ebook img

ERIC EJ1114541: A Comparison of the Handwriting Abilities of Secondary Students with Visual Impairments and Those of Sighted Students PDF

2015·0.08 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ1114541: A Comparison of the Handwriting Abilities of Secondary Students with Visual Impairments and Those of Sighted Students

2006). Handwriting combines visual-motor Research Reports abilities, motor skills, and coordination (Kai- ser, Albaret, & Doudin, 2009). Factors that increase the likelihood of experiencing hand- AComparisonoftheHandwriting writing difficulties include decreased eye- AbilitiesofSecondaryStudents hand coordination, visual ability, kinesthesia, withVisualImpairmentsand sensory awareness, motor planning, and pos- ThoseofSightedStudents ture(Cornhill&Case-Smith,1996;Karlsdot- tir & Steffansson, 2002). Many of these fac- TalithaHarris-Brown,JanetRichmond, tors have the potential to be improved with SebastianDellaMaddalena,and the correct intervention. A permanent or AlintaJaworski progressive reduction in vision due to vi- V isualimpairmentaffectsapproximately1.8 sual impairment, however, cannot be easily million people in Australia (Resnikoff et al., corrected. 2004) and is defined as a visual acuity or Theonlyexistingliteratureontheeffectof equivalent field loss of less than 6/18 (20/60) visual impairment on handwriting indicates by the International Statistical Classification that impairment of specific ocular structures of Diseases and Related Health Problems or eye conditions can result in specific hand- (WorldHealthOrganization,2010).Avariety writing difficulties (Arter, McCall, & Bow- of conditions can cause visual impairment, yer, 1996). For example, for a person with a including cataracts, glaucoma, macular de- narrow visual field, the corresponding inabil- generation, diabetic retinopathy, trachoma ity to see an entire word can make copying (Bourne et al., 2013), retinitis pigmentosa, tasks arduous; and conditions that cause optic atrophy, albinism, and congenital nys- blurry vision may make it difficult to distin- tagmus (Haymes, Johnston, & Heyes, 2002). guish letter and word forms (Arter et al., Despite the large number of people with vi- 1996). A person with visual impairment, sual impairments in Australia, all Western therefore, is more likely to experience diffi- Australian secondary students are required to cultiessuchaslowerlegibilityofhandwriting complete their secondary exams using hand- and slower writing speed (Uysal & Aki, writing,unlesstheyqualifyforspecialprovi- 2012); and difficulties with forming letters sions. Students with visual impairments do andmaintainingevenspacing,size,slant,and not necessarily qualify for special provisions alignmentofletters(Bonney,1992;Yalo,In- onthebasisoftheirvisualimpairmentalone. doshi, & Agak, 2012). It is hypothesized that students with visual Keyboarding as an alternative method of impairments in Western Australia experience communication (Arter et al., 1996) does not more difficulties with handwriting than their assist students with visual impairments who fully sighted peers, which can potentially use handwriting for the completion of school limit their optimal performance. assignments. Assistive technology can play Despite advances in technology and com- an important role in helping students with puter use within schools, a considerable por- visual impairments with their education, but tion of the school day in Australia is com- many students feel it does not replace the prised of handwriting tasks (Graham et al., need for a method to produce handwritten 2008; Marr, Cermak, Cohn, & Henderson, assignments. For example, secondary school 2003; Ziviani & Watson-Will, 1998). For in- students with visual impairments state that dividuals with visual impairments, the use of althoughtechnologyisuseful,theyenjoyhav- handwriting can be challenging (Markowitz, ing the ability to choose between technology 402 JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved and writing (D’Andrea, 2012). Furthermore, & Santos, 2001; Rodger, Brown, & Brown, technology may stop working or be unavail- 2005). To date, there is only one study that able during servicing and can be expensive indicates that the assessment of visual-motor (D’Andrea, 2012). And one student with vi- integration abilities is also positively corre- sual impairment stated that having the option lated to handwriting ability in people with of writing enabled her to be more equal to visual impairments (Uysal & Aki, 2012). people around her, as it enabled her to write The primary objective of the study pre- down phone numbers and quick messages sented here is to determine if there is a sta- without having to start up a computer tistically significant difference in the hand- (D’Andrea,2012).Handwritinghasalsobeen writing performance of people with visual shown to be superior in contributing to stu- impairments compared to that of sighted indi- dents’understandingofwhattheyarewriting viduals. As a secondary objective, this study to typing out the same content (Mueller & makes an effort to determine if there is evi- Oppenheimer, 2014). For these reasons, dence supporting the use of The Beery- handwriting remains an important skill for Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual- students with visual impairments despite the MotorIntegration(Beery&Beery,2010).As availability of assistive technology. an indicator of handwriting performance in Handwriting is a mode of self-expression individuals with visual impairments. and a subtle representation of a person’s in- dividuality that is particularly important for METHODS academic performance (Arter et al., 1996; The current study followed a matched-pair Tseng&Chow,2000).Difficultieswithhand- design. Participants were separated into two writing can negatively affect academic per- groups:thosewithandwithoutvisualimpair- formance,andresearchhasshowedthatmore ments. The groups were matched for age time is required to complete handwritten as- (within 6 months) and gender. Socioeco- signments (Connelly, Dockrell, & Barnett, nomic status was recorded, but matching for 2005;Grahametal.,2008;Tseng&Cermak, socioeconomic status was not practical be- 1993) and that poorly handwritten exams are causeofsamplerestrictions.Theconfounding marked lower than the same content pre- factors of motor skills, cognitive skills, self- sented in legible handwriting (Connelly, esteem, and method of handwriting used and Campbell, MacLean, & Barnes, 2006; Ham- taught could not be practically matched be- merschmidt & Sudsawad, 2004; Sudsawad, tween groups, but were considered when the Trombly, Henderson, & Tickle-Degnen, results were analyzed. 2001).Inaddition,difficultieswithhandwrit- ing can have a detrimental effect on self- Participants esteem, personal relationships, and students’ For the participants with visual impair- perceptions of themselves (Goyen & Duff, ments, data on visual acuity, near vision, 2005; Graham & Weintraub, 1996). and conditions causing visual impairment For students who are sighted, handwriting were obtained with parental permission difficulties can be detected through the as- from records at VisAbility, the main sup- sessment of visual-motor integration abilities portorganizationforpeoplewithvisualim- (Klein, Guiltner, Sollereder, & Cui, 2011). pairments in Perth, Western Australia. For Visual-motor integration ability refers to the participants who are sighted, visual acuity degreetowhichvisualperception(processing and near vision were measured on the date of visual information) and fine-motor move- of assessment using a standard eye chart ments are coordinated (Gabbard, Conclaves, test and a near vision assessment provided 403 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 Table1 Demographicsanddescriptivestatisticsofparticipants. Participantswithvision impairment Participantswithsight Variable Males Females Males Females Number 12 9 12 9 Averageageinyears(mean) 14.63 15.10 14.56 15.29 Handdominancedistribution* L:3 R:7 L:0 R:9 L:1 R:11 L:1 R:8 Visualefficiencyrangescores 5–68% 8–68% 91–100% 96–100% Averagevisualefficiencyscores(mean) 32.03% 33.15% 97.27% 98.89% Rangenearvisionscores 4–32 3–16 4–6 4–10 Averagenearvisionscores(mean) 17.5 8.78 5 6 Socioeconomicstatus(combined parentalincome)* (cid:2)$60,000 1 0 2 0 $60,000–$100,000 3 4 0 2 (cid:3)$100,000 2 2 9 6 Typeofvisionimpairment Congenitalnystagmusoralbinism 6 2 Retinaldystrophies 3 3 Opticnerveconditions 1 2 Other 2 2 *Missingdataaccountsforinconsistenciesinnumericalvalues. byVisAbility.Visualacuityscoreswerethen writing as their primary mode of completing convertedintovisualefficiencyscoresforanal- schoolwork.Studentswereexcludediftheyhad ysis (Snell-Sterling Visual-Efficiency Scale, multiple disabilities, had vision greater than n.d.). See Table 1 for information about the 6/18 (20/60) or equivalent field loss, were not participants. involvedinmainstreameducation,orwereona Research group (VI). Participants with vi- modified program. sual impairments were recruited through Normative data group (control). Sighted convenience sampling methods from a participantswererecruitedusingconvenience previous study conducted by VisAbility and “snowball” sampling methods through in Perth, Western Australia (VisAbility, privatecontacts.Thesamplesizeforthenor- 2014).Asamplesizeof21participantswas mativegroupwasmatchedtotheparticipants estimated to be suitable for detecting with visual impairments in the research group. changes in the outcome measures. Students Sighted participants were recruited in accor- with visual impairments were invited dance with the following inclusion criteria: to participate in the study if they met matched the age and gender of one included the following criteria: diagnosis of visual participant with visual impairment and had impairment determined by an optometrist corrected or uncorrected vision greater than or ophthalmologist, aged between 12 and 6/18 (20/60) acuity or equivalent field. Stu- 18 years old, and use of handwriting dentswereexcludedfromthesightedgroupif as the main mode of completing school they had visual impairments, diagnosed mo- work. Students whowerelegallyblind(visual tor or intellectual disabilities, or were en- acuity of worse than 6/60 [20/200]) were con- gagedinhome-schoolingormodified-school- sideredforthisstudy,providedtheyusedhand- ing programs. 404 JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved Datacollectionandmeasures to17years,andnostudentswithvisualimpair- During all assessment sessions, participants ments were included within this group. with visual impairments were permitted the Beery VMI. Beery VMI is a standardized assessmentusedextensivelyinpediatricprac- use of their preexisting assistive equipment, tice to assess the visual-motor integration such as slant boards, modified lined paper, abilities of children aged 2 to 18 years and optical devices such as dome magnifiers. in which participants to copy shapes of pro- Assessment sessions were conducted over a gressing difficulty (Beery & Beery, 2010). 4-weekperiodandorganizedatlocationsthat Shapes begin with simple figures such as cir- allowed privacy and during times that were cles and rectangles and progress to layered convenient for participants. The lengths of figures in which shapes overlap. Reliability sessions varied between 30 and 60 minutes and validity testing of Beery VMI has been and were conducted by the primary investi- conducted by the authors of the assessment gatorortherapistsfromVisAbility.Datacol- and by external parties based on an Austra- lection was conducted through the following lian population (Brown & Hockey, 2013). modes: Detailed Assessment of Speed of Overall, the high scores for interscorer reli- Handwriting (DASH) and Beery VMI. ability(0.93–0.97)andtestsforcontent,con- DASH. Participants completed all subtests current,constructive,predicative,andcontrol of the DASH assessment (Barnett, Hender- forbiassupportitseffectivenessinmeasuring son, Scheib, & Schulz, 2007) in their natural visual-motor integration abilities. However, handwriting,resultinginamixofmanuscript tests for the reliability and validity of this and cursive writing. Four subtests measured instrumenthavenotbeenconductedwithpeo- handwritingunderdifferentconditions:copy- ple with visual impairments. The test’s esti- ingasetsentencefor2minutesintheirneat- mated completion time is 5 to 15 minutes. estandthentheirfastesthandwriting,writing Onthestandardassessmentform,participants the alphabet repeatedly for 1 minute in their copied shapes using the writing utensil they typicalhandwritingmode,andcompleting10 used during their daily school activities. minutes of uninterrupted handwriting on a The size of the shapes to be copied was not topic of their choosing in their daily hand- changed for the participants with vision im- writingstyle.Eachsubtestwasscoredforper- pairments as it was deemed by professionals formance recorded as the number of legible in the field of visual impairment to be a suit- wordscompletedperminute(wpm).Thecom- able size for participants to view. binednumberoflegiblewordscompletedper minute was then standardized to provide an Observations overall score for handwriting performance. During all assessment sessions, the adminis- Inaddition,thefifthsubtesthandwritingsam- trator was required to record observations on pleprovidedapercentagescoreforlegibility. a provided template. These observations in- DASH has been standardized for subjects cluded descriptive data and recorded fac- aged9to17yearsandtakesapproximately30 torssuchashandposition,specialequipment minutes to complete (Barnett et al., 2007). Re- used, occurrence of pain, and posture, as liabilitytestingoftheDASHrevealedhighlev- wellasgeneralobservationsregardingperfor- els of inter-rater agreement (reliability coeffi- mance. cients ranging from 0.853 to 0.999 across the subtests). These coefficients were estab- Parentquestionnaire lished based on a population of American stu- The written questionnaire for parents was dentswithoutdisabilities,ranginginagefrom9 providedtoparentsatthestartofthestudyprior 405 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 to the children participating in handwriting ble words written per minute. A Mann- assessment sessions and asked for information WhitneyU-testindicatedthatthehandwriting on the percentage of homework completed us- performance of participants with visual im- inghandwriting,whetherstudentswerereceiv- pairmentswasstatisticallysignificantlylower ing or had received handwriting interventions, than that of the sighted participants (U (cid:4) currentallowancespermittedbyschools,socio- 393.50,p(cid:2).00,meanVI(cid:4)29.74,meanC(cid:4) economic status in regards to income and par- 13.26). Handwriting performance was not ents’highesteducationlevel,andgeneralcom- significantly different between genders, both ments regarding the children’s handwriting. among people with visual impairments (U (cid:4) This information was used to account for con- 68.50,p(cid:4).3,meanmalesVI(cid:4)12.61,mean founding variables and considered in the anal- females VI (cid:4) 9.79) and people who are ysis of the results. sighted(U(cid:4)43.50,p(cid:4).46,meanmalesC(cid:4) 9.83, mean females C (cid:4) 11.88). PROCEDUREANDDATAMANAGEMENT Handwriting legibility. The free-writing EthicalapprovalwasobtainedfromtheEdith subtest of the DASH assessment also pro- Cowan University Human Research Ethics vided a score for legibility as a percentage. Committee. Consent and assent were ob- Legibility was determined by dividing the tained from each participant prior to partici- number of legibly written words by the total pation, and a student was free to withdraw number of words written. Analysis of all from the study at any stage. All data were the groups revealed no significant difference stored securely and confidentially and partic- between the legibility of the writing of high ipants were provided with a summary of re- school students with visual impairments sults reported at the completion of the study. and those who are sighted (U (cid:4) 253.00, p (cid:4) .63, mean VI (cid:4) 23.05, mean C (cid:4) 19.95), DATAANALYSIS andnodifferencebetweenmalesandfemales Data were analyzed and coded in PASW-22 among participants with visual impairments SPSS(IBM,2013)forstatisticalanalysis.The (U(cid:4)71.50,p(cid:4).22,meanmalesVI(cid:4)12.94, study aims were met through descriptive sta- mean females VI (cid:4) 9.54) or participants tistics, with measures of central tendency who are sighted (U (cid:4) 75.00, p (cid:4) .15, mean usedtodeterminethedistributionofthedata. males C (cid:4) 13.33, mean females C (cid:4) 9.25). Thesetestsfornormalityresultedinnonpara- metric tests used for statistical analysis. Raw Visual-MotorIntegrationability assessmentscoreswereconvertedtostandard (BeeryVMIscores) scores for analysis. All tests for statistical Results from the Beery VMI indicated that significancewereconductedusingtheMann- the participants with visual impairments had Whitney U-test, and all correlational tests statistically significantly lower visual-motor were conducted using Spearman’s Rho coef- integration abilities than the sighted partici- ficient. Statistical significance was set at p (cid:4) pants (U (cid:4) 426.00, p (cid:2) .00, mean VI (cid:4) .05. The clinical meaning of differences 31.29,meanC(cid:4)11.71).Comparingindivid- among variables was also considered. uals from the same group indicated no statis- tically significant difference between males RESULTS andfemalesinregardtovisual-motorintegra- DASH tion abilities for both participants with visual Handwriting performance. The score from impairments (U (cid:4) 60.00, p (cid:4) .70, mean DASH was used to record handwriting per- malesVI(cid:4)11.67,meanfemalesVI(cid:4)10.50) formance in reference to the number of legi- and those with no visual impairments (U (cid:4) 406 JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved 55.00, p (cid:4) 1.00, mean males C (cid:4) 11.11, In one case, a participant with visual im- mean females C (cid:4) 10.92). pairment produced words that were indis- tinguishableduetothewidespacesbetween Correlationalanalysis letters. Nosignificantcorrelationwasfoundbetween Patterns were recorded to determine theBeeryVMIandtheDASHscoresineither whether the type of visual impairment pro- participants with (p (cid:4) .14, r (cid:4) 0.34) and duced specific effects on handwriting. The without visual impairments (p (cid:4) 0.35, r (cid:4) group of participants with congenital nystag- 0.21). A bivariate Spearman’s Rho correla- musoralbinismhadthelowestaveragescore tion coefficient test revealed no statistically (71.5) in the DASH assessment, whereas the significant correlation between the partici- group with retinal dystrophies had the lowest pants with visual impairments’ near vision average Beery VMI scores (72.67). Differ- ability and performance on the DASH (p (cid:4) ences in legibility were negligible between (cid:5).22,r(cid:4)0.40)ortheBeeryVMI(p(cid:4)(cid:5).11, the groups of participants with visual impair- r(cid:4)0.68).Forparticipantswithvisualimpair- ments. Participants with retinal dystrophies ments, visual efficiency had a weak correla- had the highest occurrence of insufficient tion to performance on the DASH (p (cid:4) .17, spacing between letters, resulting in writing r (cid:4) 0.31) and a moderate correlation to per- being very close together. Although other formance on the Beery VMI (p (cid:4) .10, r (cid:4) participants displayed insufficient spacing, 0.09). For the sighted participants, visual ef- 62.5% were represented in the group with ficiency was positively correlated to hand- retinal dystrophies. For a majority of partici- writing performance (DASH) (p (cid:4) .00, r (cid:4) pants within this group, there was also a 0.75)butnottovisual-motorintegrationabil- higher occurrence of large gaps on the right ity (Beery VMI) (p (cid:4) 70, r (cid:4) 0.09). side of the paper, in which no writing was present. A notable gap was considered larger Observationsandparticipantreporting than 15 mm (0.59 inches). Only participants Observations recorded common patterns in with retinal dystrophies produced gaps rang- the outcome measures completed by partici- ing from 18 mm to 50 mm (0.70 to 1.97 pants with visual impairments that were not inches). This neglect on the side of the paper present in the participants who are sighted. was also present in one of the corresponding There was a higher number of participants Beery VMI assessments in which drawings with visual impairments (43%) compared to were consistently positioned to the left of the participants who are sighted (4%) whose page. drawings in the Beery VMI were of a notice- ably larger size. In the handwriting samples, DISCUSSION participants with visual impairments were The results from this study support the hy- more often unable to write within the con- pothesis that participants with visual impair- straints of the lines provided (19% of partic- ments have lower handwriting performance ipants with visual impairments, compared to than participants who are sighted. The hand- 4% of sighted participants). In addition, six writing assessment in this study measured participants with visual impairments were performance using the number of legible noted to have large writing—that is, writing wordscompletedperminute;resultstherefore thatisgreaterthan1/3ofthespacebetween show that the representative sample of stu- the lines—and four participants had small dents with visual impairments in Western writing—where the body of the letter was Australia complete fewer legible words per lessthan1/3ofthespacebetweenthelines. minute. 407 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 In addition to overall lower handwriting of the population of people with visual im- performance, participants with visual impair- pairments. ments had higher occurrences of large-scale The secondary objective of this study was drawingsandwriting,alargerdiversityinthe to investigate whether the assessment of sizeofwriting,aninabilitytowritewithinthe visual-motor integration abilities can be in- constraintsofthelines,andirregularitymain- dicative of handwriting performance, a con- taining spacing between letters and words. ceptsupportedinpeoplewhoaresighted(Du- These conclusions are consistent in research iser, ven der Kamp, Ledebt & Savelsbergh, and support the findings made in Uysal and 2014; Klein et al., 2011). This study has in- Aki’s 2012 study that compared Turkish stu- dicated that people with visual impairments dentswithvisualimpairmentstotheirsighted had lower visual-motor integration abilities peers. The observations made in this study than people who are sighted, a result sup- supportsimilarobservationsmadeinprevious portedinUysal&Aki’s2012studyonTurk- studies(Yaloetal.,2012).However,astatis- ishstudentswithvisualimpairments.Despite ticalstudythatsupportstheseobservationsas this result, no correlation was found between common in people with visual impairments performance on the Beery VMI and DASH. has yet to be conducted. Therefore, lower visual-motor integration We found that handwriting legibility abilities do not necessarily result in a corre- among participants with visual impairments sponding difficulty in handwriting perfor- was not significantly different from sighted mance, which is a contrasting finding to the participants. These results are in contrast to study in which visual-motor integration abil- the only other study conducted comparing itieswerepositivelycorrelatedtohandwriting handwriting between people with visual im- ability in people with visual impairments pairmentsandthosewhoaresighted(Uysal& (Uysal&Aki,2012).Thedifferenceinhand- Aki, 2012). Differences in the handwriting writing assessments and the population from assessments used may account for the differ- which the participants were recruited may ences,however,sinceonlytwostudiesinves- account for the differences. tigatedthelegibilityofhandwritingproduced Specific visual characteristics such as near by people with visual impairments compared vision and visual efficiency did not show any to that of people who are sighted, conclusive correlationtohandwriting.Nearvisionwasre- statements will not be made. corded in font size, and visual efficiency was Studies on participants who are sighted recorded as a percentage of efficient vision. support the theory that females have more The small sample size limits the conclu- legible handwriting than males (Berninger, siveness of results, and therefore further Nielson,Abbott,Wijsman,&Raskind,2008). investigation should be undertaken in order Despite the results of this current study indi- to determine a convincing outcome regard- catingnostatisticallysignificantdifferencein ing the effect of visual characteristics such thelegibilityofhandwritingbetweenfemales as near vision and visual efficiency on and males in either group of participants, there handwriting. was still a recorded difference (males with vi- Unique occurrences in the handwriting of sual impairments 93.29%, males who are participants with retinal dystrophies were ob- sighted 94.16%, females with visual impair- served. These included producing letters close ments 95.40%, females who are sighted togetherandalackofuseofspaceontheright 96.77%). Further research is required to sideofthepage.Sincethisgroupofparticipants determine whether or not this difference includesthosewithretinitispigmentosa,which wastheresultofchanceorisrepresentative canresultinvisualfieldloss,theseobservations 408 JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved are consistent with expectations. However, ing, irregular spacing, and poor sentence since these observations were not statistically alignment for all participants with visual im- supportedandcomefromasmallrangeofpar- pairments regardless of diagnosis. A consis- ticipants, they can be considered areas for de- tentoutcomewasthatparticipantswithvisual velopment in future research. impairmentscompletedfewerwordspermin- ute than their sighted peers. Limitations A potential limitation of this study is the gen- IMPLICATIONSFORCLINICALPRACTICE eralizabilityofresultstothewiderpopulationof The implications of this study and its results people with visual impairments because of the can be used to inform parents, teachers, and small sample size and the fact that all partici- therapists of students with visual impair- pants with visual impairments were recruited ments. For parents and teachers of students through VisAbility. Despite these limitations, withdiagnosedvisualimpairments,theobser- the support of other studies investigating the vationsandresultsofthisstudycanbeusedto handwriting difficulties experienced by people determine if the student is experiencing dif- with visual impairments lends strength to the ficultiesandmayrequireintervention,suchas results.Outcomesinthisstudymaybeaffected anincreasedtimeallowancewhencompleting bytheincreaseintheuseoftechnologywithin handwriting tasks. society and schools, reflecting a decrease in Therapists are frequently the professionals handwriting use and abilities. whomakerecommendationsandliaisewithac- ademic staff members regarding allowances to CONCLUSIONS facilitatethebestperformanceforstudentswith The expectation of secondary students with vi- visual impairments. The results of this study sualimpairmentsinAustraliatocompletetheir willstrengthenobservationsmadeduringhand- secondary exams under the same conditions as writing assessments and help support the need their sighted peers does not represent an equal for recommendations made by educators or opportunity. Results indicate that Western therapists. For many therapists, a referral for Australianstudentswithvisualimpairments handwriting includes an assessment of visual- may complete fewer words per minute and motor integration abilities. However, results write at slower speeds than their peers who from this study indicate that assessment of are sighted when using handwriting. visual-motorintegrationabilitiesisnoteffective In order to create equality for people with as handwriting assessments for people with vi- visual impairments, an assessment of hand- sual impairments. The assessment of visual- writing ability is required to determine the motor integration abilities can still be used, degree to which visual impairment affects however, in conjunction with a formal hand- their ability to produce handwriting equal to writing assessment and considered in light of their sighted peers. Evidence shows that a the above finding. The generalizability of this specificvisualimpairmentdiagnosisdoesnot finding is limited to the population represented directly result in a type of handwriting diffi- in this study (secondary students with visual culty. Despite the high occurrence of de- impairments from Western Australia), but still creased space between letters and potential requires further investigation to determine neglect on the right-hand side of the page whether it is a definitive conclusion. Further- observed in participants with retinal dystro- more, the handwriting of people with visual phies, many difficulties in handwriting were impairmentsisanareainneedoffurtherinves- observed in participants with visual impair- tigation.Futurestudiesareneededtodetermine ments.Thesedifficultiesincludedlargerwrit- whethertheeyestructureaffectedincreasesthe 409 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 occurrenceofspecifictypesofdifficultieswith Connelly, V., Dockrell, J. E., & Barnett, J. handwriting. (2005). The slow handwriting of under- Despite the availability of assistive tech- graduatestudentsconstrainsoverallperfor- nology for students with visual impair- mance in exam essays. Educational Psy- chology, 25(1), 99–107. ments, the learning of handwriting enables Cornhill,H.,&Case-Smith,J.(1996).Factors freedom of choice and decreases reliability that relate to good and poor handwriting. on expensive technology that may not al- American Journal of Occupational Ther- ways be accessible. apy, 50(9), 732–739. D’Andrea,F.M.(2012).Preferencesandprac- REFERENCES ticesamongstudentswhoreadbrailleanduse Arter, C., McCall, S., & Bowyer, T. (1996). assistive technology. Journal of Visual Im- Handwriting and children with visual im- pairment & Blindness, 106(10), 585–596 pairments. British Journal of Special Edu- Duiser,I.H.F.,venderKamp,J.,Ledebt,A., cation, 23(1), 25–28. & Savelsburgh, G. J. P. (2014). Relation- Barnett, A., Henderson, S. E., Scheib, B., & ship between the quality of children’s Schulz, J. (2007). Detailed Assessment of handwriting and the Beery-Buktenica De- Speed of Handwriting (DASH). London: velopmental Test of Visuomotor Integra- Pearson Assessment. tionafteroneyearofwritingtuition.Austra- Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2010). The lian Occupational Therapy Journal, 61(2), Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of 76–82. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12064 Visual-Motor Integration (6th Ed.). San Gabbard,C.,Conclaves,V.M.G.,&Santos, Antonio, TX: Pearson Education. D. C. C. (2001). Visual-motor integration Berninger, V. W., Nielson, K. H., Abbott, problemsinlowbirthweightinfants.Jour- R.D.,Wijsman,E.,&Raskind,W.(2008). nal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Set- Gender differences in severity of writing tings, 8(3), 199–204. and reading disabilities. Journal of School Goyen, T., & Duff, S. (2005). Discriminant Psychology, 46(2), 151–172. validity of the Developmental Test of Bonney, M. A. (1992). Understanding and Visual-MotorIntegrationinrelationtochil- assessing handwriting difficulty: Perspec- drenwithhandwritingdysfunction.Austra- tivesfromtheliterature.TheAmericanOc- lian Journal of Occupational Therapy, cupational Therapy Journal, 39(3). 715. 52(2),109–115.doi:10.1111/j.1440-1630. Bourne, R. R. A., Stevens, G. A., White, 2005.00488.x R. A., Smith, J. L., Flaxman, S. R, Price, Graham, S., Harris, K. R., Mason, L., Fink- H.,...Naidoo,K.(2013).Causesofvision Chorzempa, B., Moran, S., & Saddler, B. loss worldwide, 1990-2010: A systematic (2008). How do primary grade teachers analysis. The Lancet Global Health, 1(6), teach handwriting? A national survey. 339–349. Reading and Writing, 21(1), 49–69. Brown, T., & Hockey, S. C. (2013). The Graham,S.,&Weintraub,N.(1996).Areview validity and reliability of Developmental of handwriting research: Progress and pros- Test of Visual Perception-2nd edition pectsfrom1980–1994.EducationalPsychol- (DTVP-2). Physical & Occupational ogy Review [H.W. Wilson – EDUC], 8(1), Therapy n Pediatrics, 33(4), 426–439. 787. doi:10.1007/BF01761831 doi: 10.3109/01942638.2012.757573 Hammerschmidt, L. S., & Sudsawad, P. Connelly, V., Campbell, S., MacLean, M., & (2004).Teachers’surveyonproblemswith Barnes, J. (2006). Contribution of lower handwriting: Referral, evaluation and out- order skills to the written composition of comes. American Journal of Occupational collegestudentswithandwithoutdyslexia. Therapy, 58(2), 185–192. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29(1), Haymes, S. A., Johnston, A. W., & Heyes, 175–196. A. D. (2002). Relationship between vision 410 JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved impairmentandabilitytoperformactivities Snell-Sterling Visual Efficiency Scale. (n.d.). of daily living. Ophthalmic & Physiologi- Millodot: Dictionary of Optometry and Vi- cal Optics: The Journal of the British Col- sual Science (7th Ed.). (2009). Retrieved lege of Ophthalmic Opticians (Optome- from http://medical-dictionary.thefree trists), 22(2), 79–91. dictionary.com/Snell-Sterling(cid:6)visual(cid:6) IBM.(2013).IBMSPSSStatistics[Computer efficiency(cid:6)scale Software]. Retrieved from http://www-01. Sudsawad, P., Trombly, A. C., Henderson, ibm.com/software/au/analytics/spss/ A., & Tickle-Degnen, L. (2001). The rela- products/statistics tionship between the evaluation tool of Kaiser,M.L.,Albaret,J.M.,&Doudin,P.A. children’s handwriting and teachers’ per- (2009). Relationship between visual-motor ceptions of handwriting legibility. Ameri- integration, eye-hand coordination, and can Journal of Occupational Therapy, qualityofhandwriting.JournalofOccupa- 55(5), 518–523. tionalTherapy,Schools,&EarlyInterven- Tseng, M. H., & Cermak, S. A. (1993). The tion, 2(2), 87–95. influence of ergonomic factors and Karlsdottir, R., & Steffansson, T. (2002). perceptual-motorabilitiesonhandwriting Problems in developing functional hand- performance. The American Journal of writing. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Occupational Therapy, 47(10), 919–926. 94(2), 623–662. Tseng, M. H., & Chow, S. M. K. (2000). Klein, S., Guiltner, V., Sollereder, P., & Cui, Perceptual-motor function of school-age Y. (2011). Relationships between fine- children with slow handwriting speed. motor, visual-motor, and visual perception American Journal of Occupational Ther- scores and handwriting legibility and apy, 54(1), 84–88. speed. Physical & Occupational Therapy Uysal, S. A., & Aki, E. (2012). Relationship in Pediatrics, 31(1), 103–114. between writing skills and visual-motor Markowitz, M. (2006). Occupational therapy control in low-vision students. Perceptual intervention in low vision rehabilitation. and Motor Skills, 115(1), 111–119. Canadian Journal of Ophthalmology, VisAbility. (2014). Home page. Retrieved 41(3), 340–347. from http://www.visability.com.au Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S., & Hen- World Health Organization. (2010). Interna- derson, A. (2003). Fine motor activities in tionalstatisticalclassificationofdiseasesand head start and kindergarten classrooms. related health problems, 10th revision. Ge- The American Journal of Occupational neva: WHO. Retrieved from http://www. Therapy, 57(5), 550–557. who.int/bulletin/volumes/90/10/12- Mueller,P.A.,&Oppenheimer,D.M.(2014). 104034/en The pen is mightier than the keyboard: Ad- Yalo, J. A., Indoshi, F. C., & Agak, J. O. vantagesoflonghandoverlaptopnotetaking. (2012). Challenges and strategies of work- Psychological Science, 25(6), 1159–1168. ing with learners with low vision: Implica- doi:10.1177/0956797614524581 tions for teacher training. Educational Re- Resnikoff,S.,Pascolini,D.,Etya’ale,D.,Ko- search and Reviews, 7(10), 238–243. cur, I., Pararajasegaram, R., Pokharel, Ziviani, J., & Watson-Will, A. (1998). Writ- G.P.,&Mariotti,S.P.(2004).Globaldata ing speed and legibility of 7-14 year old onvisualimpairmentintheyear2002.Bul- school students using modern cursive letin of the World Health Organization, script. Australian Occupational Therapy 82(11), 844–851. Journal, 45(2), 59–64. Rodger, S., Brown, G. T., & Brown, A. (2005). Profile of paediatric occupational Talitha Harris-Brown, B.Sc. Hons., occupa- therapy practice in Australia. Australian tional therapist, Edith Cowan University, P.O. Occupational Therapy Journal, 52(4), Box 23, Joondalup Dc, WA, 6919, Australia; 311–325. e-mail: (cid:2)[email protected](cid:3). 411 ©2015AFB,AllRightsReserved JournalofVisualImpairment&Blindness,September-October2015

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.