ebook img

ERIC EJ1134394: Emergent Literacy Development and Computer Assisted Instruction PDF

2017·0.29 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ1134394: Emergent Literacy Development and Computer Assisted Instruction

Emergent Literacy Development and Computer Assisted Instruction Judy Trotti: University of Mary Hardin-Baylor Randy Hendricks: University of Mary Hardin-Baylor Christie Bledsoe: University of Mary Hardin-Baylor In this mixed-methods study, researchers examined the literacy development of prekindergarten students (N = 162) randomly placed in one of two treatment groups with each receiving 15 minutes of computer-assisted literacy instruction for four months. Literacy development of a control group of children not receiving computer-assisted instruction was contrasted with the two treatment groups. All children in the study were eligible for free or reduced lunch. Responses from a semi- structured focus group of prekindergarten teachers (N = 5) were analyzed for corroboration. Although all three groups progressed in literacy development, the control group had significantly larger gains (p < 0.01). The effect size was moderate (eta squared = 0.63). Qualitative data supported the use of one computer program over another, but none of the teachers supported daily use of either software treatment. H istorically, instruction in literacy has 2000). The National Center for Family occurred in first grade. This practice Literacy (2004) reviewed studies that defined was based in part on the conclusions early literacy skills needed for young of Morphett and Washburn (1931) who found children. Skills listed in the panel’s report are that maturity was an important factor in alphabet knowledge; phonological gauging a student’s ability to begin to read. awareness; rapid automatic naming of letters, Olson (1949) and Gesell (1940; 1946) also digits, objects, and colors; writing letters or promoted the developmental theory of name; phonological memory; concepts about reading readiness. However, Clay (1966) books; print knowledge; oral language; and disavowed the maturational theories of visual processing. Instructional practices are reading readiness and coined the term most useful when they are code-focused, “emergent literacy” to describe what children involve shared reading, and promote know about reading and writing before they language development focused on early can actually read and write conventionally. literacy skills. When children imitate reading and writing activities, they are practicing emergent Pre-K in the United States literacy behaviors (Cecil, Baker, & Lozano, 2015). According to longitudinal studies, Over the past six decades, research on children who attended a high quality emergent literacy has found a foothold in preschool were more prepared for teaching practices designed for children kindergarten and outperformed their peers on under age six. Data from longitudinal studies language, literacy, and mathematics (Freede, reveal continuity between reading-related 2009). Further, these positive effects skills exhibited by preschool children and persisted through elementary school at the their reading performance in elementary least. In one longitudinal study, adults at age school (Lonigan, Burgess, & Anthony, 40 who experienced a quality preschool SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 30 program had higher salaries, were more competence in technology integration with likely to be employed, committed fewer applications of technology that are crimes, and were more likely to be high developmentally appropriate. Three different school graduates (Schweinhart, et al., 2005). types of technology-based curricula were Thirty-nine states in the U.S. provide state- examined in the above-mentioned literature funded pre-K for children who are at-risk reviewed including Warterford Early (WSDEL, 2013) for future high school Reading Program (Johnson et al., Tracy and graduation because of other hardships. In Young, 2007 and Powers and Price-Johnson, some states, children are eligible to attend 2007), ‘Ready, Set, Leap!’ (Davidson et al., free preschool if they meet all or some of the 2009) and LitTECH Outreach (Johanson, following stipulations: come from a home Bell, & Daytner, 2008). Researchers with a limited income, have a parent in the reported no convincing evidence that any of military, have ever been in foster care, are these technology-based curricula resulted in homeless, or do not speak or comprehend significant literacy gains when compared to English by the time they are three years old the control groups. (TEA, 2009; WSDEL, 2013). When CAI was used as an Computer-Assisted Instruction intervention with pre-K children to advance (CAI). CAI refers to specific computer vocabulary knowledge and promote reading applications or supplemental activities to comprehension, the results were moderate enhance the teacher’s instruction. It differs (Spencer, et. al, 2012). Andrews (2004) from computer-based education and showed a statistically significant positive computer-based instruction, which are terms correlation between technology integration used to represent general use of computers in and literacy learning for children ages 7-11. the classroom. The use of computer Children who used technology to enhance applications as a supplement to instruction literacy learning had a 16% acceleration of promotes student interest and motivation learning compared with children without with pictorial displays, self-pacing, and technology integration. In the same study, positive feedback built in the programs positive results in literacy progress were not (Macaruso & Walker, 2008). Teacher roles evident for children ages 11-16. are also found to be important for support of Paterson, Henry, O’Quin, Ceprano, CAI. Teachers of young children who anchor and Blue (2003) scrutinized the effectiveness their knowledge about developmentally of the Waterford Early Reading Program appropriate practices regarding literacy with kindergarteners in an urban public instruction to classroom management and school system. The Waterford Program research support for early literacy instruction involved practice in rhyming, sound have found CAI more effective than those segmenting and blending, alphabet skills, and lacking knowledge of a specific role of the concepts of print. While findings failed to teacher (Robinson et al., 2006). support success of the program, teacher Data from various studies concerning performance variables were associated with the educational benefits from use of CAI differences in classroom performance of the show mixed results. A review of 46 articles kindergarteners. Students with teachers who (Belo, MeKenney, Voogt, & Bradley, 2016), facilitated literacy learning and maximized relating to early literacy development shows instructional time showed slightly more that CAI supports children’s early literacy progress than the CAI students. Parr and development when it is used appropriately. Fung (2000) concluded “the effect of Suitable use of CAI integrates teacher SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 31 computer-assisted instruction has not been 1978). According to Weiner (1986), conclusively demonstrated” (p. V). students’ perceptions of their ability The purpose of this paper is to influence performance. When learners compare the effect of two computer programs believe poor performance is beyond their with a control group on the literacy learning control, they tend to surrender easily. of at-risk children at one pre-K campus. The Attribution theory emphasizes the state in which this study was conducted importance of meeting needs of individual provides free pre-K for four-year-olds who students and modifying learning meet at least one of the following environments to provide for optimal success. requirements: children of military parent(s), Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory emphasizes homeless, low income, or lack of English the idea that social factors highly influence language proficiency. However, only two of development. When individuals interact and the ten quality standards noted by NIEER explore together, cognitive development is (2014) are evident in the programing. The accelerated. two quality benchmarks required by this state Both computer programs used in this include comprehensive early learning study provided for individualized learning standards and inclusion of at least 15 hours of within the classroom setting. Feedback was in-service for teachers annually. provided through successful accomplishment Students leaving this pre-K in 2013 of various tasks within the program design lacked kindergarten readiness because of through auditory and visual reinforcements. poor literacy skills, so school district leaders Since children were working independently sought remedies for the lack of literacy of one another while involved in CAI, social proficiency. A pilot study was implemented interactions with other children and the in which two computer programs were teacher were limited. examined to determine if the district should Methods. A mixed methods approach purchase the software to help pre-K students was appropriate to determine both the with literacy development. The study aimed literacy attainment of pre-K students and the to answer the following research questions: teachers’ perceptions of each computer (1) What are the differences in software program. A concurrent- attainment of critical literacy skills of pre-K triangulation design (Fraenkel & Wallen, students before and after the treatment of (a) 2009) was implemented since both Imagine Learning (b) Waterford Early quantitative and qualitative data were Learning, or (c) classroom instruction collected and analyzed at approximately the without software instruction? same time. A between-subjects design with (2) How did the pre-K teachers’ a control group was used for the quantitative perceive the effectiveness of Imagine and phase. Waterford Early Learning computer Through analytic induction, focus programs for literacy development of group conversations were explored in the students? qualitative phase after three months of implementation. Analytic induction allows Theoretical Framework for a proposition that can be verified through qualitative data (Patton, 2002). The focus Improvement of early literacy skills group was semi-structured with questions can be examined through the theoretical designed before the group met. The focus frameworks of attribution theory (Weiner, group meeting allowed pre-K teachers (N = 1986) and sociocultural theory (Vygotsky, 5) to describe their observations of student SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 32 learning and experiences with and without to promote literacy development (Imagine the software support. Teacher responses to Learning, n.d.). Students worked at their own nine questions were coded to reflect level and pace. Waterford Early Learning observations and expectations about the (Group B) was advertised as research-based results of the programs on early literacy and child-friendly for students in pre-K learning. through second grade. Audio and multimedia Data Collection. Four-year-old pre- prompts facilitated self-paced progress K students (N = 162) in the study attended (Waterford, n.d.). school for a half-day (3.5 hours) and were Data Analysis and Findings divided into three groups: Group A (n = 51) Quantitative Findings. There were no received daily instruction of 15 minutes using significant differences in two areas of Imagine Learning computer software; Group literacy: Letter Recognition and Vocabulary B (n = 60) received daily instruction of 15 Recognition. The data showed significant minutes using Waterford Early Learning; and differences in Phonemic Awareness and in a Group C (n = 51) received the regular composite literacy score. The ANOVA instruction by the teacher without access to results revealed that instructional approach the computer programs. Students in the had a significant effect on phonics gain, F(2, experimental groups rotated to the computers 159) = 10.196, p < 0.001 Tukey’s post hoc during the time normally allocated for test indicated that the Control group mean of learning centers. All students were native 7.8 was significantly higher than both the English speakers and were eligible for free or Imagine group mean (M = 3.1) and the reduced lunch programs. Waterford group mean (M = 4.5). The Pre- and post-tests using mCLASS: difference between the Imagine group and CIRCLE by Amplify Insight were used to Waterford group was not significant (Tables measure literacy skills before and after the 1 and 2). The effect size (eta squared = treatment. Although the pre- and post-tests 0.114) is considered moderate (Stern, 2010). were not identical, the skill level was similar with questions being randomized in the Table 1 testing. The assessment provided brief tasks that were automatically scored and timed by Descriptive Statistics for Phonics Gain a teacher using an iPad to measure development of children individually. Method Mean Std. N Teachers administered both the pre- and post- Deviation assessments using this computer-based control 7.8627 7.40546 51 instrument. Imagine 3.0980 4.26265 51 Following discussions with school Waterford 4.5167 4.38986 60 district leaders and classroom observations, Total 5.1235 5.79223 162 researchers developed nine questions (Appendix A) to guide semi-structured focus group conversations. One researcher asked questions while one researcher took notes. Additionally, a tape recorder was used to record responses. Imagine Learning (Group A) software was designed to engage students with interactive activities, games, and videos SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 33 Table 2 The ANOVA revealed that instructional approach had a significant effect ANOVA Summary for Phonics Gain on literacy composite gains, F(2, 159) = 5.350, p =.006 (Tables 5 and 6). Tukey’s post hoc test determined that the Control group mean of 19.3 was significantly higher than both the Imagine group (M = 11.8) and the Waterford group (M = 13.1). The effect size (eta squared = 0.63) is considered moderate (Stern, 2010). Table 5 Descriptive Statistics for Literacy Composite Both the Waterford group (M = 32.5) Gain and the Imagine group (M = 32.2) were found to be significantly different from the Control Method Mean Std. N group (M = 31.0) when controlling for pre- Deviation test scores, F(2, 158) = 5.883, p = 0.003 control 19.2745 15.56288 51 (Tables 3 and 4). The effect size (eta squared Imagine 11.7843 10.96050 51 = 0.069) is considered moderate (Stern, Waterford 13.1000 10.46009 60 2010). Total 14.6296 12.76296 162 Table 3 Table 6 Descriptive Statistic for Phonics Post-Test ANOVA Summary for Literacy Composite Gain Method Mean Std. N Deviation control 31.0392 10.01990 51 Imagine 32.1961 8.05983 51 Waterford 32.4500 9.88480 60 Total 31.9259 9.35601 162 Table 4 The ANCOVA revealed that the ANCOVA Summary for Phonics Post-Test Waterford group post-test literacy composite with Phonics Pre-Test as a Covariate mean of 79.1 was greater than both the Imagine group (M =7 8.5) and the Control group (M = 72.2). However, the differences between the means were found to be not significant, F(2,158) = 2.816, p = 0.063 (Tables 7 and 8). These results indicate that instructional approach had no discernable effect on literacy composite post-test scores. SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 34 Table 7 Teachers’ suggestions for future use of either/both software programs is Descriptive Statistics for Literacy Composite categorized in these ways: time, motivating Post-Test students, literacy skill development, technological tools, and program choice. Method Mean Std. N Summarily, teachers perceived daily use of Deviation either software program should be control 72.1765 22.18441 51 abandoned. Teachers recommended using Imagine 78.4902 25.90704 51 either program 2-3 times per week in the Waterford 79.1167 24.64878 60 computer lab to avoid distractions. Updated Total 76.7346 24.36391 162 computers could prevent technology failures. Waterford was seen as the most helpful Table 8 program by four of five teachers due to its use of higher order thinking and tools that help ANCOVA Summary for Literacy Composite children stay on track. Imagine Learning was Post-Test with Pre-Test as Covariate perceived as more of a skills-based program lacking authentic classroom connections for students. One teacher had no preference between the two programs and valued each equally. Convergence of Findings. The control (Group C) had larger gains in the composite score than either Imagine (Group A) or Waterford (Group B). The qualitative data corroborates these findings by relating Qualitative Findings. Researchers implementation issues with both programs color-coded focus group responses and related to scheduling, technology delays, and categorized them in four ways: (a) positive resistance of some children. Some children and negative aspects of Waterford Early wanted to be at learning centers while others Learning; (b) positive and negative aspects of were distracted by tasks performed by peers. Imagine Learning; (c) positive and negative The control group experienced greater aspects of both programs, and (d) suggestions flexibility in time management and tasks of for future use of either/both software choice. programs. When composite post-test scores Teachers reported some positive were examined with the pre-test as a perceptions about both programs. Both covariate, the differences between the three provided troubleshooting, personalization, groups were not statistically significant. instruction and practice with literacy skills, Teachers clearly supported Waterford Early and increased readiness for technology use. Learning over Imagine Learning for literacy Teachers’ negative responses about both development of students. One teacher programs related to scheduling challenges, commented, “I have seen students make outdated computers, resistance of some connections from the program (Waterford). children, and advanced difficulty level for They learned rhyming on Waterford and struggling learners. remembered when I introduced it in class.” SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 35 Implications. Educators search for References strategies to improve the academic achievement of students. CAI has gained Andrews, R. (2004). The impact of ICT on literacy education. New York: prominence in this century as an instructional RoutledgeFalmer. tool; however, until recently few studies have Belo, N., MeKenney, S., Voogt, J., & been available to describe the effect of Bradley, B. (2016). Teacher computer programs on early literacy knowledge for using technology to learning. Recent studies show mixed results foster early literacy: A literature regarding effect on early literacy learning review. Computers in Human when CAI is implemented as part of the Behavior, 60, 372-383. curriculum. This study provides evidence Clay, M. M. (1966). Emergent reading that the classroom teacher provides as much behavior (Unpublished doctoral or more instructional support when compared dissertation). University of Auckland, with CAI in early literacy development. Pre- New Zealand. K students in this study may have had poor Cotton, K. (1991). Computer-assisted performance due to factors beyond their instruction. Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory School control as described when students cried or Improvement Research Series (Series ignored the teacher when asked to use either V, Close-Up #10). Retrieved from computer program. This behavior is Education Northwest website: supported by attribution theory (Weiner, http://educationnorthwest.org/sites/d 1986) in which students believe they are efault /files/Computer- incapable of success, so they easily give up. AssistedInstruction.pdf Additionally, sociocultural theory Dillon, A. and Zhu, E. (1997). Designing (Vygotsky, 1978) provides additional support Web-Based Instruction: A Human- for poor performance with CAI since some Computer Interaction (HCI) children worked on the computer programs Perspective. In B. Khan (ed.) Web- while others worked in table groups. Results Based Instruction (pp. 221-225), of this study support previous findings of Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Educational Belo et al., 2016, concerning CAI use with Technology Publications. Fraenkel, J.R., & Wallen, N.E. (2009). How young children. However, in the Belo study, to design and evaluate research on CAI was used with kindergarten children. teaching (7th ed.). New York: Implications from this study suggest that McGraw-Hill. independent practice may not be Frede, E., Kwanghee, J., Barnett, S.W., & developmentally appropriate for some Figueras, A. (2009). The APPLES children of pre-K age who desire more blossom: Abbott preschool program interaction with peers and the teacher. More longitudinal effects study (APPLES), research is needed across age groups and preliminary results through 2nd types of computer software to determine Grade. Retrieved from the National broader findings on the effects of computer Institute for Early Education assisted instruction on literacy skill Research website: http://nieer- development. www1.rutgers.edu/pdf/apples_secon d_grade_results.pdf SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 36 Hecht, S. A, & Close, L. (2002). Emergent Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative evaluation literacy skills and training time and research methods (3rd ed.). uniquely predict variability in Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. responses to phonemic awareness Robinson, L., Johanson, J., Schneider, C., & training in disadvantaged Hutinger, P. (2006). Final report: kindergartners. Journal of Interactive technology literacy Experimental Child Psychology, 82, curriculum online (ITLC Online). 93–115. Macomb, IL: Western Illinois Imagine Learning Programs (n.d.). In University. (ERIC Document Imagine Learning. Retrieved from Reproduction Service No. http://www.imaginelearning.com/pro ED495396) grams/imaginelearning/ Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Ingersoll, R. (2003). Is there really a teacher Barnett, W. S., Belfield, C. R., & shortage. Retrieved from the Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime effects: University of Pennsylvania Grade The HighScope Perry Preschool School of Education website: study through age 40: Summary, http://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/ conclusions, and frequently asked viewcontent.cgi?article=1133&conte questions. Retrieved from the xt=gse_pubs HighScope Educational Research Macaruso, P. & Walker, A. (2008). The Foundation website: efficacy of computer assisted http://www.highscope.org/file/specia instruction for advancing literacy lsummary_ rev2015_01.pdf skills in kindergarten students. Spencer, E.J., Goldstein, H., Sherman, A., Reading Psychology, 29, 266–287 Noe, S., Tabbah, R., Ziolkowski, R. National Center for Family Literacy. (2004). & Schneider, N. (2012). Effects of an The report of the national early automated vocabulary and literacy panel. Washington, D.C.: comprehension intervention an early United States Government. efficacy study. Journal of Early Parr, J. M., & Fung, I. (2000). A review of Intervention 34(4), 195-221. the literature on computer-assisted Stern, (2010). Statistics for the behavioral learning, particularly integrated sciences. A visual approach to SPSS learning systems, and outcomes with for windows: A guide to SPSS 17.0. respect to literacy and numeracy. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Retrieved from Washington State Department of Early http://www.educationcounts.govt.nz/ Learning, (WSDEL). (2013). Pre- __data/assets/pdf_file/0004/7672/A- kindergarten programs: An overview Review-of-the-Literature-on- and comparison with other states. Computer-Assisted.pdf Retrieved from Paterson, W. A., Henry, J.J., O’Quin, K., http://leg.wa.gov/Senate/Committees Ceprano, M.A. & Blue, E.V. (2003). /EDU/Documents/UniversalVoluntar Investigating the effectiveness of an yPreschool.pdf integrated learning system on early Waterford Early Learning (n.d.) In emergent readers. Reading Research Waterford. Retrieved from Quarterly, 38, 172–207. http://www.waterford.org/waterford- early-learning/ SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 37 Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag. Wise, B., Ring, J., & Olson R.K., (2000). Individual differences in gains from computer-assisted remedial reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77, 197–235. SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 38 Appendix A Focus Group questions 1. Describe the implementation process of the programs. What types of issues did you encounter? Were there challenges? 2. Describe the ease of use of the programs for students. Were they user-friendly? 3. Describe the student’s responses to using each program. What behaviors did you observe? 4. Did each program motivate or engage students? How do you know? Describe student behaviors or comments to support your observation. 5. For teachers who used both programs, which one do you project will produce better results on the 7 literacy skills? 6. Did you observe a transfer of learning to classroom activities/instruction for either program? 7. Were the programs beneficial to students? How? Describe how you know? 8. Were the teacher supplemental resources useful to you? Which ones and why? 9. Do you have any suggestions for using the programs during the instructional day? More time? Less time? None? SRATE Journal Winter 2017, Volume 26(1) Page 39

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.