ebook img

ERIC EJ1160258: Parents' Educational Expectations for Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder PDF

0.1 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ1160258: Parents' Educational Expectations for Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities,2017,52(4),357–368 ©DivisiononAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities Parents’ Educational Expectations for Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Hillary H. Bush Shana R. Cohen MassachusettsGeneralHospital UniversityofCalifornia,SanDiego Abbey S. Eisenhower Jan Blacher UniversityofMassachusetts,Boston UniversityofCalifornia,Riverside Abstract: Among typically developing children, many characteristics have been associated with parents’ expectations for their children’s adjustment to school and academic progress. Despite the history of increased parentalinvolvementintheeducationofchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorder(ASD)relativetoparentsof childrenwithoutASD,thereislittleresearchontheeducationalexpectationsthatparentsholdfortheiryoung children with ASD and how they are formed. In the current study, we addressed this gap by using a mixed methods design to examine the association between parents’ expectations for the level of success their children would achieve during the current school year, and multiple child-, parent/family-, and teacher/school-level factors,amongparentsof4to7yearoldswithASD(N(cid:2)121).Whenthesedifferentfactorswereconsidered simultaneously,children’sexternalizingbehaviors,parents’educationallevel,andparents’employmentstatus werefoundtobesignificantlyassociatedwithparents’expectationsofschooling. Parents’ expectations for their children shape Kean,2005).However,despitethepotentially their parenting behaviors, and consequently, influential role of parents’ expectations of children’s self-concept, school adjustment, and schooling,verylittleattentionhasbeenplaced academicoutcomes(Bandura,Barbaranelli,Ca- onparents’expectationsfortheiryoungchil- prara,&Pastorelli,1996).Infact,parentswho drenwithautismspectrumdisorder(ASD)as hold high academic expectations have chil- they transition into early schooling. In the drenwhoexhibitbetterschooloutcomesthan current study, we examined parents’ school- might be anticipated based solely on their related expectations for their young children cognitive functioning or previous academic with ASD, as well as their child-, parent/fam- achievement(deBoer&vanderWerf,2015). ily-, and teacher/school-level correlates. The Past research suggests that parents’ expecta- intent was to identify areas where profession- tions may play a protective role in children’s alsmightbetterunderstandandsupportpar- academicachievementforchildrenfacingrisk ents of children with ASD adapting to school factors,suchaseconomicdisadvantage(Alex- entry. ander, Entwistler, & Bedinger, 1994; Davis- EcologicalSystemsTheoryandSchoolAdjustment Thispaperwasbasedontheactivitiesandoriginal conceptualmodelofalongitudinalstudyfundedby In examining the individual, family, and theInstituteofEducationSciences(R324A110086; school contexts that relate to parents’ expec- J. Blacher, P.I.) Support was also provided by the tations of schooling and academic achieve- SEARCH Family Autism Resource Center in the ment for their children with ASD, Bronfen- GraduateSchoolofEducation,UCRiverside.Cor- brenner’s(1979)ecologicalmodelprovidesa respondence concerning this article should be ad- useful guide (Eisenhower, Bush, & Blacher, dressedtoHillaryH.Bush,LearningandEmotional 2015; Odom et al., 2004). Ecological systems Assessment Program (LEAP), Department of Psy- chology,MassachusettsGeneralHospital,151Mer- theory suggests examining not only direct rimacSt.,5thfloor,Boston,MA,USA,02114.E-mail: contextualinfluences,suchasparents,school, [email protected] and peers (microsystem), but also the effects Parents’EducationalExpectations / 357 ofinteractionsbetweenthesecontexts,suchas been associated with parents’ educational ex- parent-teacher communications and relation- pectationsamongparentsoftypicallydevelop- ships(themesosystem).Moreexternalfactors ing children. For example, ethnic minority also affect children’s experiences, including parents,andparentswithlessannualincome thespecialeducationservicesystem(theexo- and fewer years of education have reported system), and broader, societal factors (e.g., lower and/or more misaligned expectations lawssuchastheIndividualswithDisabilitiesEd- of schooling for their children than parents ucationAct,2004)thatgovernserviceprovision from more socio-economically advantaged (the macrosystem). Finally, developmental backgrounds(Alexanderetal.,1994;deBoer changesovertime,forboththechildandthese &vanderWerf,2015;Yamamoto&Holloway, systems, warrant consideration (the chrono- 2010). Relatively less research has examined system). Overall, child, parent, teacher, class- the impact of teacher and school characteris- room,schoolfactors,andinteractionsamong tics (microsystem), parent-teacher relation- these systems are likely to influence school ships(mesosystem),andtheirassociationson adjustment for young children with ASD (Pi- parents’ educational expectations. However, anta, 2010). The importance of considering inarecentstudybyLawrence(2015),parents’ the influence of multiple environmental and income was found to moderate the relation contextual factors in the lives of individuals betweenracialdiversitywithintheschooland withdisabilitiesisreflectedintheInternational parents’ educational expectations. Specifi- Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (WHO, 2001). Given our aim – to ex- cally, for lower income parents, expectations ploretheeducationalexpectationsofparents were higher when schools were more racially whoraisechildrenwithASD–wedonotseek diverse;however,expectationswerenotcorre- to replicate Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) ecolog- lated with racial diversity of the school for ical model, but instead to examine how par- higherincomeparents.Whilethisfindingre- ents’educationalexpectationsmayshapechil- quiresadditionalinvestigation,itdoessuggest dren’s school experiences, including the that, to varying degrees, parents of typically interactions that children have with teachers. developing children consider teacher- and This study will support a more in-depth un- school-levelfactors(e.g.,studentbodycharac- derstanding of the microsystem and the me- teristics)whenformingexpectationsfortheir sosystem for the children with ASD in our children’s schooling. Teacher- and school- sample. levelfactorsmaybeparticularlyrelevantwhen achildhasASD,asparentsmayhaveparticu- ParentExpectationsforTypicallyDeveloping lar concerns about classroom placement, Children teacher qualifications, or the amount of time the child interacts with typical peers during Prior research with typically developing chil- theschoolday(Tobinetal.,2012).Forexam- drenhasshownthatchildcharacteristics,such ple, it may be the case that when parents as greater intellectual ability, greater aca- believethattheschoolsettingisagoodmatch demic achievement, and more positive atti- for their children’s needs and have confi- tudes toward learning, are positively associ- denceinteachers’abilities,thentheymaybe ated with parents’ educational expectations more optimistic and form more positive ex- (Briley, Harden, & Tucker-Drob, 2014; John- pectationsfortheschoolyearahead.Further, son, McGue, & Iacono, 2007; Sonuga-Barke, longitudinal studies among typically develop- Stevenson, Thompson, Lamparelli, & Gold- ing children have shown a positive, transac- foot, 1995). For example, Sonuga-Barke and colleagues (1995) found that parents held tional relation between parents’ expectations greater educational attainment expectations and children’s academic performance, with forchildrenwhoshowedmoreadvancedcog- parents’ expectations and academic achieve- nitive abilities, and physical and behavioral mentpredictingincreasesinoneanotherover self-regulation;thesefindingswereevidentfor time (Briley et al., 2014). Further research is childrenasyoungas3yearsold. neededtounderstandwhetherthesepatterns Parent and family characteristics also have alsoexistforparentsofchildrenwithASD. 358 / EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities-December2017 Parents’ExpectationsforChildrenwithASD and goal-setting between parents and teach- ers. Similarly, parents may hold expectations Whilemostresearchinthisareahasexamined that prioritize or emphasize different aspects parents’ educational expectations for their of the child’s development, such as social or typically developing children, a few studies emotional development versus academically- haveexaminedparents’school-relatedexpec- focused expectations. Further, tensions may tationsfortheiryoungchildrenwithASD.Ivey arise when parent expectations do not align and colleagues (2004) explored expectations with the services that are available or feasible of educational attainment, employment, and within the school district (Russell, 2005), or family life among 25 caregivers of children whenparentsencounterdifficultiescommuni- (4-20 years old) with ASD. In this study, par- cating or sharing their perspectives with the ents identified both the importance of certain educationalteam(Tucker&Schwartz,2013). goals, and the likelihood that their children On the other hand, parents of children with would achieve them. Parents rated attending ASD, who have an Individualized Education school highly important and they viewed it Plan (IEP), are more likely to be actively in- likely that their children with ASD would do volved in their children’s day-to-day educa- so.However,theyrated“achievingthehighest tional experiences (e.g., engaging in consis- education possible” and “being successful in tent communication with the child’s teacher school” as important, yet less likely to occur. or therapists, attending conferences and In another study, Chiang and colleagues meetings more frequently), as compared to (2012) examined how parents’ expectations parentsoftypicallydevelopingchildren,with- predictedpostsecondaryeducationfor830ad- outanIEP(Wagner,Newman,Cameto,Javitz, olescents with ASD. Parents’ expectations for & Valdes, 2012; Zablotsky, Boswell, & Smith, their children to pursue postsecondary ac- 2012). This regular involvement may provide countedforuniquevarianceinstudents’sub- parents with specific knowledge about their sequent educational attainment, after ac- child’s capabilities, resulting in more align- counting for variables such as children’s mentorspecificityinparents’expectations. verbal skills, and the presence or absence of intellectualdisability.Indeed,78%ofstudents ResearchQuestions who pursued postsecondary education had parentswhoexpectedthattheywould;75%of Thecurrentstudyispartofamultisite,longi- thosewhodidnotpursuepostsecondaryedu- tudinal study on the educational experiences cation had parents who expected that they of early elementary age children with ASD, wouldnot.Thesefindingsareconsistentwith with the overarching goal of better under- those of many non-ASD studies: parents’ ex- standing the factors that promote adaptation pectationswerestrongpredictorsofacademic toformalschoolingandpositiveschooladjust- achievement. mentforthispopulation.Thefocushereison Thereareimportantwaysinwhichexpecta- thefollowingtworesearchquestions: tions may differ between parents of typically developingchildrenandparentsofthosewith 1. Whattypesofschool-relatedexpectations ASD. Parents of children with ASD may face do parents hold for their young children uniquebarriersthatimpedeformationofpos- withASD? itive expectations of schooling. Such barriers 2. Which child-, parent/family-, and teach- might include parents, teachers, and thera- er/school-levelfactorsareassociatedwith pists having different goals and beliefs about parents’ educational expectations for what would be best for the child, which may theiryoungchildrenwithASD? impedethechildfrommaximizinghisorher academic potential (Parsons, Lewis, & Ellins, Method 2009; Russell, 2005). Parents’ and teachers’ expectationsmaydifferintheextenttowhich Participants they are achievable or realistic, given chil- dren’s current level of functioning; this con- Participantsinthecurrentstudyincludedtri- trast could lead to strained communications ads of youth with ASD (n (cid:2) 121, 83% male), Parents’EducationalExpectations / 359 oneparentperchild(n(cid:2)121,86%biological The interview did not ask specifically about mothers),andoneteacherperchild(n(cid:2)104, academic versus other types of expectations participation rate (cid:2) 86%). On average, chil- (e.g.,social,behavioral),withtheunderstand- dren were 5 years, 8 months (range (cid:2) 4–7 ingthatsomeparentsmightemphasizesome years)andparentswere38yearsold(range(cid:2) domains more so than others. If parents did 24–52).Mostofthechildrenattendedapub- not discuss the presence of characteristics or lic school (88%), and were in preschool experiencesthatmightmakeschooleasieror (37%), kindergarten (31%) or first grade more challenging in their initial response, (25%). About half of the children were in then they were asked specific follow-up ques- small,specialeducationclasses(overallmean tions about these. A detailed manual was de- classsize(cid:2)16)for50%ormoreoftheschool veloped for the current study, and a coding day. Parents racially identified as White team listened to recordings of parents’ re- (69%), Latino (9%), Asian American (8%), sponses and coded each one on a 4-point multiracial(8%),Black(6%),andotherraces scale, reflecting the level of success antici- (1%),basedonanopen-endeditemlaterag- pated for the child. The codes included: 4 (cid:2) gregated into categories. Most parents were “successful”, 3 (cid:2) “mostly successful”, 2 (cid:2) married (84%) and held at least a 4-year col- “somewhat successful”, and 1 (cid:2) “not at all lege degree (74%). Half of the families had successful” (see Table 2 for further explana- annual incomes above $80,000, and 54% of tion of each code and example quotes from parentrespondentsworkedoutsidethehome. parents). The coding team consensus-coded Families were recruited through schools, au- 20% of the interviews (n (cid:2) 25) and achieved tismclinics,andwordofmouthinthenorth- inter-raterreliabilityof88%. eastern United States (42%) and southern Child behavior problems. The parent-re- California (58%). Most teacher participants ported Child Behavior Checklist ages 1.5–5 and (n (cid:2) 104) were White (77%) and female the Child Behavior Checklist ages 6–18 (CBCL; (87%), with a master’s degree or higher Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000, 2001), depend- (66%).Teachershadanaverageof13.8years ingontheageofthechild,wereusedtoassess ofteachingexperience(range(cid:2)1–44). children’s behavior problems. Items present child problems alphabetically (from “aches and pains without medical cause” to “wor- Measures ries”),parentsareaskedtoratewhethereach Parents’ educational expectations. Each par- problemwasnottrue,somewhatorsometimestrue, ent completed a 20–35 minute structured, orverytrueoroftentruefortheirchildoverthe qualitative interview with a trained research past two months. The measures yield broad- assistant, which focused on different aspects bandInternalizingandExternalizingTscores of children’s school adjustment (e.g., getting (M (cid:2) 50, SD (cid:2) 10), with higher scores indi- alongwithteachers,stepstheparentsand/or cating higher levels of symptomatology. The teacherhadtakentopreparethechildforthe CBCLhasdemonstratedexcellentvalidityand upcomingschoolyear).Giventhelimitedex- both total and broadband scores are corre- isting research on school adjustment among lated with other measures of behavior prob- young children with ASD, a structured inter- lems (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000). In the view script was developed specifically for the currentstudy,Cronbach’salphasrangedfrom current study and a qualitative, phenomeno- .82to.91. logical approach to understanding parents’ Child social skills. The parent-reported So- perceptionsoftheirchild’seducationalexpe- cialSkillsImprovementSystem-RatingScales(SSIS- riences was employed. During the interview, RS; Gresham & Elliott, 2008) was adminis- the following question was presented to all tered to assess children’s social skills. On the participants: “How do you think your child SSIS-RS, respondents rate the frequency of will do this year in school?” The aim of this behaviorallyspecifiedsocialskillsona4-point question was to explore parents’ schooling scale of never, seldom, often, and almost always. expectationsfortheirchildinanintentionally Thescaleyieldsscoresthatcanbeconvertedto broad manner, in order to capture the full standardscores(M(cid:2)100;SD(cid:2)15);children’s range of expectations that they might hold. totalstandardscoreswereanalyzedinthecur- 360 / EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities-December2017 rent study. On this measure, higher scores On these subtests, higher scores indicate indicated higher levels of social skills. The greater skills. In the standardization sample, total standardized score was chosen as it has reliability of individual subtests ranged from demonstrated high internal consistency, .74 to .94. The WJ-III has strong test-retest strong construct validity, good test-retest reli- reliability and adequate validity with young ability, and good convergent validity children (Woodcock et al., 2007), and has (Gresham & Elliott, 2008). Versions of the beenusedwithchildrenwithASD(e.g.,New- SSIS-RS have been used to assess social func- manetal.,2007). tioninginchildrenwithautism(Frankel,My- Demographics. Background information was att,&Feinberg,2007)andintellectualdisabil- obtained through parent- and teacher-report ities (Neece & Baker, 2008). In the current surveys.Variablesincludedparentageinyears sample,Cronbach’salphawas.85. (continuous), parent education level (ordi- Children’s ASD symptoms. To assess ASD nal), parent employment (ordinal; full-time/ symptomatology,parentscompletedtheSocial part-time/not employed), household income Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino & Gru- (ordinal), teacher education level (ordinal), ber, 2005). The 65-item SRS assesses recep- years of teaching experience (continuous), tive, cognitive, expressive, and motivational classroom size (continuous; number of stu- aspects of social behavior, as well as autistic dents),andthepercentageoftimethetarget preoccupations. The SRS total T score (M (cid:2) child spent in the general education class- 50;SD(cid:2)10)distinguisheschildrenwithASD room(ordinal). fromchildrenwithandwithoutdifferentpsy- Parent-teacher interactions. Parents’ percep- chological disorders (Constantino, Przybeck, tions of their interactions and relationships Friesen, & Todd, 2000). A high score on this with their children’s teachers were measured measure indicates greater autism symptom- usingmodifiedversionsoftheparentalschool atology, or less social responsiveness. In the involvement subscale (16 items) and the par- currentsample,Cronbach’salphawas.88. ent-teacherrelationshipsubscale(9items)of Child language skills. To assess language the Parent-Teacher Involvement Questionnaire: skills,childrenwereadministeredtheComprehen- Parent (PTIQ-P; Corrigan, 2002; NICHD, sive Assessment of Spoken Language (CASL-2; 2005). Items on the parental school involve- Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999), a performance-based, ment subscale assessed how frequently the standardized assessment of language for indi- parent initiated or received different types of viduals 3–21 years old. All child participants contact from the child’s school or teacher completedtheSyntaxConstructionandPrag- (e.g.,“Ireceiveawrittenreportonmychild’s matic Judgment subtests; higher scores indi- progress or activities”), while items on the cate greater language skills. The CASL-2 has parent-teacher relationship subscale assessed demonstrated good construct validity and theparent’sfeelingsabouttheirinteractions, strongreliabilitiesof.90andhigher(Carrow- communications,andrelationshipspecifically Woolfolk, 1999). The measure has been withtheteacher(e.g.,“Ifeelcomfortabletalk- widely used among children with autism, as ing with my child’s teacher”). On these sub- wellaschildrenwithlanguagedelays,aphasia, scales, higher scores indicate higher levels of andintellectualdisabilities(e.g.,Reichow,Sal- parental school involvement and perceptions amack,Paul,Volkmar,&Klin,2008);itsinclu- of the parent-teacher relationship, respec- sionofverbalandnonverbalresponses,inde- tively.ThePTIQ-Phasshowngoodpredictive pendent of reading and writing, is also validity with measures of child academic en- advantageous. gagement and student-teacher relationship Childliteracyskills. Childrenwereadminis- quality in young children, and has distin- tered the following subtests of the Woodcock- guished between high- and low-risk families JohnsonIIINormativeUpdateTestsofAchievement (Corrigan, 2002). In the current sample, (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2007), each Cronbach’salphawas.60fortheinvolvement ofwhichwasselectedtoassessacentralaspect subscaleand.94fortherelationshipsubscale. of literacy: Letter-Word Identification (letter The relatively low alpha for the involvement and word knowledge), Word Attack (pho- subscale is consistent with a technical report nics), and Picture Vocabulary (vocabulary). published by the NICHD (Corrigan, 2002), Parents’EducationalExpectations / 361 and it likely reflects the fact that it included participants, and less than 5% were missing itemsreflectingbothparents’behaviors(e.g., forparent-reportquestionnairesandchildas- asking the teacher questions about one’s sessment measures. Teacher-report data were child), and teacher and school practices that missingfor23%ofthesample.ResultsofLit- arenotunderparentalcontrolorthatparents tle’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) maynotknowasmuchabout(despitereceiv- Test suggested that data were missing com- ing a written report about the child’s daily pletelyatrandom[(cid:2)2(10,N(cid:2)85–93)(cid:2)6.39, activities,oralogentry). p (cid:2) .78]. Thus, to avoid the loss of statistical powerassociatedwithlistwisedeletionofmiss- ingteacherdata,multipleimputationwasap- Procedure pliedinfiveiterationstoestimatecontinuous Participantsweredrawnfromamulti-site,lon- missingvaluesonthescalelevel. gitudinal study of school adaptation among children with ASD. At an initial visit at study offices, children were assessed for eligibility Results with the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) and a DescriptiveAnalysesofParents’Educational brief battery of Wechsler Preschool and Pri- Expectations mary Scale of Intelligence, 3rd Edition (WPPSI-III; Wechsler, 2002). This battery in- Qualitative analysis of parents’ interview re- cludedtheMatrixReasoning,Vocabulary,and sponsesshowedthat17%ofparentsexpressed Picture Completion subtests, which has dem- whollypositiveexpectations,withnoconcerns onstrated high reliability and convergent va- fortheschoolyearahead(code(cid:2)4,“success- lidity with the complete WPPSI-III (Sattler, ful”). These parents believed that their chil- 2008). Eligible participants were those who dren would succeed in school and discussed (a) scored in the autism or autism spectrum only positive factors, such as children’s read- range on the ADOS, (b) had received a pre- ing ability or comprehensive supports pro- vious diagnosis of ASD from an out-of-school videdthroughtheIEP.Thelargestpercentage clinicianoralsoscoredintheautismorautism of parents (48%) expressed mostly positive spectrum range on the Autism Diagnostic In- expectations, but also one or two concerns terview – Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le about the upcoming school year (code (cid:2) 3, Coulter, 1994), (c) earned an estimated IQ “mostlysuccessful”).Frequently,theseparents score of 50 or higher on the abbreviated discussedrelativelystrongacademicskillsand WPPSI-III battery, and (d) were ages 4 to 7 relatively weak social skills, as well as one or yearsandenteringelementaryschoolortheir two issues (e.g., a problem behavior) that final year of pre-K. Participating child and could cause challenges at school. Some par- parentdyadsreturnedtostudyofficesforad- ents(29%)hadmostlynegativeexpectations, ditionaldatacollectionaftertheeligibilityvis- citing three or more concerns, but still ex- it; parents were also given a questionnaire pressed some guarded optimism and hope packetforthechild’steachertocompleteand (code(cid:2)2,“somewhatsuccessful”).Thesepar- mail back to the study offices. Although this ents often expressed concerns about the was a multi-wave study, data presented here greater school environment, including chil- were from the first time point following the dren’seducationalteammemberswhomthey eligibility visit. Informed consents were ob- feltwerenotmeetingtheirresponsibilities,or tainedfromallparentsandteachersincluded accommodationsintheIEPthatwerenotbe- inthestudy,andtheyreceivedanhonorarium ing provided. Six percent of parents had for their participation. IRB approval was ob- whollynegativeexpectationsanddidnotiden- tainedforallaspectsofthestudy. tifyanypositive,hopeful,orprotectivefactors thatcouldhelptheirchildrenadjusttoschool (code(cid:2)1,“notatallsuccessful”).Thus,about DataAnalyses a third of parents had negative expectations AnalyseswereperformedwithSPSS22.0.Par- about their child’s performance and experi- ent interview data were complete for all 121 enceinschool(seeTable1). 362 / EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities-December2017 TABLE 1 Parents’QualitativeResponsesRegardingEducationalExpectations(N(cid:2)121) %of Code Interpretation sample Illustrativequotes 4 “successful”(cid:2)parentsexhibited 17 “Ithinkhe’sgoingtonailkindergarten.Healreadyis. onlypositiveexpectationsabout Theteachertoldmynannythatshe’snevergivenso theirchild’sschooling manyawardstoakidbefore,andthatwasinmid- October...the academics of kindergarten, he’s alreadywellabovewhereheshouldbe.” 3 “mostlysuccessful”(cid:2)parents 48 “Ifeelhe’salwaysdonewellandwillthisyearalso.I exhibitedmainlypositive think.Hethrivesinthatconstant-typeschool expectationsabouttheirchild’s schedule ... understanding how to behave in schooling,butalsodiscussed1–2 school, he’s doing very well. He’s thriving in that concerns kind of structured environment... in all things except straight academics, first-grader level-type stuff, I think he’s going to do well this year. Of course, that’s part of an IEP, whatever level he’s academically. We don’t really have expectations about[academics].” 2 “somewhatsuccessful”(cid:2)parents 29 “Academically,Ithinkhe’sgoingtobeabletostayon exhibitedmainlynegative track... however, I have worries that he’s just not expectationsfortheirchild’s going to be able to work independently enough to schooling,citing3ormore function well in a class. Again, I think he’s very concerns,butalsoexpressed immature. It really worries me that he might not fit guardedoptimismandhope in with the other kids who are at first-grade mentality. I think socially he’s going to really, really struggle.” 1 “notatallsuccessful”(cid:2)parents 6 “Idon’tknow.He’sgoingtohaveahardtime.We exhibitedwhollynegative alreadyhadameetingbecausehe’snotdoingwell expectationsandhaddifficulty transitioningandhasbeenrefusingtogotoschool. identifyinganypositive,hopeful, So,I’mworriedhe’snotgoingtodowellthisyear.” orprotectivefactors FactorsAssociatedwithParents’Educational Parent- and family-level factors. Higher par- Expectations ent educational expectations were associated with higher levels of parental education Child-level factors. Higher parent educa- [rs(119) (cid:2) .19, p (cid:2) .04]. Male parents were tionalexpectationswereassociatedwithlower observedtoholdhigherexpectationsthanfe- levels of parent-reported internalizing prob- lems[rs(119)(cid:2)(cid:3).24,p(cid:2).01],externalizing male parents [t(119) (cid:2) 2.19, p (cid:2) .03], but problems [rs(119) (cid:2) (cid:3).34, p (cid:4) .001], ASD since the vast majority of parent participants symptoms [rs(119) (cid:2) (cid:3).29, p (cid:4) .01], and were female, this finding should be inter- marginally with children’s age [rs(119) (cid:2) pretedwithcaution.Parentexpectationswere (cid:3).20, p (cid:2) .06. Higher parents’ expectations not associated with employment status or also were associated with emerging reading householdincome. skills [rs(119) (cid:2) .24, p (cid:4) .01] and phonics Teacher- and school-level factors. Higher par- skills[rs(119)(cid:2).19,p(cid:2).04],andmarginally ent educational expectations were associated with greater social skills [rs(119) (cid:2) .18, p (cid:2) with more positive perceptions of the quality .06]. Parents’ expectations were not associ- of the parent-teacher relationship [rs(119) (cid:2) atedwithchildgender,IQ,orlanguageskills .19, p (cid:2) .04], and marginally with smaller (pragmatic language, syntactical language classroomsizes[rs(119)(cid:2)(cid:3).17,p(cid:2).08].As skills,orvocabulary;seeTable2). compared to parents who reported lower ex- Parents’EducationalExpectations / 363 TABLE 2 DescriptiveStatisticsandRelationsbetweenChild-LevelFactorsandParents’Expectations %inBorderlineor Relationwith N(%of ClinicallyElevated Parents’ Sample) Range M SD Median Range Expectations Parents’expectations 121 1–4 2.78 .80 3 — — Child-levelfactors Gender 121 83%male — — — — t(119)(cid:2).69 EstimatedIQ 121 46–123 89.6 17.1 90 13.3/12.4 .01 Age(months) 121 44–90 66.2 11.8 67 — (cid:3).17† Socialskills 121 40–120 78.9 15.0 79 27.6/24.1 .18† Internalizingbehavior 118 34–93 62.4 9.9 63 22.0/42.4 (cid:3).24* Externalizingbehavior 118 32–88 58.9 10.4 59 16.1/31.4 (cid:3).34*** ASDsymptoms 120 46–90 78.1 11.6 81 21.8/22.7 (cid:3).29** Language:Syntax 120 41–152 84.5 19.0 85.5 19.0/21.7 .07 Language:PragmaticJudgment 120 43–142 81.9 17.8 81.5 17.5/31.7 .02 Literacy:Letter-WordIdentification 121 49–156 111.1 18.3 111 1.6/1.6 .24** Literacy:WordAttack 104 65–156 114.3 17.7 114 1.0/3.0 .19* Literacy:PictureVocabulary 121 14–137 96.7 15.9 98 6.4/2.5 .07 Note. ***p(cid:4).001. **p(cid:4).01. *p(cid:4).05.†p(cid:4).10. pectations, parents held higher expectations skills were very highly correlated with each whentheyexperiencedapositiverelationship other [r (119) (cid:2) .79, p (cid:4) .001] and to avoid with their child’s teacher; their expectations multicollinearity,thephonicskillsvariablewas were slightly lower when their children were excludedfromthemodel.Aparent/family-level attendinglargerclasses.Parents’expectations factor, parents’ education, was entered in the were not associated with teacher education secondstep.Finally,ateacher/school-levelfactor, level,yearsofteachingexperience,amountof parent-teacher relationship quality, was en- timethechildspendsingeneraleducation,or tered in the third step. When these factors levelofparentalschoolinvolvement. were considered simultaneously, only exter- nalizingbehavior(B(cid:2)(cid:3).09,SE(cid:2).03,OR(cid:2) .91, p (cid:4) .01) significantly predicted parents’ RegressionAnalyses expectations (see Table 3). Specifically, par- ents with children who exhibited fewer chal- Inthefinalsetofanalyses,thechild-,parent/ lenging behaviors reported more positive ex- family-, and teacher/school-level factors pectations for the school year ahead, as found to be significantly correlated with par- comparedtoparentswithchildrenwhoexhib- ents’ expectations were entered as predictors itedmorebehaviorproblems. into a binary logistic regression. Given the ordinalnatureoftheparentexpectationvari- able,itwasdichotomized(codesof3and4(cid:2) Discussion “higher expectations”, codes of 1 and 2 (cid:2) “lowerexpectations”)beforebeingenteredas Many parents of children with ASD were op- the dependent variable. The logistic regres- timisticabouttheschoolyearahead,withthe sion contained three steps. Child-level factors majority expressing mostly positive expecta- including internalizing behavior, externaliz- tions and only mild concerns (48%), and ingbehavior,ASDsymptoms,emergingread- some expressing wholly positive expectations ingskills,andvocabularyskillswereenteredin andnoconcernsatall(17%).Thesegenerally the first step. While phonics skills also were positive expectations were consistent with significantly correlated with parents’ expecta- Ivey’s (2004) findings and may set an early, tions, phonics skills and emerging reading positivetoneforinteractionsamongstudents, 364 / EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities-December2017 TABLE 3 SummaryofBinaryLogisticRegressionAnalysisforChild-,Parent/Family-,andTeacher/School-Level VariablesPredictingParents’Expectations B SE AdjustedOR Step1:Child-levelfactors Internalizingbehavior .02 .03 1.02 Externalizingbehavior (cid:3).09** .03 .91 ASDsymptoms (cid:3).03 .03 .98 Emergingreadingskills .02 .02 1.02 Vocabularyskills .00 .02 1.00 Step2:Parent/family-levelfactor Parenteducation Highschoolorless 1.23 1.11 3.44 Somecollege .39 .93 1.47 Collegedegree (cid:3).19 .87 .83 Master’sdegree .02 .91 1.02 Step3:Teacher/school-levelfactor Parent-teacherrelationshipquality .03 .03 1.03 Note. ***p(cid:4).001. **p(cid:4).01. *p(cid:4).05.†p(cid:4).10.Thistablereflectsimputeddata. parents,andteachers.Althoughaboutathird Uniquetothecurrentstudywastheextent ofparentsdidexpressnegativeviews,ourfind- to which children’s behavioral characteristics ingssuggestthatnegativeexpectationsarerel- and ASD symptoms were associated with par- atively less common among parents of young ents’expectations.Indeed,whenotherfactors childrenwithASD.Thus,whenschoolprofes- were accounted for, children’s externalizing sionals do encounter parents with low expec- problems were the sole predictor of parents’ tations, they should try to understand and educational expectations. Surprisingly, par- addressthereasonswhyparentsmightbefeel- ents’ educational expectations were uncorre- ing less optimistic. Based on the assumption latedwithchildren’sintelligenceorestimated thatparents’expectationsmayplayanimpor- IQ – a departure from the findings of some tantrolefortypicallydevelopingchildrenand previousnon-ASDstudies(e.g.,Johnsonetal., children with ASD alike, it is important to 2007; Sonuga-Barke et al., 1995). Behavioral understand and address the barriers to par- development and adjustment are areas of entsholdingpositiveeducationalexpectations need for many children with ASD; as such, for their children with ASD. Specifically, the these areas are more salient on a day-to-day current study revealed that parents held low basis and likely more closely reflected in par- expectations for their children’s school out- ents’ expectations than IQ or academic fac- comeswhentheirchildrenexhibitedexternal- tors. Another paper with the same sample as izing behavior problems. Although this study the current study indicated that behavioral was correlational and could not address cau- challenges, more so than intellectual chal- sality,thisfindingsuggeststhatearlyinterven- lenges, accounted for difficulties with school tionproviderswouldparticularlybenefitfrom adjustment(citationremovedforanonymous training in addressing challenging behaviors peerreview).Thecurrentfindingsreflectthe and in coaching parents to respond to these factors that may help to align IEP goals and behaviorsthemselves.Conversely,itispossible expectations between parents and teachers. thatchildren’sexternalizingbehaviorsarein- Additionally, this understanding may help terferingwithsuccessinschoolandindirectly teachers and parents shape behavioral and affecting parents’ expectations, thus under- socio-emotional curriculum development in scoring the importance of supporting teach- theclassroom. ers to address challenging behaviors in the In addition to child-level characteristics, classroom. some relations were observed between par- Parents’EducationalExpectations / 365 ents’expectationsandparent/family-levelfac- majority of our child participants attended tors and teacher/school-level factors. Within publicschools,thusreflectingtheeducational Bronfenbrenner’s (1979) microsystem, par- experiencesofthemajorityofyoungchildren ents who reported higher educational levels withASDintheUnitedStates. heldmorepositiveexpectationsfortheirchil- Itislikewiseimportanttonotethatcurrent dren’s schooling than parents who reported findings may not generalize to the school ex- lower educational levels. Within Bronfen- periencesofchildrenwithASDandmoderate- brenner’s(1979)mesosystem,parentswhore- to-severe intellectual disability. Also, many ported a positive or higher relationship qual- participating families were economically re- ity with their child’s teacher also reported sourced. While household income was not higher academic expectations, compared to found to be significantly correlated with par- parents who reported a negative or lower re- ents’ expectations, our findings may not cap- lationship quality with their child’s teacher. ture the experiences of economically disad- Parents’positivereportsoftheparent-teacher vantaged parents of children with ASD. As is relationship may indicate a strong working the case with all interview data, there is the relationship with the teacher, a belief in the possibilityofsocialdesirabilityeffects,orthat teacher’s capabilities to meet the needs of parents may have overemphasized the extent theirchild,andparents’activeinvolvementin to which they felt positive about school. Also school – all factors, logically, that could in- as noted, the correlational nature of the cur- crease parents’ academic expectations. Inter- rent study cannot speak to causality or to estingly, parents’ perceptions of parent- transactionalrelations.Specifically,itisuncer- teacher relationship quality were significantly tain whether parents held high expectations correlated with their expectations for the for their children because they noticed chil- school year, while parents’ self-reported in- dren’sstrengths,whetherchildrendeveloped volvementwiththeschoolwasnot.Similarto greaterstrengthsinresponsetotheirparents’ ourfindings,Zablotskyandcolleagues(2012) positive expectations, or whether there were found that parents’ level of school involve- unmeasured variables that influenced both mentwasuncorrelatedwiththeiroverallsatis- expectationsandchildoutcomes. factionwiththeschoolamongparentsofchil- Animportantareaforfutureinvestigationis drenwithASD.Thesefindingssuggestthatit whether parents’ expectations play a unique isimportanttoconsiderthenatureandqual- roleinpredictinglaterschooladjustmentand ity of parents’ interactions with their child’s academicoutcomesforchildrenwithASD,as teacher and school – not just the quantity – they may for typically developing children when examining the relation between paren- (e.g., de Boer & van der Werf, 2015). In- tal school involvement and their feelings creased understanding of parents’ expecta- abouttheirchild’sschoolexperiences. tions, and the different factors they consider when forming these expectations, may ulti- mately inform supports for parents as they StrengthsandLimitations navigateschoolingfortheirchildwithASD. The current study contains multiple method- ological strengths, including a large sample recruitedfromthecommunity,theuseofthe References ADOS to verify ASD diagnosis among child participants,andtheeffortsofacodingteam, Achenbach,T.M.,&Rescorla,L.A.(2000).Manual which achieved high inter-rater reliability, to fortheASEBApreschoolforms&profiles.Burlington, code qualitative interview data. The current VT:UniversityofVermontDepartmentofPsychi- atry. study also benefited from a high rate of Achenbach,T.M.,&Rescorla,L.A.(2001).Manual teacher participation. While children’s level for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burling- of cognitive functioning was not found to be ton,VT:UniversityofVermont,ResearchCenter significantly correlated with parents’ expecta- forChildren,Youth,&Families. tions,theinclusionofchildrenwitharangeof Alexander,K.L.,Entwisle,D.R.,&Bedinger,S.D. cognitiveabilitiesincreasestheexternalvalid- (1994).Whenexpectationswork:Raceandsocio- ityandapplicabilityofourwork.Further,the economicdifferencesinschoolperformance.So- 366 / EducationandTraininginAutismandDevelopmentalDisabilities-December2017

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.