ebook img

ERIC ED607135: Task Engagement during Narrative Writing in School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Compared to Peers with and without Attentional Difficulties PDF

2020·0.53 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED607135: Task Engagement during Narrative Writing in School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Compared to Peers with and without Attentional Difficulties

Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/rasd Task engagement during narrative writing in school-age children T with autism spectrum disorder compared to peers with and without attentional difficulties Matthew Carl Zajica,*, Emily Jane Solaria, Nancy Susan McIntyreb, Lindsay Lerroc, Peter Clive Mundyd,e aReadingandAcademicDevelopmentResearchGroup,CurrySchoolofEducationandHumanDevelopment,UniversityofVirginia,405EmmetSt. South,Charlottesville,VA22904,UnitedStates bFrankPorterGrahamChildDevelopmentInstitute,UniversityofNorthCarolinaatChapelHill,CB8180,ChapelHill,NC27599,UnitedStates cTheSwainCenterforListening,CommunicationandLearning,795FarmersLn#23,SantaRosa,CA95405,UnitedStates dSchoolofEducation,UniversityofCalifornia,Davis;OneShieldsAve,Davis,CA95616,UnitedStates eMINDInstitute,UniversityofCalifornia,Davis,282550thSt.,Sacramento,CA95817,UnitedStates ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Background: Childrenwithautismspectrumdisorder(ASD)demonstratehighlyvariablewriting Attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder skills. Few studies have examined if engagement during writing assessments may differ for Autismspectrumdisorder childrenwithASDandiftaskengagementisrelatedtotheirwritingassessmentperformance. Education ThisstudyexaminednarrativewritingandbroadtaskengagementinchildrenwithASDcom- School-age pared to peers with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and typically developing Taskengagement (TD)peers. Writtenexpression Method: SixtychildrenwithASD,32childrenwithADHD,and29TDchildrencompletedas- sessmentsofcognitiveskills,symptomseverity,andspontaneousnarrativewriting.Timespent engagedduringwritingwasassessedduringthespontaneousnarrativewritingtask. Results: TheASDgroupperformedlowestontextorganizationandqualityscoresaswellasword productionscoreswhilealsospendingtheleasttimeengagedwiththewritingtask.Timespent engagedwasmoststronglyassociatedwithnarrativewritingscoresintheASDgroupandex- plaineduniquevarianceintextorganizationandqualityscoresandwordproductionscoresafter controllingforrelatedage,cognitiveskills,andsymptomseverityvariables.TheADHDgroup showedsimilarassociationsbetweentimespentengagedandwordproductionscores,andtime spentengagedexplaineduniquevarianceinwordproductionscores. Conclusions: TimespentengagedcompletingthewritingtaskappearedlowestfortheASDgroup andmaysuggestwritingtaskengagementtobeamoreprominentdifficultyareaforchildren withASDcomparedtopeerswithADHDandTDpeers.Implicationsforbetterunderstandingand supportingthewritingskillsofchildrenwithASDarediscussed. ⁎Correspondingauthor. E-mailaddresses:[email protected](M.C.Zajic),[email protected](E.J.Solari),[email protected](N.S.McIntyre), [email protected](L.Lerro),[email protected](P.C.Mundy). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2020.101590 Received6June2019;Receivedinrevisedform8May2020;Accepted24May2020 Available online 17 June 2020 1750-9467/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 1. Introduction Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder wherein individuals demonstrate persistent difficulties in social communication and social interaction across multiple contexts alongside the presence of restricted, repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests, or activities (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In recent years, researchers have expanded beyond clinical understandings of ASD to consider how the social cognitive features of ASD may influence development across different domains, such as academic skill development (Bauminger-Zviely, 2014). Children with ASD without a co-occurring intellectual disabilityorothersevere communication difficultiesdemonstrateheterogenous academicskills(Ibrahim, 2020; Keen,Webster,& Ridley,2016;Whitby&Mancil,2009).Yet,limitedresearchhassoughttounderstandthedifferentfactorsunderlyingthishetero- geneity(e.g.,Corbett&Iqbal,2018). Anemergingresearchareafallsonunderstandingthewrittenlanguageskillsofschool-agechildrenwithASD(e.g.,Finnegan& Accardo,2018;Zajic&Asaro-Saddler,2019;Zajic&Wilson,2020).Overthelast20years,studieshavefoundthatchildrenwithASD demonstrate heterogeneous writing skills (e.g., Brown, Johnson, Smyth, & Oram Cardy, 2014; Dockrell, Ricketts, Charman, & Lindsay,2014;Grace,Enticott,Johnson,&Rinehart,2017;Griswold,Barnhill,Myles,Hagiwara,&Simpson,2002;Kushki,Chau,& Anagnostou,2011;Mayes&Calhoun,2003a,2006a;Mylesetal.,2003;Zajicetal.,2018)withwritingdifficultiessuggestedtooccur more frequently than reading or mathematics difficulties (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006a; Mayes, Waschbusch, Calhoun, & Mattison, 2019).WhilenotallchildrenwithASDstrugglewiththesamewritingdifficulties,empiricalstudiescomparingchildrenwithASDto theirtypicallydeveloping(TD)peersandtotheirpeerswithotherdevelopmentaldisordershavesuggestedahigherprevalenceof difficulties in handwriting and text organization and quality (Mayes & Calhoun, 2006a; Zajic & Wilson, 2020) with a lower pre- valenceofdifficultiesinspelling,grammar,andpunctuation(Finnegan&Accardo,2018).Empiricalstudieshaveofferedtentative explanationsforsomeofthesedifficulties,includingfactorsrelatedtocognition(i.e.,IQ;Mayes&Calhoun,2003b,2008),language andsocial communication(Brownetal.,2014;Dockrelletal., 2014;Graceetal.,2017; Hilvert,Davidson,&Gámez,2020),and attentionandexecutivefunctions(Dirlikovetal.,2017;Graceetal.,2017;Hilvert,Davidson,&Scott,2019;Zajicetal.,2018). PartofthedifficultyofunderstandingthecomplexityofthewritingchallengesaffectingchildrenwithASDisthatwritingisa complex,multifacetedsetofskills.Accordingtothesimpleviewandthenot-so-simpleviewofwriting,writingskillscanbebroadly conceptualizedintotranscription(handwriting,keyboarding,andspelling)andtextgeneration(turningideasintowords,sentences, andlargerdiscourseunits;Berninger&Amtmann,2003;Berninger&Winn,2006).Thesewritingskillsrelyonadditionalsocialand cognitive processes to help navigate different writing scenarios, including executive functions and attention (Berninger & Winn, 2006)aswellasmotivation(Hayes&Berninger,2014).Thesesocialcognitiveprocessessupportself-regulationduringwritingby managinghowwritersdevelopplans,gathercontenttowriteabout,organizeandconstructcontentappropriatetothewritingtask, andregulatethoughtsandbehaviorstoremainengagedduringwritingtasks(Graham,2018;Hayes&Berninger,2014;Zimmerman &Risemberg,1997).Theimportanceofself-regulationtowritinghasbeenwellsupportedbyunderlyingtheoriesofwrittenlanguage developmentandhasresultedineffectiveinstructionalapproachesthathelpstrugglingwritersbecomemorestrategicintheirwriting choices(seeBerninger&Winn,2006;Graham,2018;Harris&Graham,2016;Hayes&Berninger,2014;Zimmerman&Risemberg, 1997). Writers remain strategically engaged with the writing process through formulating intentions, planning, monitoring, and reacting in order to gather, organize, write, and revise across most writing activities (see Graham, 2018). Children experiencing difficulties with self-regulation and associated skills (e.g., control mechanisms; Graham, 2018) may have challenges with staying engagedwiththeirwriting. Few studies haveofferedpreliminary insightsintothe roles ofthesecontrol mechanisms inchildrenwith ASD(Hilvert et al., 2019; Zajic & Wilson, 2020). Recent observational and intervention studies have been split regarding the examination of skills associatedwithattentionandself-regulation:observationalstudiesgenerally focusonattentional difficultiesthroughuse ofcom- parisongroups(e.g.,Dirlikovetal.,2017;Graceetal.,2017;Zajicetal.,2018),whileinterventionstudiesprioritizestructuringthe writingprocess via self-managementinterventions (Asaro-Saddler, 2016). The focus onattention acrossobservational studieshas suggested that attentional difficulties may play a role in the writing difficulties experienced by some individuals with ASD in handwriting(Dirlikovetal.,2017;Graceetal.,2017)aswellastextgeneration(Zajicetal.,2018).Followingthisfocusonexecutive functionskills,Hilvertetal.(2019)reportedthatchildrenwithASDwithgreaterexecutivedysfunctionweremorelikelytoexhibit idiosyncraticlanguageuse(i.e.,greaterfrequencyoflongwordsandrarewords). Alimitationofthecurrentresearchliteratureisalackofexaminationintohowchildrenengagewiththewritingprocessduring writingassessments.AwidearrayofpsychoeducationalassessmentshavebeendeemedappropriateforusewithchildrenwithASD (Corbett&Iqbal,2018),andtherelativelysmallsetofstudiesthathaveexaminedthewrittenlanguageskillsofchildrenwithASD haveusedaverywidearrayofdifferentassessments(seeZajic&Wilson,2020).Additionally,theseassessmentsoftenemphasize scoringwrittenproductswithlessemphasisplacedonhowchildrencompletethetasks. Children with ASD demonstrate highly variable engagement patterns in educational settings (Bryan & Gast, 2000; Sparapani, Morgan,Reinhardt,Schatschneider,&Wetherby,2016;Wilkinson,2008),yetstudiesfocusedonengagementduringwritingtasks remainlimited(e.g.,Hilvertetal.,2020).Typically,studiesrelyonwrittenproductmeasuresorexamineproductivitysolelyasword production,resultinginfewavailable studiesthat provideinsightsinto howparticipantsengage duringtheassessment.Griswold etal.(2002)commentedthatsomeoftheirchildrenwithASDrefusedtowritebutofferedlimitedinsightsintowhytheyrefusedor howchildrenengagedduringtheassessment.Dockrelletal.(2014)comparedwritersandnonwriterswitheitherASDorspecific languageimpairmenttofindthatnonwritersacrossgroupswereyoungeranddemonstratedhigherASDsymptomseverityscoresbut werenodifferentfromwritersoncognitive,language,ortranscriptionskills.Inarecentstudy,Hilvertetal.(2020)reportedthat school-agechildrenwithASDexperiencegreaterdifficulties gettingstartedandstaying ontaskduringpersonalnarrativewriting 2 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 compared to their TD peers. Other studies have examined writing skills using designs that did not allow for examining writing engagement.Forexample,Brownetal.(2014)askedparticipantstocompleteapersuasiveessayusinganunspecifiedamountoftime (reportingthatallparticipantsfinishedwithin30minbutwithlimitedadditionaldetails). Examining time spent engaged during writing also may provide further exploration into the role of attentional skills during writingforchildrenwithASDcomparedtopeerswithattentionaldifficulties.Attentionisadistinctcontrolmechanismthatsupports broaderexecutivefunctionandself-regulationskillsbyhelpingtoregulatethebehaviorsusedduringawritingtask(seeBerninger& Amtmann,2003; Graham,2018;Hayes&Berninger, 2014).Studiesfocused ontheattentionalskillsofchildrenwithASDduring writing have usedboth measures of attentional difficulties as well as comparisongroups, commonly incorporating peers withat- tention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder(ADHD)toofferamultiplegroupperspective(e.g.,Graceetal.,2017;Mayes,Breaux,Calhoun, &Frye,2019;Mayes&Calhoun,2006a;Zajicetal.,2018).IndividualswithADHDexhibitpatternsofinattentiveand/orhyperactive behavior that can impact self-regulation and academic task management (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; DuPaul & Langberg,2015).ChildrenwithADHDdemonstratesimilarwritingchallengestothosenotedinchildrenwithASD,withdifficulties fallingacrosstextorganization,production,andconstruction(DeBonoetal.,2012;Mayes&Calhoun,2006b;Mayesetal.,2019a; Molitor,Langberg,&Evans,2016).Yet,similartothefindingsofchildrenwithASD,thewritingskillsofchildrenwithADHDappear to be heterogeneous (see Molitor et al., 2016) and have received limited sustained examination (see Graham, Fishman, Reid, & Hebert,2016).PrevalenceestimatesofASDandADHDcomorbiditygreatlyrangeinschool-agechildren(16–44%)withchildren demonstratinginternaldistractibilitywithhighASDsymptomseverityandhighexternaldistractibilitywithhighADHDsymptom severity(seeDeprey&Ozonoff,2018).ADHDsymptomseveritymayplayaroleinthewrittenlanguagedifficultiesexperiencedby childrenwithASD,asthelevelofADHDsymptomseverityobservedinchildrenwithASDhasbeenshowntobepredictiveofwriting challengesthatarerelatedtobroadercognitiveandexecutivefunctiondifficulties(Dockrelletal.,2014;Hilvertetal.,2019;Zajic etal.,2018).OngoingresearchisneededthatexamineschildrenwithASDtopeerswithADHDthatconsidersbothbetween-group differencesinengagementaswellaswithin-grouprelationshipsbetweentaskengagementandwritingskills. 2. Currentstudy ThisstudyexaminednarrativewritingandbroadtaskengagementinchildrenwithASDcomparedtochildrenwithADHDandTD childrentoconsiderbetween-groupdifferencesinnarrativewritingandtimespentengagedandtoexplorewithin-grouprelationships between narrative writing, time spent engaged, and related factors (i.e., age, cognitive abilities, and ASD and ADHD symptom severity).ThisstudycontributestotheliteratureexploringwritingdevelopmentinchildrenwithASDandoffersnewinsightsrelative totaskengagementduringwritingthatmayinformfutureassessmentandinstructionalresearch.Importantly,thestudyusedtheTest of Written Language, 4th Edition (TOWL-4; Hammill & Larsen, 2009), a standardized, picture-based psychoeducational narrative writingassessmentthatseparateswritingintodistincttaskstages(Scripting,Planning,andWriting).UsingtheTOWL-4allowedfor examinationofbroadtaskengagementduringwritingwhereparticipantsreceivedavisualstimulustowriteabout(whichpotentially allowedforlessambiguityaboutthetaskdemandscomparedtowhatmightbeexpectedwithmoreabstractornon-picture-based writingtasks).ThecurrentstudyfocusedontaskengagementspecificallyduringtheWritingTaskStage,andapreviouspublication examinedengagementduringthePlanningTaskStagewiththeincludedparticipants(seeZajic,Solari,McIntyre,Lerro,&Mundy, 2020).Threeresearchquestionsguidedthisstudy: 1) DochildrenwithASDdifferfrompeerswithADHDorTDpeersa)onnarrativewritingscoresandb)ontimespentengagedwitha structured,picture-basednarrativewritingtask? 2) Dogroupsdifferintheira)relationshipsbetweennarrativewritingscoresandtimespentengaged,andb)relationshipsbetween thesewritingtaskvariablesandage,cognitiveskills,andASDandADHDsymptomseverity? 3) Within groups, does time spent engaged explain additional variance in narrative writing scores after controlling for related variables(i.e.,age,cognitiveskills,andASDandADHDsymptomseverity)? 3. Methods All data were collected from children participating in a longitudinal study on academic, cognitive, and social development. Familieswererecruitedthroughauniversity-affiliatedcenter’ssubjecttrackingsystem,byfliersthroughlocalschooldistricts,andby wordofmouth.ThisresearchwasconductedincompliancewiththeInstitutionalReviewBoardattheUniversityofCalifornia,Davis. In order to control for conditions that might significantly impact participants’ performance, exclusionary criteria included an identifiedsyndromeotherthanASDorADHD,significantsensoryormotorimpairment,aneurologicaldisorder,psychoticsymptoms, or any major medical disorder that could be associated with extended absences from school. This study included 121 children between9–17yearsold,whichincluded60childrenwithASD(50male),32childrenwithADHD(25male),and29TDchildren(18 male). Children with ASD and ADHD came into the study with community diagnosesthat were confirmed via trained researcher assessmentandparentreport. 3.1. Diagnosticandsymptomseveritymeasures The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, 2nd Edition (ADOS-2; Lord et al., 2012) confirmed diagnosis (ASD group) and assessed ASD symptom severity (ASD and ADHD groups). The ADOS-2 is a semi-structured, standardized assessment of 3 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 communication, social interaction, play, and restricted and repetitive behaviors. A research-reliable team member administered either Module 3 (Child/Adolescent) or Module 4 (Adolescent/Adult) to calculate algorithm-adjusted Total Scores. Modules de- monstratestronginterraterreliabilityviapublisher-reportedintraclasscorrelationcoefficients(αs=0.82−0.94;Lordetal.,2012). TheSocialResponsivenessScale(SRS;Constantino&Gruber,2005)providedanadditionalassessmentofASDsymptomseverityfor allgroups.Parents/guardianscompletedthe65-itemratingscalethatmeasuresASDsymptomseverityandgeneralsocialrespon- siveness in naturally occurring contexts. The SRS provides a Total Score as a composite of five subscales (Awareness, Cognition, Communication,Motivation,andMannerisms)andhasdemonstratedstronginternalconsistency(αs=0.93−0.97;Constantino& Gruber,2005). TheConners3(Conners,2008)confirmedADHDdiagnosis(ADHDgroup)andassessedADHDsymptomseverityacrossgroups. TheConners3isacomprehensiveassessmentforidentifyingthepresenceofADHDsymptomatology,andparents/guardianscom- pleted the DSM-IV Inattentive Scale and DSM-IV Hyperactive/Impulsive subscales. Both parent-report forms have strong demon- stratedinternalconsistency(αs>0.90;Conners,2008). 3.2. Cognitivemeasures TheWechslerAbbreviatedScaleofIntelligence,2ndEdition(WASI-II;Wechsler,2011)assessedcognitiveabilities.Twoverbal (VocabularyandSimilarities)andtwononverbal(BlockDesignandMatrixReasoning)subtestswereadministeredtocomputeverbal reasoning(VerbalComprehensionIndex[VCI]),nonverbalreasoning(PerceptualReasoningIndex[PRI]),andFull-ScaleIQ(FIQ) estimates(Ms=100,SDs=15).InternalconsistencyCronbach’salphacoefficientsforthesampledemonstratedgreat-to-excellent subtestreliabilities(αs=0.87−0.93)andwereconsistentwithpublisher-reportedalphas(α=0.96;Wechsler,2011).Allpartici- pantshadanestimatedFIQofatleast73. 3.3. Writingmeasures TheTOWL-4assessednarrativewritingviatheSpontaneousWritingsubtestwhereparticipantsproducedahandwritten,fictional narrativeaboutaprovidedpicture.Duringtheassessment,participantslistentoasamplestoryaboutanexamplepicture(Scripting), plan their own story about a new picture (Planning), and draft their story (Writing). Written responses are evaluated based on ContextualConventions(21items;spelling,morphological,andgrammaticwritingconventions)andStoryComposition(11items; textorganizationandqualityviavocabulary,plot,prose,characterdevelopment,andreaderinterest).Thesescaledscores(M=10, SD=3)areusedtocalculateaSpontaneousWritingIndexcompositescore(M=100,SD=15).Asnostandardizedscoreforword productionisprovided,thetotalnumberofwordsproducedwerecountedforeachsample.Thoughwordproductionmayserveasa proxy for handwriting, this measure of word production is impacted by task demands. Therefore, word production is used as a measureoftextproductionandshouldnotbeinterpretedasanisolatedhandwritingorfluencymeasure. All writing samples were scored by two research assistants trained by the first author using the TOWL-4 scoring guidelines (Hammill&Larsen,2009).Trainingwascompletedwhenresearchassistantsreached100%agreementacrossexamplesamplesand couldarticulaterationalesforallscoringdecisions.Interraterreliabilitywascalculatedonarandomselectionofwritingsamples(n= 34)usingintraclasscorrelationcoefficientstreatedastwo-wayrandom(Shrout&Fleiss,1979).Interraterreliabilitywasgreat-to- excellentforContextualConventions(α=0.94),StoryComposition(α=0.84),andwordproduction(α=1.00). 3.4. Tasktimespentengaged Timespentengagedwasoperationalizedasthetotalduration(inseconds)participantsspent completingtheTOWL-4Writing TaskStage.Participantswereprovidedamaximumof10min(600s)tocompletetheirwrittennarratives(whichwasreducedfrom therecommended15minduetolargerstudyvisittimeconstraints).Thestartpointforcalculatingtimespentengagedbeganaftera trainedresearchteammemberexplainedthewritingtaskandansweredanyclarifyingquestionsfromtheparticipant.Participants receivedremindersatfiveandnineminutestocontinuewriting.Ifaparticipantusedtheentiretimeandwasstillwritingwhentime wasup,theycouldfinishtheircurrentsentence.Thestoppointforcalculatingtimespentengageddiffereddependingonindividual behaviors.Ifaparticipantremainedengagedwiththetaskforthefull10min,thenthestoppointwastheendofthetask(with600s recordedfortimespentengaged).Ifparticipantssaidtheywerefinishedbeforethetaskwasdone,thenaresearchteammember encouraged participants to try to write a full page and prompted participants to think about the details in the task picture. If participantsthenrefusedtowritemoreorsaidtheyhadwrittenasmuchastheycould,thenthetaskwasended,andthestoptime wasrecorded. 4. Dataanalysis Analyses were conducted using SPSS 25 (IBM Corp., 2018). Groups were matched on age but differed on IQ (ASD=ADHD, ASD < TD,andADHD < TD;Table1).Toaccountforthisdifference,groupcomparisonscovariedforFIQ.Toaddresstheresearch questions, three sets of analyses were conducted. First, a univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with follow-up pairwise comparisonswithaBonferroniadjustmentexaminedoverallgroupdifferencesontheTOWL-4SpontaneousWritingIndex.Second,a multivariateanalysisofcovariance(MANCOVA)followedbyANCOVAswithaBonferroniadjustmentexaminedgroupdifferenceson TOWL-4 writing scores (Contextual Conventions, Story Composition, and word production) and task time spent engaged. Third, 4 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 Table1 DescriptiveStatisticsandGroupComparisonsforAge,CognitiveAbilities,andSymptomSeverity. ASD ADHD TD n=60 n=32 n=29 M(SD) Range M(SD) Range M(SD) Range F p 2 p Age 12.63(2.11) 9.33−17.17 13.13(2.30) 9.64−17.31 12.74(2.29) 9.35−17.02 0.55 0.58 0.01 VCIb 95.92(15.48) 60−136 98.84(14.33) 72−128 111.24(13.79) 81−138 10.72 < 0.001 0.15 PRIb 102.25(16.86) 70−150 99.94(15.81) 74−126 116.83(14.14) 91−160 10.44 < 0.001 0.15 FIQb 98.73(15.13) 73−132 99.31(15.00) 74−131 115.62(12.82) 90−150 14.46 < 0.001 0.20 ADOS-2a,c 10.75(3.61) 7−24 4.14(3.81) 0−13 – – 62.80 < 0.001 0.42 SRSd 82.49(10.24) 52−91 61.29(14.66) 42−91 43.69(7.50) 34−66 127.01 < 0.001 0.69 Inattenb 73.97(11.19) 50−90 77.63(10.57) 54−90 46.90(10.67) 35−90 75.60 < 0.001 0.56 Hyperb 71.40(15.52) 39−90 71.13(17.27) 40−90 47.14(8.93) 38−73 29.58 < 0.001 0.33 Note. aOnly the ASD and ADHD groups received the ADOS-2. bASD=ADHD, ASD<TD, ADHD<TD. cASD>ADHD. dASD>ADHD, ASD>TD, ADHD>TD. ASD=autism spectrum disorder. ADHD=attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. TD=typically developing. VCI=VerbalComprehensionIndex.PRI=PerceptualReasoningIndex.FIQ=Full-ScaleIQ.ADOS-2=AutismDiagnosticObservationSchedule, 2nd Edition Total Score. SRS=Social Responsiveness Scale Total Score. Inatten=Conners 3 DSM-IV Inattentive Parent Report Scale. Hyper=Conners 3 DSM-IV Hyperactive/Impulsive Parent Report Scale. Reprinted from Zajic, Solari, McIntyre, Lerro, & Mundy (2020) with permissionfromElsevierwitheffectsizecalculationsadded. Pearsonproduct-momentcorrelationswereconductedseparatelyforeachgrouptoexaminea)therelationshipsbetweennarrative writingscoresandtimespentengagedandb)therelationshipsbetweenwritingandtaskengagementvariablesandage,cognitive skills (VCI, PRI, and FIQ), and symptom severity (ADOS-2, SRS, and Conners 3). These interrelationships guided the building of group-specifichierarchicalmultipleregressionspredictingContextualConventions,StoryComposition,andwordproductionscores thatincludedvariablesatStep1(age,cognitiveskills,andsymptomseverityestimatesrelatedtothatTOWL-4score)andStep2(time spent engaged). Models were examined for variance explained (R2) and change in variance explained between steps (ΔR2) with contributionfromindividualpredictorsassessedviastandardizedbetacoefficients(β). 5. Results Descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. As mentioned, groups were matched on age but differed on cognitive abilities (ASD=ADHD,ASD < TD,andADHD < TD;Table1).TheASDgroupdemonstratedelevatedsymptomseverityontheADOS-2 comparedtotheADHDgroupandontheSRScomparedtoADHDandTDgroups.TheASDandADHDgroupsdemonstratedelevated ADHDsymptomseverityrelativetotheTDgroupbutdidnotdifferthemselves.Norm-referencedscoresfortheTOWL-4scoresforthe ASD and ADHD groups predominantly fell in the below average to average ranges (TD fell in the average range), and groups demonstratedwiderangesforthetotalnumberofwordsproduced(Table2). 5.1. GroupcomparisonsforTOWL-4scoresandtimespentengaged 2 GroupsdifferedontheSpontaneousWritingIndex,F(2,117)=7.96,p<0.001, p=0.12.TheASDgroupperformedlowerthan Table2 MultivariateandUnivariateBetween-GroupEffectsforGroupbyTOWL-4WritingScoresandTimeSpentEngaged. ASD ADHD TD n=60 n=32 n=29 M(SE) Range M(SE) Range M(SE) Range F p 2 p SpontaneousWritingIndex 101.09(2.18) 55−128 106.79(2.95) 58−135 117.54(3.35) 93−158 7.96 0.001 0.12 ContextualConventionsa 10.10(0.33) 2−15 10.15(0.45) 2−15 12.08(0.51) 8−18 5.28 0.006 0.08 StoryCompositiona 9.36(0.42) 2−16 11.13(0.57) 2−15 12.52(0.65) 8−20 8.68 < 0.001 0.13 WordProductiona 82.52(5.67) 2−183 107.75(7.67) 2−186 135.32(8.70) 74−230 12.78 < 0.001 0.18 TimeSpentEngaged(s)a 460.23(16.11) 112.48−600.00 537.77(21.79) 254.72−600.00 544.56(24.73) 421.08−600.00 6.10 0.003 0.09 Note.ASD=autismspectrumdisorder.ADHD=attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder.TD=typicallydeveloping.TOWL-4=TestofWritten Language,4thEdition.SWI=SpontaneousWritingIndex.CC=ContextualConventions.SC=StoryComposition.FIQincludedasacovariatefor allanalyses(FIQ=102.93). a V=0.26,F(8,230)=4.24,p<0.001, 2p=0.13. 5 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 yacross 5 0.300.330.360.31–0.200.220.180.24–0.220.06−0.08 ntextualOS-2=3DSM- ndADHDSymptomSeverit 34 0.85***0.46*0.89***0.69***–0.65***0.65***–0.360.310.090.42*0.230.170.160.220.240.24––0.030.02−0.08−0.05−0.06−0.01 Edition.CC=TOWL-4Cox.FIQ=Full-ScaleIQ.ADortScale.Hyper=Conners gagedwithAge,CognitiveSkills,andASDa TD 4512 0.64***0.35*–0.53**0.74***0.030.53**–0.78***0.340.85***0.89***–0.57**0.46*0.69***0.57**–0.300.330.54**0.33−0.020.200.310.130.230.16−0.06−0.020.130.150.140.070.230.19−0.18−0.16––−0.44*−0.26−0.110.140.120.18−0.03−0.09−0.26−0.32−0.130.04 TOWL-4=TestofWrittenLanguage,4thonIndex.PRI=PerceptualReasoningInde=Conners3DSM-IVInattentiveParentRep weenTOWL-4WritingScoresandWritingTaskStageTimeSpentEn ADHD 345123 0.92***0.75***0.35**–0.56**0.87***0.96***0.80***0.49***0.56**–0.89***–0.83***0.46***0.87***0.89***–0.83***–0.54***0.64***0.74***0.78***0.46***0.54***–0.35*0.260.340.33*0.46***0.120.38*0.41*0.45*0.49***0.47***0.43**0.45*0.250.39*0.32*0.26*0.27*0.00−0.03−0.020.48***0.43**0.42**0.240.120.20−0.19−0.13−0.13−0.24−0.29−0.31−0.29*−0.19−0.20−0.22−0.46**−0.39*−0.13−0.040.110.21−0.020.10−0.12−0.060.04−0.02−0.33−0.20 ention/deficit-hyperactivitydisorder.TD=typicallydeveloping.=TOWL-4SpontaneousWritingIndex.VCI=VerbalComprehensiTotalScore.SRS=SocialResponsivenessScaleTotalScore.Inatten Table3PearsonProduct-MomentCorrelationCoefficientsbetASD,ADHD,andTDGroups. ASD Variable12 1CC–0.77***2SC0.77***–3SWI0.92***0.96***4WordProduction0.75***0.80***5TimeSpentEngaged0.35**0.49***6Age0.28*0.33**7VCI0.46***0.45***8PRI0.34**0.27*9FIQ0.47***0.43**10ADOS-2−0.09−0.2511SRS−0.11−0.39**12Inatten−0.11−0.1313Hyper−0.12−0.10 Note.ASD=autismspectrumdisorder.ADHD=attConventions.SC=TOWL-4StoryComposition.SWIAutismDiagnosticObservationSchedule,2ndEditionIVHyperactive/ImpulsiveParentReportScale.***p<0.001,**p<0.01,*p<0.05. 6 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 theTDgroup(p < 0.001)butnodifferentfromtheADHDgroup(p=0.36).TheTDandADHDgroupsdidnotdiffer(p=0.06). TheMANCOVAfortheTOWL-4subtestscores,wordproduction,andtimespentengageddemonstratedheterogeneityofvariance- covariance matrices (Box’s M=62.02, p<0.001). Groups differed on these scores, V = 0.26, F(8, 230)=4.24, p < 0.001, 2 p=0.13, with follow-up ANCOVA pairwise comparisons showing group differences across variables (ps<0.01; Table 2). On ContextualConventions,theASDgroupperformedlowerthantheTDgroup(p=0.007)butnodifferentfromtheADHDgroup(p= 1.00).TheADHDgroupperformedlowerthantheTDgroup(p=0.02).OnStoryComposition,theASDgroupperformedlowerthan theADHD(p=0.04)andTDgroups(p < 0.001).TheADHDandTDgroupsdidnotdiffer(p=0.36).Onwordproduction,theASD groupproducedfewerwordsthantheADHD(p=0.03)andTDgroups(p<0.001).TheADHDandTDgroupsdidnotdiffer(p= 0.07).Ontimespentengaged,theASDgroupspentlesstimeengagedwiththetaskthantheADHD(p= 0.01)andTDgroups(p= 0.02).TheADHDandTDgroupsdidnotdiffer(p=1.00). 5.2. RelationshipsbetweenTOWL-4scoresandtimespentengaged Correlation coefficients are reported in Table 3. For the ASD group, time spent engaged demonstrated moderate associations acrossTOWL-4scores(rs=0.35−0.54,ps<0.01).FortheADHDgroup,timespentengagedwasmoderatelyassociatedwithword production (r = 0.57, p = 0.001) and Contextual Conventions (r = 0.35, p = 0.05) while not significantly associated with the SpontaneousWritingIndex(r=0.34,p=0.054)andStoryComposition(r=0.26,p=0.15).FortheTDgroup,timespentengaged wasnotassociatedwiththeTOWL-4scores(rs=0.30−0.36,ps>0.05). 5.3. RelationshipsbetweenTOWL-4scoresandtimespentengagedwithage,cognitiveskillsandsymptomseverity CorrelationcoefficientsarereportedinTable3.FortheASDgroup,TOWL-4scoresshowedweak-to-moderateassociationswith age(rs=0.28−0.46,ps<0.05)andcognitiveabilities(rs=0.27–0.49,ps<0.05).SRSTotalscoreshowedaweakassociationwith theSpontaneousWritingIndex(r=-0.29,p=0.03)andamoderateassociationwithStoryComposition(r=−0.39,p=0.002). ADOS-2 Total Score showed no significant relationships (ps>0.05). Neither Conners 3 parent report scores showed significant relationshipswithTOWL-4scores(ps>0.05).Timespentengagedshowedweak-to-moderatecorrelationswithcognitiveabilities(rs =0.27−0.43,ps<0.05)andnosignificantcorrelationswithageandADOS-2,SRS,orConners3scores(ps>0.05). For the ADHD group, TOWL-4 scores showed overall moderate associations with age (rs = 0.38−0.54, ps<0.05). The Spontaneous Writing Index and Contextual Conventions showed moderate associations with VCI and FIQ (rs = 0.39−0.45, ps<0.03) but not PRI (ps>0.05). SRS Total Score showed moderate correlations with the Spontaneous Writing Index, Story Composition,andwordproduction(rs=-0.39−-0.46,ps<0.05).ADOS-2TotalScoreandConners3parentreportscoresshowedno significantassociationswithTOWL-4scores(ps>0.05).Timespentengagedshowednosignificantcorrelationswithage,cognitive abilities,orsymptomseverity(ps>0.05). For the TD group, only word production showed a moderate correlation with age (r = 0.42, p = 0.02) with no significant correlationsbetweenTOWL-4scoresandcognitiveabilities orsymptomseverity (ps>0.05). Timespent engagedshowednosig- nificantcorrelationswithage,cognitiveabilities,orsymptomseverity(ps>0.05). 5.4. Hierarchicalmultipleregressions RegressionswereconstructedforContextualConventions,StoryComposition,andwordproductionseparatelyfortheASDand ADHD groups. The TD group was excluded from these analyses due to the limited variability in time spent engaged and limited correlationalevidencebetweentimespentengagedandTOWL-4scores.Step1variablesfortheASDandADHDgroupswereselected basedonthesignificanceoftheircorrelationcoefficients(p<0.05).Step2forbothgroupsincludedtimespentengaged. Hierarchical regression models for the ASD group are reported in Table 4. For Contextual Conventions, Step 1 (age and FIQ) explained30%oftheoverallvariance,F(2,57)=12.05,p<0.001.FIQ(β=0.47,p<0.001)butnotage(β=0.27,p=0.25) explaineduniquevarianceatStep1.TheinclusionoftimespentengagedatStep2resultedinanon-significantincreaseinadditional varianceexplained,ΔR2=0.02,F(1,56)=1.24,p=0.27.ForStoryComposition,Step1(age,FIQ,andSRS)explained42%ofthe overallvariance,F(3,55)=13.08,p<0.001.TheinclusionoftimespentengagedatStep2accountedforanincreaseinoverall varianceexplained,ΔR2=0.06,F(1,54)=6.15,p=0.016.Age(β=0.33,p=0.002),FIQ(β=0.24,p=0.035),SRSTotalScore (β=-0.33.p=0.002),andtimespentengaged(β=0.27,p=0.016)eachexplaineduniquevarianceatStep2.Forwordpro- duction, Step 1 (age and FIQ) explained 39 % of the overall variance, F(2, 57)=18.14, p<0.001. The inclusion of time spent engagedatStep2accountedforanincreaseinoverallvarianceexplained,ΔR2=0.11,F(1,56)=12.84,p=0.001.Age(β=0.41, p<0.001),FIQ(β=0.26,p=0.02),andtimespentengaged(β=0.37,p=0.001)explaineduniquevarianceatStep2. HierarchicalregressionmodelsfortheADHDgrouparereportedinTable4.ForContextualConventions,Step1(ageandVCI) explained 27 % of the overall variance, F(2, 29)=5.47, p = 0.01. VCI (β=0.37, p = 0.03) but not age (β=0.28, p = 0.10) explained unique variance at Step 1. The inclusion of time spent engaged at Step 2 accounted for a non-significant increase in additionalvarianceexplained,ΔR2=0.05,F(1,28)=2.17,p=0.15.ForStoryComposition,Step1(ageandSRS)explained29%of theoverallvariance,F(2,28)=5.59,p=0.01.SRSTotalScore(β=-0.36,p=0.04)butnotage(β=0.23,p=0.10)explained unique variance at Step 1. The inclusion of time spent engaged at Step 2 accounted for a non-significant increase in additional varianceexplained,ΔR2=0.00,F(1,27)=0.34p=0.56.Forwordproduction,Step1(ageandSRS)explained38%oftheoverall 7 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 Table4 HierarchicalMultipleRegressionsforTOWL-4WritingScoresfortheASDandADHDGroups. ASD Model1 Model2 R2 ΔR2 B SE β t p B SE β t p ContextualConventions Step1 0.30 – Age 0.36 0.15 0.27 −1.16 0.25 0.34 0.15 0.26 2.28 0.03 FIQ 0.09 0.02 0.47 4.20 < 0.001 0.08 0.02 0.41 3.33 0.002 Step2 0.31 0.02d TimeSpentEngaged 0.00 0.00 0.14 1.12 0.27 StoryComposition Step1 0.42 – Age 0.66 0.19 0.37 3.53 < 0.001 0.59 0.18 0.33 3.26 0.002 FIQ 0.09 0.03 0.34 3.25 0.002 0.06 0.03 0.24 2.16 0.035 SRS −0.13 0.04 −0.37 −3.48 < 0.001 −0.12 0.04 −0.33 −3.22 0.002 Step2 0.48 0.06a TimeSpentEngaged 0.01 0.00 0.27 2.48 0.016 WordProduction Step1 0.39 – Age 9.29 2.13 0.45 4.37 < 0.001 8.41 1.95 0.41 4.31 < 0.001 FIQ 1.20 0.30 0.42 4.04 < 0.001 0.74 0.30 0.26 2.48 0.02 Step2 0.50 0.11b TimeSpentEngaged 0.10 0.03 0.37 3.58 0.001 ADHD Model1 Model2 R2 ΔR2 B SE β t p B SE β t p ContextualConventions Step1 0.27 – Age 0.35 0.20 0.28 1.72 0.10 0.24 0.21 0.19 1.11 0.28 VCI 0.07 0.03 0.37 2.28 0.03 0.07 0.03 0.37 2.27 0.03 Step2 0.33 0.05d TimeSpentEngaged 0.01 0.00 0.25 1.47 0.15 StoryComposition Step1 0.29 – Age 0.40 0.23 0.29 1.71 0.10 0.36 0.25 0.26 1.46 0.16 SRS −0.08 0.04 −0.36 −2.15 0.04 −0.08 0.04 −0.35 −1.99 0.06 Step2 0.29 0.00d TimeSpentEngaged 0.00 0.01 0.10 0.59 0.56 WordProduction Step1 0.38 – Age 9.33 3.24 0.45 2.88 0.01 7.12 3.04 0.35 2.34 0.03 SRS −0.96 0.52 −0.29 −1.87 0.07 −0.75 0.47 −0.23 −1.58 0.13 Step2 0.51 0.13c TimeSpentEngaged 0.18 0.07 0.39 2.70 0.01 Note.TOWL-4=TestofWrittenLanguage,4thEdition.ASD=autismspectrumdisorder.ADHD=attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder.FIQ= Full-ScaleIQ.TimeSpentEngaged=TimespentcompletingtheTOWL-4spontaneouswritingsubtest.VCI=VerbalComprehensionIndex.SRS= SocialResponsivenessScaleTotalScore. a p=0.016. b p=0.001. c p=0.012. d p>0.05. variance,F(2,28)=8.62,p=0.001.TheinclusionoftimespentengagedatStep2accountedforanincreaseinoverallvariance explained,ΔR2=0.13,F(1,27)=7.25,p=0.012.Timespentengaged(β=0.39,p=0.01)andage(β=0.35,p=0.03)butnot SRSTotalScore(β=-0.22,p=0.13)explaineduniquevarianceatStep2. 6. Discussion This study had three main goals to understand differences and relationships in narrative writing and time spent engaged in childrenwithASDcomparedtopeerswithADHDorTDpeers:1)examinegroup-leveldifferencesinnarrativewritingandtimespent engaged;2)examinegroup-specificrelationshipsbetweennarrativewritingandtimespentengagedaswellastoage,cognitiveskills, andsymptomseverity;and3)examineiftimespentengagedexplainsadditionalvarianceinnarrativewritingaftercontrollingfor relatedvariables(age,cognitiveskills,andsymptomseverity). Group-leveldifferencesinnarrativewritingshowedtheASDgroupgenerallyperformedlowerthantheADHDandTDgroups(i.e., SpontaneousWritingIndex,StoryComposition,andwordproduction).ThispatterndifferedforContextualConventions,wherethe 8 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 ASDandADHDgroupsdidnotsignificantlydifferbutperformedlowerthantheTDgroup.Inonlyonescore(SpontaneousWriting Index)wastheredifferentiationbetweentheASDandTDgroupswhiletheADHDgroupdidnotdifferfromeithergroup.Group-level differencesintimespentengagedshowedtheASDgroupspentlesstimeengagedrelativetotheADHDandTDgroups.Within-group relationshipsshowedtimespentengagedwasassociatedmorestronglytoawiderarrayofnarrativewritingscoresintheASDgroup comparedtotheADHDandTDgroups.TimespentengagedexplainedadditionalvarianceinStoryCompositionandwordproduction scoresintheASDgroupbutonlyinwordproductionscoresfortheADHDgroup(afterincludingassociatedfactors). 6.1. Narrativewritingscores TheASDgroupscoredaroundaverageontheSpontaneousWritingIndexandContextualConventionswithslightlylowerthan average performance on Story Composition. However, not all children scored in the average ranges, as seen by the wide scoring rangesthatvariedfromverylowtoveryhigh(Table2).TheADHDgroupperformedsimilarlytotheASDgroup(withoverallscores around average but with very wide scoring ranges). These findings support previous findings from both groups that have shown heterogeneousgroup-levelwritingskillswherechildrenoftenscoreasagroupintheaveragerangebutshowhighlyvariablescoring ranges(Graceetal.,2017;Grahametal.,2016;Mayes&Calhoun,2006a,2006b,2008;Zajic&Wilson,2020). Overallfindingsoflowerscoresfortextorganizationandqualityandaveragescoresforspelling,grammar,andpunctuationfor the ASD group align with previous research findings (Brown et al., 2014; Dockrell et al., 2014; Mayes & Calhoun, 2006a; Price, Martin, Chen, & Jones, 2020; Zajic et al., 2018). The ADHD group, in contrast, demonstrated average spelling, grammar, and punctuationskillswhileshowingslightlyelevatedgroup-leveltextgenerationandorganizationskills.Thesefindingsdeviatefrom some past research findings that highlighted text organization and quality as areas of concern using different writing tasks (e.g., DeBonoetal.,2012;Mayes&Calhoun,2006a).Additionally,whilesomepriorresearchhasfoundchildrenacrossbothgroupsto demonstrate challenges with skills like spelling that were assessed as part of Contextual Conventions (e.g., DeBono et al., 2012; Finnegan&Accardo,2018),currentfindingsofnogroup-leveldeficitsmaybepartiallyduetothelackofisolatedskillassessments. TheASDgroupscoredlowerintextorganizationandqualityrelativetoboththeADHDandTDgroups,highlightingapotential areaofdifficultyonthisassessmentinrelationtothecomparisongroups.Thismoderategroup-leveldifferenceissupportedbyprior researchhighlightingtextgenerationasanareaofdifficultyforsomechildrenwithASD(Brownetal.,2014;Dockrelletal.,2014; Priceetal.,2020).Furthermore,theASDgroupproducedthefewestnumberofwordsrelativetobothcomparisongroups,andthe ADHDgroupproducedfewerwordsrelativetotheTDgroup.Difficultieswithwordproductionmayberelatedtohandwritingand graphomotordifficultiesnotedascommoninchildrenwithASDandADHD(e.g.,Dirlikovetal.,2017;Mayesetal.,2019a).However, withoutanisolatedassessmentofhandwritingskills,thesedifferencesmayhavealsobeenduetodifficultiesrelatedtogenerating textinlinewithnarrativetaskdemandsinadditiontopotentialtranscriptiondifficulties(seeBerninger&Amtmann,2003;Berninger &Winn,2006). FindingsdifferfromthreestudiesimplementinganearlierversionoftheTOWL(TOWL-3;Hammill&Larsen,1996)withchildren with either ASD or ADHD. A key difference between the two versions is the TOWL-3 produced three scores for the spontaneous writingsubtest:ContextualConventions,ContextualLanguage,andStoryConstruction.Troybetal.(2014)foundnogroupdiffer- encesbetweentheirASDandTDgroupsinwordproduction,ContextualConventions,ContextualLanguage,andStoryConstruction. Mylesetal.(2003)foundnogroupdifferencesbetweentheirASDandTDgroupsinContextualConventions,ContextualLanguage, andStoryConstruction,however,theynotedasignificantdifferenceinwordproduction(TD > ASD).DeBonoetal.(2012)foundno group differences across subscales between their ADHD group and their subclinical ADHD group while also reporting no group differences in word production. The differences between these studies and the current study may be due to scoring sensitivity concernspresentintheTOWL-3thatwerebetteraddressedbytheTOWL-4(seeHammill&Larsen,2009).However,thesedifferences mayalsobeduetosampledemographics,shorteningoftheallottedSpontaneousWritingTaskadministrationtimeforthisstudy,or statisticalpowerduetolimitedsamplesizes. 6.2. Timespentengaged TheASDgroupspentlesstimeengagedwiththeoverallwritingtaskcomparedtotheADHDandTDgroups.Onaverage,theASD groupengagedfor460s(7min,40s)whiletheADHDgroupengagedfor537s(8min,57s)andtheTDgroupengagedfor544s (9min,4s).ThoughtheASDandADHDgroupsshowedawiderrangeofdurationsfortimespentengagedwiththetaskrelativetothe TD group, the ASD group remained lowest in terms of overall time spent engaged. The low engagement noted in the ASD group appearssimilartorecentfindingsshowingthatchildrenwithASDdemonstrategreaterdifficultiesgettingstartedwithandstayingon task during expository writing (Hilvert et al., 2020). Time spent engaged appeared most strongly associated with cognitive skills (predominantlyverbalskills)onlyfortheASDgroup,asnootherrelationshipsappearedsignificantacrossothervariablesorother groups.Thisfindingsuggeststhatverbalknowledgeandtaskengagementmaybepositivelyrelated,whichalignswithpriorresearch findingrelationshipsbetweenlinguisticprocessesandwritingskillsinchildrenwithASD(e.g.,Brownetal.,2014;Dockrelletal., 2014; Hilvert et al., 2020). Additionally, the lack of significant associations between time spent engaged with age and symptom severity also suggest that task engagement was not related to the age of the participants or the degree of either ASD or ADHD symptomseverity.However,itisimportanttoconsiderthattheserelationshipsmaydifferinanon-picture-basedtask,giventhat childrenwouldnothaveavisualstimulustofocuson.Additionally,therelationshipsfortheADHDandTDgroupsremainednon- significant.Oneexplanationforthelackofsignificantrelationshipsinthesegroupsmaybeduetolessheterogeneityobservedintime spentengagedwiththetask(asaveragetimesforbothgroupsappearedclosertotheoveralltotalallottedtime). 9 M.C.Zajic,etal. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 76 (2020) 101590 LowertimespentengagedwiththetasksuggestspotentialchallengesfortheASDgroupthatmayberepresentativeofstruggling writers,suchasdifficultywithassessingcompletenessoftaskdemands,navigatingthegoalsofthetask,orfeelingoverwhelmedby thedemandsofthetask(Graham&Perin,2007;Harris&Graham,2016).Lowertimespentengagedmayalsosuggestmotivation difficultieswithcompletingthetaskdemandsthatmayberelatedtofrustrationswithtranscriptionortextgeneration.Motivation towardswritinginfluenceshowindividualsseethemselvesaswriters(Graham,2018)andhasbeensuggestedasanareaofdifficulty forsomewriterswithASD(e.g.,Boucher&Oehler,2013)butwithlittlecurrentempiricalexamination.However,notallindividuals withASDendedthetaskbeforetheendoftheallottedtime,suggestingthatremainingengagedforthedurationofthewritingtaskis notadifficultyforallchildrenwithASD.ItisalsoimportanttonotethatwhiletheADHDgroupspentmoretimeengagedoverallwith thetaskthatdidnotsignificantlydifferfromtheTDgroup,therewerechildrenwithADHDwhoendedthetaskwellbeforethealloted time. 6.3. Relationshipsbetweenwriting,timespentengaged,age,cognitiveabilities,andsymptomseverity Resultsindicatedthatrelatedfactorslikeage,cognitiveskills,andsymptomseverityaccountedfor30–42%and27–39%ofthe varianceinthewritingscoresfortheASDandADHDgroups,respectively.Theadditionoftimespentengagedaccountedforad- ditionalvarianceexplainedfortwoscoresfortheASDgroup(StoryComposition:42%increasedto48%;wordproduction:39% increasedto50%)andonescorefortheADHDgroup(wordproduction:38%increasedto51%).Modelsforbothgroupsincluded agewhilemostincludedcognitiveskillsandASDsymptomseverity.NomodelsincludedADHDsymptomseverity. 6.3.1. Age Initialcorrelationalanalysesshowedweak-to-moderateassociationsforageacrossallnorm-referencednarrativewritingscoresin theASD andADHD groupswhileshowingnon-significantassociationsfortheTDgroup.Allgroups showedamoderate-to-strong relationship between ageand word production and non-significant associationsbetween age and time spent engaged in the task. However,whenaccountingforadditionalfactors,ageremainedasignificantpredictorfortheASDgroupforonlyStoryComposition andwordproduction(withoutandwithtimespentengaged)andfortheADHDgroupforwordproduction(withoutandwithtime spentengaged). Giventhat word productionis expectedtoincrease withage (astheinvolved processesbecomemore automatic;Berninger& Amtmann,2003), the associationsbetweenage andword productionwere expected acrossgroups. Furthermore,as thenarrative writing scores were norm-referenced by age groups, the lack of association between age and Contextual Conventions was also expected.Unexpected,however,wasthesignificantassociationbetweenageandStoryCompositionfortheASDgroup.Priorstudies involvingchildrenwithASDhaveoftenincludedageasavariableofinterestorasacontrolvariableduetotheuseofnon-stan- dardizedwritingmeasuresorwidesampleageranges(e.g.,Brownetal.,2014;Dockrelletal.,2014;Hilvertetal.,2019).Findings fromthecurrentstudy,however,suggestthatageexplaineduniquevarianceintextqualityandorganizationfortheASDgroupina norm-referenced narrative writing score while accounting for cognitive skills and symptom severity. This distinction for Story CompositionisimportantgiventhattheTOWL-4hasbeenarecommendedassessmentofnarrativewrittenlanguageskillsofchildren withASD (Corbett &Iqbal, 2018). Possible explanationsfor this association might include transcription, textgeneration, and ex- ecutivefunctionskilldifferencesacrosstheincludedageranges(e.g.,olderchildrenwrotetextmorefluentlythatmetthenarrative taskdemandduetounderstandingtheunderlyingnarrativestorystructure)aswellasthepotentialforthistobeduetoshortening theoverall task-timelength (e.g., older children are more able to meet the task demands in the shortened duration compared to younger children). Understanding the influence of age requires further insights across the school-age years (see Zajic, Dunn, & Berninger,2019forfurtherdiscussion). 6.3.2. Cognitiveskills Initial correlational analyses showed overall cognitive skills to be most strongly related to narrative scores in the ASD group comparedtotheADHDandTDgroups.TheASDgroupshowedmoderaterelationshipsbetweenverbalskillsandnarrativescoresand weakrelationshipsbetweenperceptualskillsandnarrativescores.Acrossmodels,overallcognitiveskillsremainedauniquepredictor ofnarrativewritingscores(withoutandwiththeinclusionoftimespentengaged).Incontrast,theADHDgroupshowedmoderate relationships between verbal skills and Contextual Conventions with no significant relationships between perceptual skills and narrative scores. Verbal skills were only included in the model explaining Contextual Conventions (where they explained unique variance).TheTDgroup,incontrasttobothothergroups,showednosignificantrelationshipsbetweenverbalorperceptualskillsand narrativescores. Current results support prior findings of the relationships between verbal and perceptual skills in relation to writing skills in childrenwithASDandADHD.Inonepriorstudy,MayesandCalhoun(2008)reportedasimilarfindingthatwrittenlanguageskillsin childrenwithASDwererelatedsimilarlytobothverbalandperceptualskills.Thestrongerrelationshipfoundforverbalskillsrather thanperceptualskillsmayalsoberelatedtorecentfindingsoftheimportanceoflanguagetothewrittenlanguageskillsofchildren withASD(e.g.,Brownetal.,2014;Dockrelletal.,2014;Priceetal.,2020).TurningtochildrenwithADHD,MayesandCalhoun (2006b) found verbal skills to be more strongly related to writing scores than perceptual skills in children with ADHD, which is similartothecurrentfindings.However,MayesandCalhoun(2006b)reportedoverallcognitiveskillstohavethestrongestasso- ciationwithwritingforchildrenwithADHD,whichwasnotthecaseforthecurrentstudy.Onereasonforthisdifferencemaybethe useoftheWASI-IIratherthantheWechslerIntelligenceScaleforChildren(WISC;Wechsler,2003).TheFIQestimatefromtheWASI- IIisderivedfromverbalandperceptualreasoningabilitiesthatdonotassessworkingmemoryandprocessingspeedskillsthathave 10

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.