in children until 1989. Currently,children justments to the child’s map will be made pe- overtheageof18monthswhohaveprofound riodically as perceptual performance changes. bilateral sensorineural hearing loss and dem- Several modifications have been made to onstrate limited benefit from conventional the cochlear implant device components and hearing aids might be considered candidates speechprocessingstrategiesasthetechnology for cochlear implants. More than 20,000 peo- hasevolved.Currently,threecochlearimplant ple around the world, half of them children, devices have been cleared by the FDA for have received cochlear implants since the clinical use. The Cochlear Corporation re- clinical introduction of the device (Bonn, ceived FDA approval for clinical use of the 1998). Nucleus 22 cochlear implant in children in A cochlear implant is comprisedofinternal 1989. This device was subsequently replaced and external components (see Figure 1). The by the Nucleus 24 cochlear implant in 1998. internal components consist of electrodes, In 1997, the Advanced Bionics Corporation whicharesurgicallyplacedinthecochlea(in- receivedFDAapprovalfortheuseoftheClar- ner ear), and a receiver or stimulator, which ion cochlear implant in children. A fourth de- lies beneath the skin behind the ear. The ex- vice, the COMBI 40(cid:1), manufactured by the McKinley & Warren 253 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. Figure 2. Illustration of the external device of a cochlear implant. Med.El.Cooperation,iscurrentlyundergoing onset of deafness, age of implantation,speech clinical trials. The most significant advance- processor type, number of implanted elec- ment in cochlear implant technologyhasbeen trodes, duration of deafness, communication in speech processing. Whereas the cochlear mode,anddurationofimplantuse(Miyamoto implant devices of the early 1990s encoded et al., 1994). In addition, it is possible that only specific features of speech, the current implant success is affected by the physiolog- devices encode the entire speech signal. ical functioning of the surviving neurons in In addition to device alterations, therehave the auditory nerve, the position of the elec- been changes in the demographic character- trodes in relation to excitable tissue, neural istics of children undergoing cochlearimplant supply to the cochlea, the central processing surgery. Specifically, children are receiving abilities,andthechild’scognitiveandlinguis- implants at a younger age because research tic abilities. Furthermore, the quality of audi- hascontinuedtosuggestsuperiorperformance tory training, parental motivation,and teacher associated with early implantation(Miyamoto expectations might also influence the amount et al., 1994). In 1998, the FDA lowered the of success a child achieves with a cochlear minimal age of pediatric implantation from implant. Finally, the child’s success is depen- two years to 18 months of age. dent on the parents’ insistence and the child’s complianceinwearingtheexternaldevicefull Performance of Children With Cochlear time. Implants When investigating the performance of Because children with profound sensorineural children with cochlear implants, it is impor- hearing loss constitute a very heterogeneous tant to differentiate between children who be- population, performance of children with co- camedeafprelinguallyandthosewhobecame chlear implants varies greatly. Factors asso- deaf postlingually. Children with prelingual ciated with the amount of benefitthe childre- deafnessmightnotdemonstratetheimmediate ceives from a cochlear implant include age of successseeninchildrenwithpostlingualdeaf- 254 JEI, 2000, 23:4 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. ness. Children with postlingual deafness have thespeechperceptionperformanceofchildren had some language and auditory experiences with cochlear implants. prior to the onset of deafness and, therefore, might be expected to benefit from a cochlear Speech Perception implant at a faster rate than those with prelin- Speech perception skills progress develop- gual deafness do. The aim of this paper is to mentally, beginning with speech detection, examine the effectiveness ofcochlearimplan- then discrimination, recognition, and finally tation in children with prelingual deafness. comprehension. Speech perception abilities Whereas a number of studies have included are typically evaluated using closed-set tests, children who became deaf after the age of 3 which measure speech discrimination, or years,weonlydescribestudiesthatexclusive- open-set tests, which measure speech recog- ly evaluated implant performance of children nition. In closed-set tests, a small set of an- who became deaf prior to the age of 3 years. swers is provided from which the child must Several studies are not included in this re- choose the correct response. In open-set tests, viewbecausethey did not meetourcriteriaof the examiner presents a word or phrase and thechildmustrespondbyrepeatingorsigning internalvalidity.Todeterminethebenefitchil- the word. Open-set measures of speech per- drenreceivefromacochlearimplant,itisnec- ception are considered more difficult than essary to minimize extraneous variables, par- closed-settestsbecausethepossibleresponses ticularly maturation and education.Therefore, are not provided to the child. this paper only reviews longitudinal studies Speech perceptionperformanceofcochlear including a control group consisting of chil- implant versus hearing aid users. When co- dren without cochlear implants. chlear implantation was first recognized as a Werecognizethatsomeresearchinthearea possible habilitative component for deaf chil- of cochlear implants is criticized for having dren,theevaluationofspeechperceptionabil- selection bias. Because it is up to parents to ities of children who receive cochlear im- decide whether or not their child undergoes plants versus those who used conventional cochlear implantation, random assignment to hearing aids or tactile aids became the focus treatment groups is impossible. It is conceiv- of several researchers. able that there are characteristics inherent in Afewstudiescomparedtheperformanceof children who receive cochlear implants that children who received cochlear implants to differ from those in children who do not re- the performance of children who used multi- ceive cochlear implants. Particularly, it might channel tactile aids. Miyamoto, Robbins, Os- bearguedthatchildrenwithcochlearimplants berger, Riley, and Kirk (1995) compared the come from families who have a stronger de- speech perception performance of children in sire for their child to develop verballanguage the predevice condition and at a post-device skills, than the families of children without intervalafteranaverageof1.5yearsofdevice cochlear implants. Although this potential use. The results demonstratedstatisticallysig- source of bias might have a slight influence nificant improvement in closed-set and open- on these studies, it is unlikely that it accounts set word recognition between the predevice for the notable effect. and postdevice condition for thechildrenwho The majority of research on the perfor- received cochlear implants, but not for the mance of children with cochlear implants has children who used the tactile devices. In ad- been in the area of speech perception. The dition, the scores of the cochlear implant number of studies examining speech produc- group were statistically significantly higher tion and language skills has been more limit- thanthescoresofthetactilegrouponallmea- ed. Becausetheperceptionofspeechisanec- sures after 18 months of device use. essary requisite to speech production and the Geers and Tobey (1995) assessed the acquisition of an oral language, we will first speech perception abilities of children with describe current research findings regarding cochlear implants and children who wore tac- McKinley & Warren 255 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. tile devices. After 12 months of device use, implantation allows deaf children access to the auditory speech perception scores of chil- auditory information during the preschool drenwithcochlearimplantsexceededthoseof years, which might positively impact the de- children with a similar hearing loss whowore velopment of speech and language. Miyamo- tactile devices.Inaddition,statisticallysignif- to, Kirk, Svirsky, and Sehgal (1999) selected icant improvements in lip-reading abilities children with prelingual deafness, who re- were documented in the cochlear implant ceived a cochlear implant prior to 5 years of group but not in the tactile aid group. Similar ageandexaminedtheirspeechperceptionper- findingswerereportedbyCarneyetal.(1993) formance asafunctionofageatimplantation. who found that children with multi-channel These children were administered speech per- cochlear implants statistically significantly ception tests prior to receiving their implant, outperformed children using tactile aids on a and then at 6-month intervals following im- change/no change speech discrimination pro- plantation. For each subject, data were ana- cedure. lyzed until the child’s chronological age was Vermeulen, Beijk, Brokx, van den Borne, closest to 4 years and 6 months.Resultsdem- and van den Broek (1995) compared the onstrated that, on average, the children who speech perception of children with cochlear received their cochlear implant prior to 3 implants to the speech perception of children years of age achieved higher speech percep- withanequivalenthearinglosswhousedcon- tion scores than children who received their ventionalhearingaids.After12monthsofde- cochlear implants after that time did. As ac- vice use, the children with cochlear implants knowledged by the authors, however, this performed statistically significantly better on study was confounded by the fact that the every measure than the children who wore children who received their implants at youn- hearing aids did. Whereas the cochlear im- ger ages also had more years of experience plantgroupachieved61%onadiscrimination with their device than the children who re- task of segmental aspects of speech, the hear- ceived cochlear implants after the age of 3 ing aid group scored 23%. years. It is not clear if the speech perception Five studies have comparedthespeechper- abilities of children who received their co- ceptionperformanceofchildrenwithcochlear chlear implants at older ages will ultimately implants to the performance of children with plateau at the same level. varying amounts of residual hearing who Todate,nostudieshavebeenpublishedthat wore conventional hearing aids (Geers, 1997; exclusively compared the speech perception Geers & Moog, 1994; Miyamoto, Kirk, Todd, abilitiesofchildrenwhoreceivedcochlearim- Robbins, & Osberger, 1995; Myer, Svirsky, plants beyond the age of 5 years, to age- Kirk, & Miyamoto, 1998; Svirsky & Myer, matched children who continued to use con- 1999). Collectively, these studies assessed ventional amplification. However, using a re- open-set and closed-set speech phoneme, peatedmeasuredesign,Osberger,Fisher,Zim- word, and sentence perception abilities. Re- merman-Phillips, Geier, and Barker (1998) sultsofallfivestudiesdemonstratedthatafter compared preoperative performance with an average of 2 to 3 years of device use,chil- hearing aids to postoperative performance dren with cochlear implants outperformed with cochlear implants after 3 and 6 months children with hearing aids who had pure tone of device use in children who received their hearing thresholds greater than 100 dB HL, cochlear implants after the age of 5 years. and performed comparably to children with Their findings demonstrated statistically sig- hearing aids whose hearing thresholds were nificant improvement in both closed-set and between 90 and 100 dB HL. open-set speech perception abilities over time Speechperceptionandearlyversuslateco- following cochlear implantation, particularly chlear implantation. There is a world-wide inchildrenwhowereeducatedthroughanoral trendtowardsgivingcochlearimplantstodeaf modality. Although unlikely, it cannot be children at increasingly younger ages. Early ruled out that the children in this study might 256 JEI, 2000, 23:4 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. have achieved similar gains in speech percep- nificant improvement only in the production tion had they continued to use their conven- of diphthongs. tional hearing aids. Nevertheless, the child’s Spencer, Tye-Murray, and Tomblin (1998) age is an important consideration forcochlear reported that children with cochlear implants implant candidacy if the child has prelingual comprehend more and use more bound mor- deafness.Theexpectationsofparentsandpro- phemes in theirspontaneous speechthanchil- fessionalsmightneedtobereadjustedforold- dren who use conventional hearing aids do. er children undergoing cochlear implantation. Finally, findings from Tye-Murray, Spencer, andWoodworth(1995)indicatedthatchildren withprelingualdeafnesswhoreceivecochlear Speech Production implants prior to the age of 5 receive greater The acquisition of intelligible speech is often benefitfromtheircochlearimplantintermsof the goal of parents and teachers of children speech production than children who receive with cochlear implants. The acoustic benefits a cochlearimplant afterthe ageof5yearsdo. providedbyacochlearimplantmightcontrib- ute to articulation of precise speech. Speech Language Development production, however, typically does not Although improved speech intelligibility is change as quickly as speech perception. Al- typically the focus of parents and teachers of though there might be slow progress during deaf children, speech skills do not directlyre- the first 2 years following implantation, it has flect language competence. Speech refers to been suggested that speech production will oral production, whereas language refers to improve as long as the child is trained and the abstract knowledge basis of symbolic challenged in an oral modality (Nevins & communication. The ultimate goal for chil- Chute, 1996). dren with prelingual deafness who have co- Research has demonstrated that speech in- chlear implants is to integrate auditory infor- telligibility of children with cochlearimplants mationprovidedbytheirdevicesintotheirde- increases graduallyovertime.AstudybyMi- veloping language system (Hasenstab & To- yamotoetal.(1997)demonstratedthatspeech bey, 1991). intelligibilityofprelinguallydeafchildrenim- The Sensory Aid Study conducted at the provedfrom0%priortoimplantation,to40% Central Institute for the Deaf (Geers, 1997; after 4.5 to 7.5 years of device use. In addi- Geers & Moog, 1994), longitudinally com- tion, after 2 years of device use, the speech pared language acquisition of children with intelligibility of children with cochlear im- cochlearimplantsandchildrenwithothersen- plants surpassed the speech intelligibility of sory aids (i.e., conventional hearing aids and hearing aid users whose pure-tone auditory tactileaids).Thechildrenrangedfrom2to12 thresholds averaged 100–110 dB HL. Rob- years of age at the beginning of the study. bins, Kirk, Osberger, and Ertmer (1995) re- Participants’ language performance was eval- ported similar findings. uated based on spontaneous language sam- Ertmer, Kirk, Sehgal, Riley, and Osberger ples, receptive and expressive vocabulary (1997) compared vowel production by chil- tests, and a battery of language tests normed drenwithcochlearimplantstothatofchildren on children with a hearing impairment. Re- with multi-channel tactile aids, and demon- sults demonstrated statistically significant im- stratedbetterperformanceofchildrenwithco- provement in all sensory aid groups on all chlearimplantsthanchildrenwithtactileaids. measures. After 3 years, the performance of Specifically, they found that after an average children with cochlear implants was superior of 20 months of device use, children with co- to all other groups of children who had pure- chlear implants showed a statistically signifi- tone thresholds of 100(cid:1) dB HL (essentially cant improved production of diphthongs and noresidualhearing)andsimilartothehearing most vowel categories, whereas the children aiduserswhohadpure-tonethresholdsof90– withtactileaidsdemonstratedstatisticallysig- 100dBHL(alimitedamountofresidualhear- McKinley & Warren 257 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. ing).Inaddition,resultsindicatedthatthedeaf velopment of 29 children with prelingual childrenusinghearingaidsprogressedintheir deafness who wore conventionalhearingaids. vocabulary development at a slower rate than Children received cochlear implants between normally hearing children did, whereas the the ages of 2 and 13 years. Samples of ex- vocabulary development of children with co- pressivelanguagewereobtainedfromallchil- chlear implants progressed at the same rateas dren using story recall. These samples were that of normally hearing children. analyzedusingtheIndexofProductiveSyntax Miyamoto, Svirsky, and Robbins (1997) scoring system (Scarborough, 1990). In addi- also examined the efficacy of cochlear im- tion, the children with cochlear implantswere plants in children with prelingual deafness on administered the Rhode Island Test of Lan- language acquisition. In this study, cross-sec- guageStructure(Engen&Engen,1983)toas- tional data from 89 children with prelingual sess sentence comprehension. The normspro- deafness who had not received cochlear im- vided by the test were used to represent per- plants were used to obtain a regression slope, formance by deaf children without cochlear which the authors referred to as the ‘‘deaf implants. Results indicated superior language slope.’’ The deaf slope was then used to ob- achievement by children with cochlear im- tain a predicted score for the children with plants compared to their peers without im- cochlear implants. Longitudinal data were plants. Performance on the Index of Produc- then collected on 23 children with cochlear tive Syntax indicated that children with co- implants and compared to the predicted deaf chlearimplantsscoredhigheronallsubscales, slope. Children were tested between 0 and 3 than did the control group ofchildrenwithout months before implantation and at 6 and 12 implants. In addition, scores on the Rhode Is- months postimplantation. Results indicated land Test of LanguageStructuredemonstrated that profoundly deaf children without im- a mean percentile rank of 95% for children plants,onaverage,couldbeexpectedtomake with cochlear implants, based on norms from only 5 months of language growth in 1 year. chronologically age-matched deaf peers. This The language growth of children with cochle- study supports the growing body of evidence ar implants exceeded the predicted growth by of the benefits of cochlear implants on lan- 7.1 and 6.9 months for expressive and recep- guage comprehension and production. tive language, respectively. In addition, the authors reported that during the 1st year fol- Summary lowing implantation, the rate of language de- Research has demonstrated the positive ben- velopment was the same for deaf children efits of cochlear implants on the speech per- with cochlear implants as for normally hear- ception, speech production, and language de- ing children. Consequently, the gap in lan- velopment of young children with prelingual guage scores between children with implants deafness. Anecdotal reports, not described and children with normal hearing did not in- here,havealsoindicatedpositiveoutcomesof crease, but remained constant during the 1st cochlear implants on reading skills, academic year.Theauthorssuggestedthatthesefindings achievement, and social acceptance (Bonn, support early implantation because there is a 1998; Nevins & Chute, 1995; Radcliffe, smallerlanguagegapbetweenadeafchildand 1999), but empirical research in these areasis his or her hearing peers at a younger versus needed. Additional research examining the an older age. performance of children with cochlear im- Tomblin, Spencer, Flock, Tyler, and Gantz plants continues. Future research seemslikely (1999) also conducted a study comparing the to document even more impressive levels of language achievement of children with and performance, as studies begin to reflect the without cochlear implants. These researchers improvedspeechprocessingofthecurrentde- compared the English language development vices and the trend toward children receiving of 29 children with prelingual deafness and implants at a younger age. A clearer under- whohadcochlearimplantstothelanguagede- standing of the vital role of effective habili- 258 JEI, 2000, 23:4 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. tative and educational techniques might also technical aspects of the implant and can re- positively influence the level of child perfor- solve problems with the device. mance reported in future investigations. Although audiologists, otolaryngologists, and speech-language pathologists at the im- Implications for Early Childhood plant center might make up the core of the Educators implant team, it is essential toincludeadjunct Universal newborn hearing screenings, early members relevant to the life of the candidate, identification, advanced technology, height- such as the child’s teacher or caregiver. Their ened public awareness, and positiveempirical input is instrumental for decisions regarding findings are all likely to increase the frequen- cochlear implant candidacy and auditory as cy in which early childhood educators come well as linguistic goals. For example, they in contact with young children with cochlear might provide information not readily avail- implants. Unlike their deaf peers, who have able to the rest of the implant team, such as onlyminimalaccesstoauditorycues,children the child’s academic progress, motivation to with cochlear implants have the capacity to communicateverbally,andsocialskills.Inad- use the auditory code as the primary source dition, because they observe the child daily, of language development and learning. Early they might be the first to recognize perfor- childhood educators are likely to encounter mance changes indicating the need for an ad- these children in a variety of educationalcon- justment of the child’s speech processor map. texts, including inclusive settings. Conse- Eachprofessionalcontributesarequisitecom- quently,itisimportantthatearlyintervention- ponent to the child’s success. Parents and istsandearlychildhoodeducatorsarefamiliar teachers become more integral members of with cochlear implants, the auditory benefit the implant team when a communicative link provided by implants, and the components of is established between the implant center and a successful habilitative or educational pro- thechild’sschool.Ideally,thiscommunicative gram.Thefollowingdiscussionfocusesonthe link is initiated when the child is assessed as fundamentals of an optimal educational envi- a potential candidate for a cochlear implant ronment for young children with cochlearim- and then continues indefinitely. plants. Analytical Therapy Versus Naturalistic A Communicative Link Habilitation Children with cochlear implantsmightbenefit As Robbins (1998a) explained, the enhanced considerably from the existence of an estab- auditory capacity afforded by a cochlear im- lished communicative link between the im- plant and the younger age of implantationhas plantcenterandtheirschoolorchild-carecen- altered the focus of habilitativetechniquesfor ter (Stojny, Harrison, & Zimmerman-Phillips, deaf children with cochlear implants. Tradi- 1998). At the implant center, the child is typ- tionally, auditory and linguistic training for ically seen by a team ofprofessionals,includ- children with profoundsensorineuraldeafness ing an audiologist, an otolaryngologist, a hasbeencenteredonverynarrow,didacticex- speech-language pathologist, and often a psy- ercises designed to facilitate sound detection, chologist.Elsewhere,achildmightbeinclose discrimination, identification,andcomprehen- contact with a classroom teacher, school sion. Auditory Verbal Therapy is an example speech-language pathologist, itinerantteacher, of a habilitative program using this type of or child care provider. Professionals obtain analytical auditory training exercises to facil- important information about the child, and itate verbal communication in children with their areas of expertise are essential to maxi- severe to profound hearing loss. These types mize the child’s education and development. of structured exercises are advocated particu- Members of the cochlear implant team have larly for older children who have had limited expertise to assess the child’s potential with a listening experiences. cochlear implant. They also specialize in the Cochlear implants provide deaf children McKinley & Warren 259 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution. with auditory information not previously ac- at the time of implantation dictates whether cessible through their hearing aids, particular- the intervention program should emphasize ly at the high frequencies. Therefore, the ac- direct or indirect instruction. Whereas older quisition of spoken English as a primary lan- childrenandadultsmightbenefitsubstantially guage is often a goal for deaf children with from direct instruction and rote exercises to cochlear implants. Because of their enhanced facilitate the development of auditory skills, auditory capabilities and focus on spoken En- younger children are likely to derive greater glish,ithasbeensuggestedthatthesechildren listening and language benefits from contex- reap greater benefits from a more naturalistic tualized and incidental experiences. rather than a direct approach to language and Although it has not been empirically dem- auditory learning (e.g., Robbins, 1998a; Nev- onstrated, clinical observation attests that, in ins & Chute, 1996). addition to their naturalistic learning experi- These clinical observations mirror empiri- ences, most children with cochlear implants cal data about children with normal hearing will require structured auditory exercises to that indicate that young children generally advance their listening skills. Estabrooks learn more efficiently from naturalistic or in- (1998) suggested that these structured activi- cidentalexperiencesthanfromdirectteaching ties are most effective when they incorporate experiences, until the mean length of utter- meaningful communication skills appropriate ances is atleastthreemorphemeslong(Yoder for the child’s linguistic and cognitive levels, & Warren, 1998). Children with cochlear im- and occur as a natural consequence of an ac- plants have the potential to enhance their lis- tivity. Ideally, the clinician or educator con- tening and learning skills throughreal-lifeex- ducting the therapy session will integrate lan- periences. Cochlear implants provide the ca- guagegoalsintothelisteningtasks.Tradition- pability for deaf children to generalize and alauditorytrainingwhichisslightlymodified, learnfromnaturalisticandincidentaleventsat mightgreatlyenrichthemeaningfulnessofthe a level not previously possible with conven- task for the child. For further suggestions for tionalhearingaids.Habilitationforthesechil- designing meaningful listening activities, the dren, therefore, should more closely mirror reader is referred to Estabrooks (1998), Nev- theeducationofchildrenwithnormalhearing. ins and Chute (1996), and Robbins (1998b). That is, intervention should focus on teaching a broad array of developmentally appropriate Supporting Parental Efforts targets in a naturalistic environment with the Parentsandthehomeenvironmentmighthave idea that the child will generalize what he or the greatest impact on the auditory develop- she learns to other contexts and other targets mentof thechildintheearlyyearsofimplant (Robbins, 1998a).For example,parentsmight use. Young children often interact with their focus on getting their 18-month-old child to parents off and on many hours a day, 7 days identify and produce his or her name in the a week, year in and year out. The cumulative context of daily routines. At preschool, the effect of this environment, either positive or child might learn auditory or listening skills negative, has shown to be quite substantial best in the context of the existing curriculum. (e.g., Hart & Risley, 1995). For that reason, TheinterestedreaderisreferredtoEstabrooks the role of early childhood professionals (1998)andRobbins(1998a)forcreativeclass- should be to equip parents to provide an en- room activities enhancing incidental and gen- riched language environment for their child. eralized learning in young children with co- Likewise, one can presume that the effects chlear implants. of a cochlear implant on a child’s language Theageofimplantationmightinfluencethe development will depend, to some extent, on typeoflearningenvironmentmostoptimalfor the level of use of the device. Compliance children with cochlear implants. Based on with wearing the devices is one of the more their clinical experience, Nevins and Chute frequent issues facing parents of young im- (1996)havesuggestedthattheageofthechild plant recipients. A strong-willed child might 260 JEI, 2000, 23:4 Downloaded from http://jei.sagepub.com by M Peterson on May 28, 2008 © 2000 Division for Early Childhood of the Council for Exceptional Children. All rights reserved. Not for commercial use or unauthorized distribution.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.