ebook img

ERIC ED612379: Building a Knowledge Base: Predicting Self-Derivation through Integration in 6- to 10-Year-Olds PDF

2018·0.64 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED612379: Building a Knowledge Base: Predicting Self-Derivation through Integration in 6- to 10-Year-Olds

JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Building a knowledge base: Predicting self-derivation through integration in 6- to 10-year-olds ⇑ Alena G. Esposito , Patricia J. Bauer DepartmentofPsychology,EmoryUniversity,Atlanta,GA30322,USA a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Articlehistory: Self-derivation of new factual knowledge through integration of Received16January2018 separateepisodesoflearningisonemeansbywhichchildrenbuild Revised10July2018 knowledge. Content generated in this manner becomes incorpo- Availableonline10August2018 ratedintotheknowledgebaseandisretainedovertime;successful self-derivationpredictsacademicachievement.Yetthecomponent Keywords: processes on which self-derivation through integration depends Self-derivation are as yet unknown. In parallel studies with 6- and 8-year-olds Semanticknowledge Integration (N=41; Experiment 1) and 8- and 10-year-olds (N=40; Verbalcomprehension Experiment2),wetestedanumberofpredictorsrelatedtoother Academicachievement productiveprocessesandlearning(reasoning,executivefunctions, Transfer verbal comprehension, and long-term retrieval). Across studies, withdifferentmethods,onlyverbalcomprehension,ameasureof accumulatedsemanticknowledge,accountedforuniquevariance in self-derivation through integration performance. The results indicate that self-derivation through integration of separate epi- sodes relates to accumulation of knowledge and the ability to recruit the knowledge in the service of specific task demands. Implicationsforcognitivetrainingandtransferarediscussed. (cid:1)2018ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved. ⇑ Correspondingauthor. E-mailaddress:[email protected](A.G.Esposito). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.011 0022-0965/(cid:1)2018ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved. 56 A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 Introduction Constructingaknowledgebaseisoneofthemostimportanttasksindevelopment.Knowledgelays afoundationforlanguageandthought,andknowledgesupportstheacquisitionofmoreknowledge (Goswami,2011).Childrenbuildknowledgethroughdirecttuitionandexperiences.Becauselearning experiences are distributed over time and modalities, for knowledge to accumulate, separate but relatedepisodesmustbeintegratedwithoneanother.Importantly,elaboratingtheknowledgebase alsodependsonproductiveprocessesthatpermitself-derivationofnewsemanticcontentbasedon integrated representations (see Bauer, 2009, 2012). Research has revealed that new facts resulting from integration and self-derivation become incorporated into the knowledge base (Bauer & Jackson,2015)andareretainedovertime(Varga&Bauer,2013;Varga,Stewart,&Bauer,2016).Crit- ically, performance on tasks that test self-derivation of new factual knowledge through integration predicts academic achievement in both reading and math (Esposito & Bauer, 2017). Currently unknownarethecomponentcognitiveabilitiesonwhichself-derivationthroughintegrationdepends. Inthe current research,in separatestudies with6- and 8-year-olds(Experiment1) and 8- and 10- year-olds(Experiment2),wetestedcandidatepredictorssuggestedbyresearchonotherproductive processes,specificallyreasoning,executivefunctions,verbalcomprehension,andlong-termretrieval. Productiveprocessesandcomponentcognitiveabilities Productive processes such as analogy, deduction, and induction allow learning to proceed effi- cientlybecauselearnerscanextendbeyondinformationexplicitlyprovidedinalessontoderivenovel understandings(e.g.,Goswami,1992,2011;Perret,2015).Productiveprocesseshavebeenobserved across the lifespan (see Mandler & McDonough, 1996, for examples even during infancy) and are assumed to be a major mechanism of cognitive development (Bauer & Varga, 2017; Bauer, 2012; Brown,Bransford,Ferrara,&Campione,1982;Goswami,2011;Siegler,1989). Thecurrentliteratureonthecomponentcognitiveabilitiesonwhichproductiveprocessesdepend hasexaminedonlyalimitednumberofcandidates,withparticularemphasisoninhibitorycontroland workingmemory.Thenumberofdirecttestsofthesecandidateprocessesissmall,yetthereiscon- sensusthatbothinhibitorycontrolandworkingmemoryplayarole(seeGoswami,2011,fordiscus- sion). In terms of direct tests, Moutier (2000b) studied inhibitory control, training in logic, and deductivereasoningin10-and11-year-oldchildren.Inoneconditionchildrenweretrainedtoinhibit acommonperceptualmatchingerror,andinanotherconditiontheyreceivedlogicaltraining.Atpostt- est,onlythechildrenwiththetrainingininhibitorycontrolshowedimprovementinthedeductive reasoningtask.Inarelatedstudy,Moutier(2000a)tested10-and11-year-olds’deductivereasoning andperformanceonanumberofinhibitorycontroltasks(StroopTask,TrailMaking,andWisconsin CardSort).Higherperformanceonthedeductivereasoningtaskwasrelatedtohigherinhibitoryper- formance(seealsoMoutier,Plagne-Cayeux,Melot,andHoudé,2006,foradditional,indirectevidence ofrelationsbetweencognitiveinhibitionand8-to10-year-olds’performanceondeductivereasoning problems). Otherstudieshavenotmeasuredinhibitorycontroldirectlybuthaveimplicatedit,workingmem- ory, or both based on patterns of task performance. For example, Richland, Morrison, and Holyoak (2006)investigatedanalogicalproblemsolvinginchildrenaged3–14years.Theyfoundthatyounger childrenweremoresusceptibletothepresenceoffeature-leveldistractors,relativetoolderchildren, andhadlowerlevelsofperformance.Theyinterpretedthepatterntoindicatethatyoungerchildren did not yet have sufficient inhibitory control to ignore the distractors and maintain the task- relevantinformationinworkingmemory. Inadditiontoinhibitorycontrolandworkingmemory,accumulationanduseofpriorknowledge hasbeenimplicatedasafactoraccountingforage-relatedchangesininductiveanddeductivereason- ing (e.g., Goswami, 2011). Yet this conjecture has not been subjected to strong empirical testing becausetherearefewstudiesthathaveincludedameasureofverbalcomprehensionorothermea- sures of acquired knowledge among the candidate predictor variables. An exception is Fisher, Godwin,andMatlen(2015),inwhichmeasuresofverbalcomprehensionandreasoningwereexam- A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 57 inedalongwithworkingmemoryandinhibitorycontrolaspredictorsofinductivegeneralizationwith familiarcategories. Yetin thisstudytheverbalcomprehension andreasoningmeasureswerecom- binedintoageneralintelligencecomposite,therebyprecludingexaminationoftheiruniquecontribu- tionstoperformance. Anotherliteraturethatbearsonthequestionofrelationsbetweenaccumulationanduseofprior knowledgeandproductiveprocessesisresearchoninferencemakinginthecontextofreadingcom- prehension.Readingcomprehensionisstronglycorrelatedtoverbalcomprehension(e.g.,Riedel,2007; Yovanoff,Duesbery,Alonzo,&Tindal,2005),andbotharedescribedasmeasuresofsemanticknowl- edgeanditsrecruitmentanduse(e.g.,Akshoomoffetal.,2013;Schrank,2010).Consistentwithsug- gestionsof relationsbetweenprior knowledgeand successfulinference,severalstudies havefound correlationsbetweenreadingcomprehensionandinferencemakingwithinasingletextsuchasdraw- inganinferencefromtwoconsecutivesentencesinastory(e.g.,Cain&Oakhill,1999;Cain,Oakhill,& Bryant,2004;Oakhill,Cain,&Bryant,2003).Interestingly,inthisliteraturethequestioniswhether inferencemakingpredictsreadingcomprehension,notwhetherreadingcomprehension—asanindex ofaccumulationanduseofpriorknowledge—predictsinferentialability.Thisorientationisapparent inCainetal.(2004),forexample,whereitwasfoundthatbothworkingmemoryandinferencemaking predicteduniquevarianceinreadingcomprehensioninchildrenaged7–9years.Thisandothersuch studiessuggestthattherearerelationsbetweeninferencemakingandaccumulatedknowledge,yet they do not directly inform relations between acquired knowledge and inference as a productive process. In summary, productive processes such as induction and deduction are supported by inhibitory controlandworkingmemory.Readingcomprehensionisrelatedtothesecomponentcognitiveabili- tiesaswellasinferencemaking,suggestingthataccumulatedknowledgemayalsoberelatedtopro- ductive processes. Thus, both reasoning and verbal comprehension are indicated in productive processing but have not been directly tested, and it is not clear whether they both explain unique variance. Self-derivationthroughintegrationandcomponentcognitiveabilities Thespecificproductiveprocessthatisthesubjectofthecurrentresearchisself-derivationofnew factualknowledgethroughintegrationofseparateyetrelatedepisodesofnewlearning.Thisprocessis ofparticularsignificancebecauseitisaviablemodeloflearningasitoccursoutsidethecontrolofthe laboratory.Initialinvestigationsofthisspecificproductiveprocessusedastorypassageparadigmin whichchildrenwerepresentedwithtruebutpreviouslyunknownfacts(e.g.,dolphinsliveingroups calledpods)embeddedwithinrichlycontextualizedstorypassages.Afterabriefdelay,arelatedtrue butpreviouslyunknownfact(e.g.,dolphinstalkbyclickingandsqueaking)waspresentedviaasep- aratestorypassagethathadauniquestoryline.Afteranotherbriefdelay,childrenwereaskedques- tions (e.g., how do pods talk?), the answers to which could be generated by integrating the informationfromtheseparateepisodes(e.g.,Bauer&SanSouci,2010).Performanceoncontroltrials (inwhichonlyoneofthetwostoriesispresented)makesclearthatbothfactsarenecessarytoanswer thequestions(Bauer&Larkina,2017;Bauer&SanSouci,2010). Consistent with the suggestion that self-derivation through integration is importantto learning, new factual knowledge derived through this process is retained over time. For example, in Varga and Bauer (2013), 6-year-olds correctly produced the novel facts on 63% of open-ended trials and recalled60%ofthefacts1weeklater(seeVargaetal.,2016,andVarga&Bauer,2017,forsimilarfind- ings with 4-year-olds and college students, respectively). Moreover, performance on tests of self- derivationthroughintegrationrelatestoacademicachievementinbothreadingandmathasindexed by measures that determine grade promotion in kindergarten through Grade 3 (state standardized performanceevaluations,classroomgrades,andnationallynormedassessments).Thisresearchsug- geststhattheskillsandabilitiesonwhichself-derivationofnewfactualknowledgethroughintegra- tiondependssharevariancewiththosethatsupportacademicachievementbroadly. Especiallyinlightofevidenceofrelationsbetweenself-derivationthroughintegrationtaskperfor- manceandacademicachievement,itisimportanttodeterminetheskillsandabilitiesonwhichthe process depends. Like other productive processes such as analogy, deduction, and induction, self- 58 A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 derivationthroughintegrationmaydependongeneralcognitiveabilitiessuchasinhibitorycontrol and working memory (Goswami, 1992, 2011). These abilities tend to be interrelated (e.g., Fukuda, Vogel,Mayr,&Awh,2010;Nettelbeck&Burns,2010)andespeciallyduringearlychildhoodmayalso relatetoothergeneralcognitiveabilities,includingabstractreasoningandprocessingspeed(Mungas etal.,2013).Measuresoftheseabilitiesalsorelatetoacademicperformance(i.e.,highachieversshow highperformance;e.g.,Bhat,2016;Colom,Escorial,Shih,&Privado,2007;Floyd,Evans,&McGrew, 2003; Lynn, Meisenberg, Mikk, & Williams, 2007). As such, we would expect positive correlations betweenmeasuresoftheseabilitiesandself-derivationthroughintegration.Consistentwiththissug- gestion,inastudywith4-and6-year-olds,VargaandBauer(2014)foundthatmeasuresofreasoning andverbalcomprehensionbothwerecorrelatedwithself-derivationperformance.Yetonlytherea- soningtasksignificantlypredictedperformanceinaregressionmodel. Inadditiontogeneralcognitiveabilities,self-derivationofnewfactualknowledgethroughintegra- tionmayalsodependonaccumulatedsemanticknowledgeandthelong-termretrievalthereof.This possibilityissalientbecausethematerialoverwhichthecognitiveprocessesarecarriedoutisreal- worldsemanticknowledge.Thefactsthatsupportself-derivationarepreviouslyunknown,yetthey relatetoconceptsabout whichchildrenhavesomefamiliarity(e.g.,animals ingeneral, dolphinsin particular,howanimateobjectscommunicate).Assuch,long-termretrievalandsemanticknowledge could be expected to relate to self-derivation through integration of separate episodes. This logical possibility is supported by the reading comprehension literature that points to a relation between inferencemaking and knowledge (e.g., Cain & Oakhill, 1999). It is further supported by the finding of a correlation between reading comprehension and self-derivation of new factual knowledge throughintegrationin7-to11-year-olds(Bauer,Blue,Xu,andEsposito,2016,Experiment2).Thecor- relationwasobservedbothwhenthefactswerereadbythechildrenthemselvesandwhentheywere readbyanexperimenter.Giventhatreadingcomprehensionisstronglycorrelatedtoverbalcompre- hension and both are considered measures of semantic knowledge and the ability to recruit it, the relationisconsistentwithsuggestionsofrelationsbetweensemanticknowledgeandself-derivation throughintegration. Insummary,self-derivationofnewfactualknowledgethroughintegrationofseparateepisodesof newlearningisaparticularlypromisingareaofstudyduetotheecologicalvalidityofthestimuliand paradigmandtherelationwithacademicperformance.Yettodate,fewstudieshaveexaminedthe relation between self-derivation through integration and other cognitive abilities. The available researchisnotsufficienttodeterminewhatcognitiveabilitiesrelatetoself-derivationthroughinte- gration,althoughtheextantresearchofferssupportforgeneralcognitiveskillssuchasexecutivefunc- tionsandreasoningaswellasaccumulatedknowledgeandtheabilitytouseittoreason. Thecurrentresearch Inthecurrentresearch,weconductedtwoexperimentstotesttheskillsandabilitiesonwhichself- derivationthroughintegrationdepends.Weaddressedthequestioninchildrenacrosstheearlyschool years,whentheyareheavilyengagedinthedevelopmentaltaskofaccumulatingaknowledgebase.In Experiment1,weassessedself-derivationthroughintegrationin6-and8-year-oldsusingastorypas- sageparadigm(Bauer&Larkina,2017;Bauer&SanSouci,2010).InExperiment2,weassessedself- derivationthroughintegrationin8-and10-year-oldsusinganadaptationofthesingle-sentencepara- digmusedbyBauerandcolleagues(Baueretal.,2016;seealsoVarga&Bauer,2017,foruseofthe paradigm with adults). Thus, across the experiments, we examined self-derivation performance in childrenaged6–10years. Inbothexperiments,childrencompletedacognitivebatterythatassessedabstractreasoning,exec- utivefunctions(includinginhibitorycontrol),verbalcomprehension,andlong-termretrieval.Weused the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Skills (COG; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) to assess reasoning (Concept Formation), verbal comprehension (Verbal Comprehension), and long- term retrieval (Visual Auditory Learning). Given the specific hypotheses regarding inhibition, we added measures of inhibitory function to supplement the Woodcock–Johnson III COG battery. We expected positive associations between self-derivation through integration and reasoning, long- termmemoryretrieval,andexecutivefunctionsbecauseself-derivationthroughintegrationisapro- A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 59 ductiveprocessand,thus,logicallycouldbeexpectedtofollowthesamepatternofrelationasother such processes, including analogy, deduction, and induction (e.g., Moutier, 2000a; Richland et al., 2006). Based onpriorresearchonself-derivation throughmemoryintegrationspecifically(Varga & Bauer,2014),weexpectedrelationswithabstractreasoninginparticular.Wealsoexpectedpositive associations with semantic knowledge because the content over which the processes involved in self-derivationthroughintegrationoperateisreal-worldfactualknowledge.Previousworkhasshown a relation between reading comprehension and inference making (e.g., Cain & Oakhill, 1999) and betweenreading comprehension and self-derivation throughintegration (Baueret al., 2016). Given thatreadingcomprehensionisameasureofsemanticknowledge,itlogicallycouldbeexpectedthat self-derivationthroughintegrationwouldrelatetothedepthandbreadthofaccumulatedsemantic knowledgeandtheskillstorecruitittoreasonondeclarativetasks. Evenasweadvancethesepredictions,wenotethatthecognitivefunctionsunderstudyoverlap. Yetgiventhepsychometricstructureoftheinstrumentsweusedtomeasurethem,itisreasonable to anticipate that we could identify relatively unique contributions. Specifically, in factor analyses, the subtests of the Woodcock–Johnson III COG battery (Woodcock et al., 2001) load onto different clusters and, thus, are interpreted to measure separate constructs; Concept Formation loads to the Fluid Reasoning cluster (.81), Verbal Comprehension loads to Comprehension Knowledge (.95), and VisualAuditoryLearningloadstoLong-TermRetrieval(.71)(Taub&McGrew,2004).Therearenosig- nificant residual correlations between the measures. Although the measures of executive functions (includinginhibitoryfunction)arenot partoftheWoodcock–JohnsonIII COGbattery andhave not beensubjectedtofactoranalyseswiththeabovemeasures,similarmeasuresfromtheDelis–Kaplan ExecutiveFunctionSystemhavebeencomparedwiththeWoodcock–JohnsonIIICOGbatteryclusters, withresultsrevealingonlymoderatecorrelations,indicatingthatthesemeasuresalsorepresentasep- aratefactor(Floydetal.,2006).Thus,althoughwecannotaccomplishcompleteisolationofthediffer- ent cognitive components under study, we can identify relative unique contributions. The effort is important because understanding the factors that contribute to successful self-derivation through integrationcanguidethedevelopmentofinterventionsandeducationalmethodsaimedtofacilitate productiveknowledgegeneration. Experiment1 Method Participants Theparticipantswere20 6-year-olds(9girlsand11boys;M =6years7months,range=6;2– age 6;10 [years;months] and 21 8-year-olds (11 girls and 10 boys; M =8years 6months, range=8; age 2–8;10). Children participated in two sessions approximately 1week apart, each lasting approxi- mately1h.ThechildrenwererecruitedfromavolunteerpoolconsistingofEnglish-speakingfamilies fromalargemetropolitanareainthesoutheasternUnitedStateswhohadexpressedinterestinpar- ticipating in child development research. Based on parental report, the sample was 24.4% African American, 7.3% biracial or multiracial, and 68.3% Caucasian, with 2% of the sample self-identifying asHispanic.Intermsofeducation,5%ofreportingparentsindicatedthattheprimarycaregiverhad somecollege,2.5%reportedhavinganassociate’sdegree,40%reportedhavingacollegedegree,and 52.5% reported having education beyond a bachelor’s degree. One family did not report education level. Parents provided informed written consent and children gave verbal assent at the beginning ofSession1.Forthisandthesubsequentexperiment,auniversityinstitutionalreviewboardapproved theprotocolandprocedures.Attheendofeachsession,childrenreceivedanage-appropriatetoyto acknowledgetheirparticipation.AtthecompletionofSession2,parentsweregivena$10giftcard. Stimuli The stimuli were novel ‘‘stem” facts from three relational structure groups: ‘‘used to make,” ‘‘located in,” and ‘‘talks by.” Therewere two different domains within each relationalstructure, for atotalofsixuniquedomains:palmtreesandseaweed(usedtomake),desertsandforests(located 60 A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 in),anddolphinsandhippos(talksby).Withineachrelationalstructure,onedomainwaspresentedat eachtestingsession,resultingintwosetsofsimilarbutuniquestimuli.Witheachdomain,eachstory pairfeaturedtwonovelstemfactsthatcouldbecombinedtoderiveanovelintegrationfact.Thestem facts and integration facts from the domains of palmtrees, deserts, and dolphinshad been used in prior related research (e.g., Bauer & Larkina, 2017); the stem facts and integration facts from the domains of seaweed, forests, and hippos were developed for this research. All facts were accurate anddeterminedbypilottestingtobenoveltochildreninthetargetagerange.Furthermore,pilottest- ing confirmed that both stem facts were necessary to derive the integration fact (see also Bauer & Larkina,2017;Bauer&SanSouci,2010). Eachstemfactwaspresentedinthecontextofatextpassagereadaloudbyanexperimenter(see Appendixfor an example text passage).The passages were 81–89 wordsin lengthdistributed over fourpages.Eachpageconsistedofahand-drawncolorillustrationdepictingthemainactionsofthe text;thetext was notvisible on thepage. The passageswere similarin structure; in each passage, acharacteruniquetothatpassage(e.g.,ladybug)learnedanewfactduringthecourseofan‘‘adven- ture.”Onlythestemfactswereincludedinthepassages;theintegrationfactswerenotpresented. Measures Weassessedanumberofcognitivedomains. Reasoning. TheConceptFormationsubtestoftheWoodcock–JohnsonIIICOG(Test5)wasusedasa measureofcategoricalreasoningbasedoninductivelogic(medianreliability=.94from5to99years) (Woodcock et al., 2001). Prior research has revealed relations between abstract reasoning and self- derivationthroughintegration(Varga&Bauer,2014).Participantssawarowofshapeandcolorpat- ternsandwererequiredtoderivetherulethatgovernseachsequence.Participantsreceived1point foreachcorrectlyanswereditem;pointsweresummedforatotalscore.Thetotalscorewasthenused todeterminethestandardizedscoreusingtheWoodcock–JohnsonIIICompuscoreandProfilespro- gram.Theresultingscorewasbasedonchildren’sperformancecomparedwithacomparisongroup ofsame-agedpeers,withameanof100andastandarddeviationof15. Executivefunctions. Executivefunctionsweremeasuredwiththreetasks:theTrailMakingTask,Biva- lent Shape Task, and Go/No-Go task. All three were administered on a Dell Optiplex 755 computer attached to a Dell S2240Tb 21.5-inch color touchscreen monitor using the Psychology Experiment Building Language (Mueller, 2010). These tasks were included due to relations between executive functionsandotherproductiveprocesses(Goswami,1992,2011). WeadministeredaversionoftheTrailMakingTask,ameasureofcognitiveflexibility,developed specifically for children(e.g., Bowie & Harvey, 2006; Delis, Kaplan, & Krames, 2001; Mueller,2010; Reitan,1971).Childrencompletedthreetrailsbyputtingitemsinorder.Thefirsttrailrequirednumer- ationfrom1to16(1–2–3).Inthesecondtrail,thesequencewasalphabeticalfromAtoK(A-B-C).In thelasttrail,thesequencealternatedbetweenanumberandaletter(1-A-2-B-3-C).Responsetimefor thelasttrailisthemeasureofcognitiveflexibility.TheBivalentShapeTask,ameasureofinhibitory control, requires children to match the shape between objects and ignore the highly salient color (Esposito,Baker-Ward,&Mueller,2013).Thedependentvariablewasmeanreactiontimeoncorrect incongruenttrials(colorandshapemismatch).TheGo/No-Goparadigm(Archibald&Kerns,1999),a secondmeasureofinhibitorycontrol,wasadaptedfortouchscreenuse(Mueller,2010).Participants wereaskedtotapthescreeninresponsetothetargetstimulus.Thetargetstimulusappearedwith 80%frequencyandnontargetwith20%frequency.Thedependentvariabletakenfromthismeasure wastotalerrors. Long-term retrieval. The Visual Auditory Learningsubtest of the Woodcock–Johnson III COG (Test 2 and Test 10) served as a measure of long-term memory for associated stimuli (median reliability= .86 from 5 to 99years) (Woodcock et al., 2001). This measure was included to ascertain whether self-derivationthroughintegrationisrelatedtothestoringandreactivationofinformationinlong- termmemory.Inthistask,participantsareshownaseriesofrebuses(pictographicsymbolsofwords) and are later asked to recall the visual–auditory associations from long-term memory. Specifically, A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 61 oncetherebusesarelearned,participantsarepresentedwithseveralrebusesformingasentenceand areaskedtospeaktheassociatedwordsaloud(Test2).Oneweeklater,participantswerepresented withnewseriesofrebusesto‘‘read”withoutbeingremindedofthewordassociations(Test10).In both Test 2 and Test 10, participants received 1 point for each incorrectly answered item, defined asafailuretoidentifythecorrectwordortodosowithin5sofviewingarebus.Totalerrorswere enteredintotheWoodcock–JohnsonIIICompuscoreandProfilesprogram,whichreturnedstandard scoresforeachmeasurebasedonchildren’sperformancecomparedwithsame-agedpeers(again,a meanof100andastandarddeviationof15). Verbalcomprehension. TheWoodcock–JohnsonIIITest1isanorm-referencedverbalcomprehension measure(medianreliability=.90from5to99years)(Woodcocketal.,2001).Thetestincludesfour subtests—PictureVocabulary,Synonyms,Antonyms,andAnalogies—whichassesscomprehensionof individual words as well as the relation among words. The test represents context-free knowledge andsematicmemory(Schrank,2010).Thismeasurewasincludedtoascertaintherelationbetween self-derivation through integration and acquired semantic knowledge and is based on evidence of relations between self-derivation and academic performance and reading comprehension (Bauer etal.,2016;Esposito&Bauer,2017).Childrenreceived1pointforeachcorrectlyanswereditemin eachsubtest.ThesubtestscoreswerethenenteredintotheWoodcock–JohnsonIIICompuscoreand Profilesprogram,whichreturnedacompositestandardscorebasedonchildren’sperformancecom- paredwithsame-agedpeers(again,ameanof100andastandarddeviationof15). Procedure The procedure was modeled after related research (Bauer & Larkina, 2017; Bauer & San Souci, 2010).Childrenweretestedindividuallyinalaboratoryroombyoneoftwofemaleexperimenters; childrenweretestedbythesameexperimenterateachofthetwosessions.Eachsessionwasapprox- imately1hlong.Sessionswerevideo-recorded,andtherecordingswereregularlyreviewedtoensure protocolfidelity. Session1. Atthefirstsession,childrenwereexposedtothreestorypassagepairscontainingrelated facts,onestorypassagepairfromeachoftherelationalstructuregroups(i.e.,usedtomake,located in,andtalksby).Eachstorypassagewasreadtwicewhilechildrenwereencouragedtolistenandlook atthepictures.Thefirstpassagefromeachpairwasread,followedbyverbalcomprehensionSubtests AandB.Thesecondpassagefromeachstorypairwasthenread.Followingthesecondpassage,chil- dren participated in the computerized measures of executive functions in random order. Children were then tested for self-derivation through integration performance in an open-ended format. Following open-ended testing, for trials on which children did not provide a correct answer in open-endedtesting,theywerepresentedwithaforced-choicequestionwiththreealternatives,one of which was correct. Forced-choice testing was included to ensure that there would be sufficient variability if open-ended testing resulted in a restricted range. We concluded the session with the long-termretrievaltask. Session 2. Children returned to the laboratory approximately 1week after their initial visit (M=7.2days).ThesessionproceededinmuchthesamewayasSession1.Previouslyunseenpassage pairsfromeachrelationalstructurewerepresented.Storypassageswithineachpairwereseparated by the verbal comprehension Subtests C and D. Computerized executive function tasks again separated the passages from self-derivation through integration performance. Children were first askedself-derivationquestionsinanopen-endedformatfollowedbyaforced-choiceformat(again, onlywhenchildrendidnotproduceacorrectanswerinopen-endedtesting).Afterthetests,children completedthereasoningmeasureandthedelayedlong-termretrievalmeasure.Fortheself-derivation through integration task, across participants, story passage pairs were presented approximately equally often in Session 1 and Session 2. In addition, within a story passage pair, stories were presentedfirstandsecondapproximatelyequallyoftenacrossparticipants. 62 A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 Scoringofself-derivationthroughintegration Theexperimentersrecordedchildren’sanswerstoboththeopen-endedandforced-choiceintegra- tion questions. Participants were awarded 1 point for each correctly provided answer in the open- ended and forced-choice formats. Thus,there were two measures of performance: open-ended and totalscore(calculatedbyaddingopen-endedandforced-choicescores).Scoringwascheckedbyeach experimentertoensurefidelity. Results The results are reported in two sections. First, we report the self-derivation through integration performance and the relation betweensessions. Second,we report the relations betweenmeasures ofcognitiveabilitiesandself-derivationthroughintegrationperformance.Analyseswereconducted usingSPSSVersion24.Allstatisticaltestsweretwo-tailed. Self-derivationperformance Self-derivation through integration performance for both Sessions 1 and 2 is represented in Fig.1forboth6-and8-year-olds.Overall,the6-year-oldsshowedevidenceofself-derivationthrough integration at both sessions without ceiling or floor effects. The 8-year-olds showed variability in open-endedperformanceatbothSessions1and2butreachedceilinginthetotalscoreatbothsessions. PerformancefromSession1toSession2didnotdiffersignificantlyforeither6-year-olds(open-ended: t=(cid:1)1.19,p=.25;totalscore:t=(cid:1)1.19,p=.65)or8-year-olds(open-ended:t=(cid:1)0.96,p=.35;total score: t=1.45, p=.16). Across the sample, Session 1 and Session 2 were significantly correlated (r=.45,p=.004).Giventhattherewerenodifferencesinperformanceandthatperformanceacross the two sessions was correlated, Session 1 performance and Session 2 performance were summed, effectivelyincreasingtherangefrom0to3to0to6.Inaddition,giventhattherewassufficientvariance in open-ended testing (M=3.51, SD=1.86, 59% correct, range=6) but not in total score (M=5.28, SD=1.34,88%correct,range=5),onlyopen-endedperformancewasexaminedfurther. Relationswithcognitiveabilities PerformanceonthemeasuresofcognitiveabilitiesisreportedinTable1,PanelA.InTable2,Panel A, wereportthe correlations.Reasoning and verbal comprehension weresignificantlycorrelated to self-derivationthroughintegrationperformance.Noothercognitiveabilitieswerecorrelatedtoself- derivationperformance. e c n a m r o rf e p n a e M 6-year-olds 8-year-olds 6-year-olds 8-year-olds Total score Par(cid:2)cipant age Fig.1. Meanself-derivationperformanceacrosstestsessions:Experiment1. A.G.Esposito,P.J.Bauer/JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology176(2018)55–72 63 Table1 Performanceoncognitivemeasures. PanelA:Experiment1 PanelB:Experiment2 Verbalcomprehension,standardizedscore 115.49(14.35) 115.58(13.87) Long-termmemory,standardizedscore 112.93(15.91) 104.15(11.90) Long-termmemorydelay,standardizedscore 118.74(13.53) 103.87(10.58) Reasoning,standardizedscore 107.29(13.53) 108.47(11.87) TMT,totalcompletiontime 48.48(29.31) 35.60(14.08) BST,meanreactiontime 1.14(0.21) 1.05(0.19) GNG,meanerrors 4.37(2.60) 3.68(1.61) Note.Standarddeviationsareinparentheses.TMT,TrailMakingTask;BST,BivalentShapeTask;GNG,Go/No-Gotask. Wenextconductedaregressionanalysistoascertainwhetherreasoningandverbalcomprehen- sion, the measures that were significantly correlated with self-derivation performance, explained uniquevariance(seeTable3).Ageofparticipantswasalsoincludedinthemodel.Themodelwassig- nificantandexplained51%ofthevarianceinself-derivationthroughintegrationperformance.Only ageandverbalcomprehensionweresignificantpredictorsofperformance;reasoningdidnotexplain asignificantportionofthevariance. Discussion Both6-and8-year-oldsshowedevidenceofself-derivationofnewknowledgethroughintegration ofseparateyetrelatedepisodesofnewlearning.PerformanceatSession1andperformanceatSession 2werenotsignificantlydifferent,andperformanceatthetwosessionswassignificantlycorrelated. Ofthemeasuresofgeneralcognitiveabilities,onlyreasoningandverbalcomprehensionwerecor- relatedtoself-derivationthroughintegrationperformance.Yetintheregressionmodel,reasoningdid notpredictuniquevariance.Instead,verbalcomprehension—ameasureofthedepthandbreadthof accumulatedsemanticknowledge—andparticipantageexplainedsignificantanduniquevariance. Themethodusedinthecurrentexperimentaffordedavalidtestofthequestionofthespecificityof relations between measures of cognitive and semantic development and self-derivation of new knowledgethroughintegration.Yetonelimitationoftheapproachwasthatwemeasuredchildren’s performanceononly6trials,yieldingasomewhatrestrictedrangeofscores.Inopen-endedtesting, the performance range was 6 and in total score the range was 5. Accordingly, in Experiment 2 we adopted a new method of testing that more than quadrupled the number of trials in each session. Wealsoincreasedtheagerangetoinclude10-year-olds.Wedid notinclude6-year-olds,however, inanticipationthattheywouldnotbeabletomeettherequirementthattheyreadthestimulusfacts themselves(seebelow). Experiment2 Method Participants The participants were 21 8-year-olds (8 girls and 13 boys; M =8years 5months, range= age 8;2–8;11)and1910-year-olds(9girlsand10boys;M =10years5months;range=10;1–10;10). age Children participated in two sessions, each 1h long, approximately 1week apart. Children were recruitedfromthesamesourceandrepresentthesamepopulationasinExperiment1.Noneofthe children had participated in Experiment 1. Based on parental report, the sample was 25% African American, 18% biracial or multiracial, 3% Asian, and 50% Caucasian, with 3% of the sample self- identifying as Hispanic. Two parents did not respond to the request for racial self-classification. In terms of education, 15% percent of parents reported the primary caregiver having a high school degree, 10% reported having an associated degree, 22.5% reported having a college degree, and 52.5% reported havingeducationbeyond a bachelor’s degree. Asin Experiment 1, parentsprovided 6 4 Table2 Correlationsbetweenself-derivationthroughintegrationandcognitivemeasures. A .G 1.Self- 2.Verbal 3.Long-term 4.Long-termmemory 5. 6. 7.BST 8. .E derivation comprehension memory delay Reasoning TMT GNG sp o PanelA:Experiment1 sito , 1. Open-endedself-derivation P 2. Verbalcomprehension,standardized .45** .J.B score au 3. Long-termmemory,standardizedscore .28 .33* er/ 4. Long-termmemorydelay,standardized .17 .30 .77** Jou score rn 5. Reasoning,standardizedscore .33* .59** .25 .18 al o 6. TMT,totalcompletiontime .20 (cid:1).21 .15 .05 (cid:1).002 fE 7. BST,meanreactiontime (cid:1).05 (cid:1).007 (cid:1).02 .09 (cid:1).20 (cid:1).002 xp e 8. GNG,meanerrors (cid:1).006 .01 .11 .001 .02 .30 (cid:1).007 rim e PanelB:Experiment2 n 1. Open-endedself-derivation tal 2. Verbalcomprehension,standardized .42** Ch score ild P 3. Long-termmemory,standardizedscore .17 .30 sy 4. Long-termmemorydelay,standardized .05 .15 .59** ch o score lo g 5. Reasoning,standardizedscore .39* .44** .45** .58** y 1 6. TMT,totalcompletiontime (cid:1).22 (cid:1).08 .14 .41* .007 76 7. BST,meanreactiontime (cid:1).06 .29 .07 .13 .18 .28 (2 8. GNG,meanerrors (cid:1).11 (cid:1).11 .002 .16 .005 .23 (cid:1).16 01 8 ) Note.TMT,TrailMakingTask;BST,BivalentShapeTask;GNG,Go/No-Gotask. 55 * p<.05. –7 2 ** p<.01.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.