LSHSS Review Article Listening-Related Fatigue in Children With Unilateral Hearing Loss Fred H. Bess,a Hilary Davis,a Stephen Camarata,a and Benjamin W. Y. Hornsbya Purpose:Listening-relatedfatigueisanunderstudied Results:Resultsofourliteraturereviewandexploratory constructthatmaycontributetotheauditory,educational, analysessuggestthatadultsandCHLareatincreased andpsychosocialproblemsexperiencedbychildrenwith risk for listening-related fatigue. Importantly, this unilateralhearingloss(UHL).Herein,wepresentanoverview increased risk was similar in magnitude regardless oflistening-relatedfatigueinschool-agechildrenwithhearing of whether the loss was unilateral or bilateral. loss(CHL),withafocusonchildrenwithUHL. Subjective ratings, based on child self-report and Method:Followingareviewofresearchexamininglistening- parent proxy report, were consistent, suggesting relatedfatigueinadultsandCHL,wepresentpreliminary that children with unilateral and bilateral hearing loss findingsexploringtheeffectsofunilateralandbilateralhearing experienced greater listening-related fatigue than lossonlistening-relatedfatigueinchildren.Forthese childrenwithnohearing loss.Incontrast, resultsbased exploratoryanalyses,weuseddatacollectedfromour onteacherproxyreportwerenotsensitivetotheeffects ongoingworkdevelopingandvalidatingatool,theVanderbilt ofhearingloss. FatigueScale,formeasuringlistening-relatedfatiguein Conclusions:ChildrenwithUHLareatincreasedrisk children.Presently,weareassessing3versionsofthe forlistening-relatedfatigue,andthemagnitudeoffatigue fatiguescale—childself-report,parentproxy,andteacher issimilartothatexperiencedbychildrenwithbilateral proxy.Usingthesescales,datahavebeencollectedfrom hearingloss.Problemsoflistening-relatedfatiguein morethan900participants.Datafromchildrenwithunilateral school-ageCHLmaybebetteridentifiedbyCHL andbilateralhearinglossandforchildrenwithnohearingloss themselvesandtheirparentsthanbyteachersand arecomparedwithadultVanderbiltFatigueScaledata. specialistsworkingwiththechildren. U nilateralhearingloss(UHL)inchildrenisacom- Prevention[CDC],2005;Tharpe&Sladen,2008).Theleading moncondition.Althoughtheestimatedpreva- causesofUHLinchildrenarecongenitalcytomegalovirus lenceofcongenitalsensorineuralhearingloss followedbycongenitalinnerearmalformation(e.g.,enlarged (HL;>40dBHL)atbirthisonlyapproximatelytwoper vestibularaqueduct),bacterial/viralmeningitis,viral/bacterial 1,000and30%–40%exhibitUHL(Kral,Hubka,Heid,& mumps,andChiarimalformation(Porter,Bess,&Tharpe, Tillein,2013;vanWieringen,Boudewyns,Sangen,Wouters, 2016;Tharpe&Sladen,2008;vanWieringenetal.,2019). &Desloovere,2019),theprevalenceofUHLappearsto AlthoughdefinitionsofUHLmayvaryacrossstudies, increasewithage.Forexample,theprevalenceofUHLin complicatingcomparisons,proceedingsfromthe2005 school-agechildren(Grades3,6,and9)is3%orapproxi- NationalWorkshoponMildandUnilateralHearingLoss mately1,380,000childrenintheUnitedStates(Bess,Dodd- suggestUHLexistswhentheaveragepure-toneair-conduction Murphy,&Parker,1998;CentersforDiseaseControland thresholdat0.5,1,and2kHzis≥20dBHLorwhenpure- toneair-conductionthresholdsare>25dBHLattwoormore frequenciesabove2kHzintheaffectedear,coupledwith anaveragepure-toneair-conductionthresholdinthegood aDepartmentofHearingandSpeechSciences,VanderbiltBillWilkerson earof≤15dBHL(CDC,2005). Center,VanderbiltUniversitySchoolofMedicine,Nashville,TN Researchfindingsrelatedtothepsychoeducational CorrespondencetoFredH.Bess:[email protected] outcomes of children withUHL aremixed.Nonetheless, Editor-in-Chief:ElizabethA.Walker numerousstudies have reportedthat school-agechildren Editor:DawnaE.Lewis withUHLexperiencea varietyofauditory,educational, ReceivedFebruary15,2019 andpsychosocialproblems(Bess,1982;Bessetal.,1998; RevisionreceivedJuly17,2019 Bess&Tharpe,1984;Fitzpatricketal.,2019;Lewisetal., AcceptedAugust9,2019 https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-OCHL-19-0017 PublisherNote:ThisarticleispartoftheForum:Evidence-Based PracticesandOutcomesforChildrenWithMildandUnilateral Disclosure:Theauthorshavedeclaredthatnocompetinginterestsexistedatthetime HearingLoss. ofpublication. 84 Language,Speech,andHearingServicesinSchools (cid:129) Vol.51 (cid:129) 84–97 (cid:129) January2020 (cid:129) Copyright©2020AmericanSpeech-Language-HearingAssociation Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions 2016;Porteretal.,2016;Reeder,Cadieux,&Firszt,2015). riskofrecurrentlistening-relatedfatigueanditsnegative Compared to children without HL, children with UHL consequences(Bess,Gustafson,&Hornsby,2014;Bess exhibit more difficulty understanding speech in noisy etal.,2016;Bess&Hornsby,2014a;Hornsby,Werfel, conditions (Bess, Tharpe, & Gibler, 1986; Hartvig Jensen, Camarata,&Bess,2014;Hornsbyetal.,2017;McGarrigle, Johansen, & Borre, 1989; Lewis et al., 2016), localizing Gustafson,Hornsby,&Bess,2019).Giventhesefindingsand sounds in the horizontal plane (Bess et al., 1986; Humes, theunderlyingdeficitsinbinauralprocessingresultingfrom Allen, & Bess, 1980; Reeder et al., 2015), developing age- UHL,itisreasonabletosuspectthatchildrenwithUHL appropriatelanguageandcognitiveskills(Fitzpatricketal., mightalsoexperiencefatiguedifficulties,especiallyinthe 2019;Halliday,Tuomainen,&Rosen,2017;Klee&Davis- schoolsetting. Dansky,1986),andmaintainingsatisfactoryperformance Here,weofferareviewonlistening-relatedfatiguein inschool(Bessetal.,1998;Bess&Tharpe,1984;Brookhouser, childrenwithUHL.GiventhatresearchspecifictoUHL Worthington,&Kelly,1991;Lieu,2004,2013;Lieu,Tye- doesnotexist,wefirstprovideafocusedreviewofthelit- Murray,&Fu,2012;Oyler,Oyler,&Matkin,1988).In eraturerelatedtosubjectivefatigueinadultswithHLand addition,somechildrenwithUHLhavelowself-esteem, childrenwithbilateralHL.Additionally,weexploreexisting anxiety,strainedpeerrelations,anddecreasedsocial evidencerelatedtofatigueinadultswithUHLandpresent support(Bessetal.,1998).EvenwhentheUHLisidentified somepreliminaryfindingsonlistening-relatedfatiguein early,childrenmaystillexperiencesomeoftheabove- childrenwithUHLusingvalidationdatafromourongoing referencedcommunicativeandeducationalproblems workdevelopingtheVanderbiltFatigueScale(VFS). (Fitzpatricketal.,2019;Yoshinaga-Itano,Johnson,Carpenter, &Brown,2008). Fatigue, Effort, and HL Althoughmanystudieshaveshownthatchildrenwith UHLexperienceavastarrayofacademicandcommunica- Acomprehensiveexaminationoffatigueisbeyondthe tiveproblems,suchfindingsarenotuniversal(Hallmo, scopeofthisreviewarticle;readersinterestedinanin-depth Møller,Lind,&Tonning,2009;Keller&Bundy,1980). coverageofthissubjectarereferredtootheroverviewpapers Theseconflictingresultsmaybeduetoalackofstatistical (Bess&Hornsby,2014a;Hornsby,Naylor,&Bess,2016; power(Keller&Bundy,1980)and/orafailuretoinclude McGarrigleetal.,2014).Briefly,fatigueiscommonplace standardizedtestmeasuresoracontrolgroup(Hallmoetal., inoursociety.Itisacomplexconstructwithphysicaland 2009);however,contemporaryresearchclearlydemonstrates mental/cognitivedimensions,anditisoftencharacterized thatsomechildrenwithUHLhaveanincreasedriskfor byfeelingsofweariness,tiredness,agenerallackofenergy, communicationandeducationaldifficulties(Fitzpatrick and/orareduceddesiretocontinueonwithatask.When etal.,2019;Porteretal.,2016). fatigueissevereandrecurrent,thesesubjectiveeffectsmay Themechanismsresponsibleforthecommunicative alsobeaccompaniedbyfatigue-relateddecrementsinphys- andpsychoeducationalproblemsexperiencedbychildren icaland/ormentalperformance. withUHL arenot wellunderstood;however,a primary Whilemanyfactorsmodulateitsmagnitude,fatigue factorfrequentlyreferencedintheliteratureisrelatedto —bothmentalandphysical—isacommonconsequenceof theimportanceofbinauralhearingincommunication.Itis sustainedhigh levels of effort (Hockey, 2013).Here, we welldocumentedthatbinaurallisteningoffersdistinctad- focusonfatigueresultingfrommental,ratherthanphysi- vantagesovermonaurallistening,especiallyinnoisycondi- cal,effortasitismostrelevanttotheproblemsofchildren tions.Factorssuchasbinauralsummation,headshadow, withhearingloss(CHL).Ourspecificfocusisonlistening andbinauralreleasefrommasking(squelcheffect)provide effort,atypeofmentaleffortthathasbeendescribedas listenerswithoutHLtheuniqueabilitytoidentifyandlo- theallocationofmentalresourcestolisten,process,and calizesoundsources(Blauret,1997).Whenchildrenhave understandspeechandotherauditorystimuli(Downs,1982; UHL,however,thesebinauralcuesarediminished,thus McGarrigleetal.,2019;Pichora-Fulleretal.,2016). producingrippleeffectsthatmayimpactareassuchasedu- Children in classroom settings havemanydifferent cation,speechrecognitioninnoise,andlanguageandcog- demands placedonthemthat requiresignificant mental nitiveskills.Otherfactorsthatmightalsocontributetothe effortforsuccessfulcompletion.Forexample,mentaleffort problemsofchildrenwithUHLincludechangesinauditory isrequiredtocompleteawrittenassignment,readaschool- andbrainstructuresasaresultofauditorydeprivation,age book,orlistentotheteacherandotherchildrenintheclass- ofidentification/intervention,degreeofHLintheimpaired room.Themagnitudeoflisteningeffortrequiredinthese ear,andourinabilitytoidentify“earlyon”thosechildrenat situationscandependonmanyfactors,suchasthestudent’s riskforacademicandcommunicativedifficulties(Fitzpatrick cognitiveandattentioncapabilitiesandtheclassroom etal.,2019; Kumpik & King, 2019; Tharpe, 2008; van acoustics(McGarrigleetal.,2019).Importantly,tocompen- Wieringen et al., 2018). satefortheirlisteningdifficulties,childrenandadultswith Althoughresearchislimited,emergingevidencesug- HLmustincreasetheirmentaleffortcomparedtopersons geststhatlistening-relatedeffortandfatiguemayalsobean withoutHLwhenattemptingtodetect,process,andre- importantcontributortosomeofthedifficultiesexperienced spondtoauditorystimuli,suchasspeech(Hicks&Tharpe, bychildrenwithUHL.Recentinvestigationshaveshownthat 2002;McGarrigleetal.,2014,2019).Thisrecurrentneed children,somewithevenmild,bilateralHL,areatincreased forhighlevelsoflisteningeffortputsindividualswithHL Bessetal.:Listening-RelatedFatigue 85 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions atriskfordevelopmentoflistening-relatedfatigueandits haveconcentratedonfatigueintheadultpopulation negativeconsequences. (Alhanbali,Dawes,Lloyd,&Munro,2017;Hetuetal., Formostchildren,complaintsofmildtransientfatigue 1988;Hornsby&Kipp,2016;Nachtegaaletal.,2009). arenormal,andsuchfatiguetypicallyresolvesreadilywith Many of these studies focused on quality of life in the ashortrestorbreaks.Hence,theimpactoftransientfatigue workplace using broad interviews and general surveys, as onacademicperformanceandqualityoflifeisexpectedto opposed to validated, fatigue-specific measures. However, beminimal.Forsomechildren,however,theirfatiguecanbe interview and survey results suggested that adults with moresevereandrecurrentandelicitedbyroutinedailyactiv- HL experienced more stress, a common antecedent of fa- ities,suchasself-care;completingdifficult,buttypical,class- tigue (Hockey, 1983) in the workplace, than their peers roomassignments;orrelevanttoCHL,listeninginnoisy withoutHL,andsuchstressandfatiguenegativelyimpacted conditions(Bess&Hornsby,2014b).Thistypeoffatigueis workperformance(Hasson,Theorell,Wallén,Leineweber, knowntoimposeadverseconsequencesonbothadultsand &Canlon,2011;Krameretal.,2006;Morataetal.,2005; children.Forexample,researchsuggeststhatfatigueexperi- Nachtegaaletal.,2009). encedbyworkingadults,withorwithoutHL,cannegatively HornsbyandKipp(2016)wereoneoftheearliestto impactworkperformanceandoveralllifequality(Deluca, investigatefatigueand vigor1usinga validated,fatigue- 2005;Hetu,Riverin,Lalande,Getty,&St-Cyr,1988;Kramer, specificmeasure.TheyusedtheProfileof MoodStates Kapteyn,&Houtgast,2006;Morataetal.,2005). (McNair,Lorr,& Droppleman,1971) to assessfatigue Likewise,researchexaminingchildrenwithchronic andvigorinalargesampleofadultswithmild-to-profound healthconditions(CHCs;e.g.,cancer,cerebralpalsy,rheu- losswhopresentedtoanaudiologyclinicseekinghelpfor maticdiseases,andchronicfatiguesyndrome)suggeststhat hearingdifficulties.Theirresultsshowedthat,comparedto thistypeoffatiguecanimposeadverseeffects.Forinstance, normativedata,adultsseekinghelpforhearingdifficulties comparedtononfatiguedchildren,thosesufferingfrom weremorethantwiceaslikelytoexperienceseverefatigue fatiguearemorelikelytohaveincreasedschoolabsences, andmorethanfourtimesaslikelytoreportseveredeficits reducedacademicperformance,andareducedabilityto inenergy.2Interestingly,regressionanalysesrevealedno engageinnormaldailyactivities,allofwhichnegatively associationbetweendegreeofHLandsubjectiveratings affectqualityoflife(Gaba&Howard,2002;Hockenberry- offatigueorvigor.Thatis,adultswithamildHLorapro- Eatonetal.,1999;Ravid,Afek,Suraiya,Shahar,&Pillar, foundlosswereequallysusceptibletoincreasedfatigueand 2009).Importantly,individualswithadditionalhandicap- vigordeficits. pingconditions,asarecommonlyfoundinCHL,areespe- Alhanbalietal.(2017)usedadifferentvalidatedfatigue- ciallyvulnerabletofatigueanditsnegativeconsequences specificmeasure,theFatigueAssessmentScale(Michielsen, (Hardy&Studenski,2010).Thesignificanceofthefatigue DeVries,VanHeck,VandeVijver,&Sijtsma,2004),torepli- experiencedbyCHLishighlightedwhenwecomparetheir cateandexpandonthefindingsofHornsbyandKipp.They fatiguetothatofchildrenwithotherCHCs.Theoverallfa- comparedfatigueratingsfromthreedifferentgroupsofadults tigueofCHLissimilarto,orsignificantlygreaterthan,that withHL(UHL:n=50;bilateralhearingaidusers:n=50; ofchildrenwithmostotherCHCsknowntoaffectfatigue andcochlearimplantusers:n=50)toratingsfromanage- (e.g.,cancer,diabetes,rheumatoidarthritis,andinflamma- matchedcontrolgroupwithoutHL(n=50).AllHLgroups toryboweldisease;Hornsbyetal.,2017). reportedsignificantlymoresubjectivefatiguethantheage- Summarizingtothispoint,fatigueisacommonphe- matchedcontrolgroup.Importantly,fatigueratingswere nomenoninoursociety,andtheconsequencesoffatigue similarforallthreeHLgroups,regardlessofwhetherthey canbesignificant.Mountingevidencesuggeststhatadults worebilateralhearingaids,usedacochlearimplant,or,rele- andCHLareatincreasedriskforlisteningeffortandfatigue. vanttothisreviewarticle,hadonlyUHL.Thus,researchin Becauseoftheirhearingdifficulties,agreaterlevelofeffortis adultssuggeststhatHLofanykind(e.g.,bilateraloruni- requiredinordertolistenattentivelyandunderstandspeech laterallosses,cochlearimplantusersorhearingaidusers) innoisyconditions.Theadditionalattention,concentra- mayincreaseriskforfatigue.However,additionalwork tion,andlisteningeffortrequiredtoovercomeauditory isneededtoensureotherfactors,suchassamplebias, deficitsassociatedwithHLresultsinincreasedreportsof arenotdrivingthisfinding(seeHornsby&Kipp,2016; listening-relatedfatigue.Moreover,asthelisteningeffort Alhanbalietal.,2017,fordiscussions). andfatigueofCHLincreasesthroughouttheday,their impactonimportantcognitiveabilities(e.g.,listening,memo- rizing,attending)forprocessing/decodingspeechandlearning mayalsoincrease(Bessetal.,2014;Hornsbyetal.,2016). 1Vigor,likefatigue,isamoodstate.Itisassociatedwithageneral Thesectionbelowhighlightssomeoftheevidencerelated feelingofbeingenergetic,alert,andfullofenergyorpep.Although tofatigueinadultsandCHL. fatigueandvigorareoftenstronglyandnegativelycorrelated,validation studiessuggesttheyareindependentconstructs,ratherthanasingle bipolarfactor(McNair&Heuchert,2010;Stein,Jacobsen,Blanchard, HL and Listening-Related Fatigue in Adults &Thors,2004;Stein,Martin,Hann,&Jacobsen,1998). 2Severefatigueandvigordeficitsweredefinedasratingsthatwere StudiesexaminingfatigueinindividualswithHLare, 1.5SDsormoreabove(highfatigue)orbelow(vigordeficit)the ingeneral,scant.Mostofthosethathavebeenconducted ProfileofMoodStatesnormativemeans. 86 Language,Speech,andHearingServicesinSchools (cid:129) Vol.51 (cid:129) 84–97 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions HL and Listening-Related Fatigue in Children associationbetween degree of HLandfatigueratings was observed,suggestingchildrenwithevenmildhearinglosses Inthepast,muchofwhatweknewaboutfatiguein were as equally susceptible to fatigue as children with CHLwasbasedonanecdotalreportsandpilotstudies.In more moderately severe losses. In addition, highlighting recentyears,however,wehavelearnedmuchmoreabout thepotentialimportanceofincreasedfatigueinCHL,sig- fatigueinpediatricHL.Similartoadults,thereisagrow- nificantassociationsbetweenlanguageabilityandratings ingbodyofresearchshowingthatsustainedlisteningde- ofcognitivefatiguewereobservedinthatstudy.Werfeland mandsexperiencedbyCHLputthematincreasedriskfor Hendricks(2016)reportedasimilarfindinginchildrenwith listening-relatedfatigueanditsadverseconsequences.The profoundHLwhousedcochlearimplants.Theseauthors studiestofollowfocusonareviewofsubjectivefatiguein foundthatchildrenwithcochlearimplants(Grades3–6, childrenwithbilateralHLandchildrenwithUHL. N=19)reportedPedsQL-MFSratingssimilartothoseof theCHLfromthestudyofHornsbyetal.(2014).Impor- BilateralHL tantly,theyalsofoundthattheincreasedfatigueratings OneoftheearlieststudiestoexplorefatigueinCHL wereassociatedwithpoorerlanguageandliteracyskills wasBess,Dodd-Murphy,andParker(1998).Theseinvesti- intheirCHL.Whethertheincreasedfatigueexperienced gators conducteda clinical-basedstudyonchildren with byCHLleadstolanguageandliteracydeficitsorviceversa minimalHL—minimal HL included threecategoriesof remainsanunknown,butimportant,researchquestion. HL(unilateralsensorineuralHL,minimalbilateralsenso- rineural HL,andhigh-frequency sensorineuralHL;for UHL definitions,seeCDC,2005).Onecomponentofthisstudy AlthoughinformationonfatigueinchildrenwithUHL wasananalysis of functionalhealth statusinschool-age islimited,pilotstudiesandreportsfromparents,clini- children (Grades6 and 9) using theCOOPAdolescent cians,andteachersofferindirectevidencethatchildrenwith Chart Method (COOP;Nelson et al.,1987).TheCOOP UHLcouldbeatincreasedriskforfatigue,suchastheir chartsmeasurefunctionalstatusonacoresetoffunctional peerswithbilateralHL.Forexample,inthe1998studyby dimensions—physical,emotional,andsocial.Childrenwith Bessandcoworkersdescribedearlier,COOPresults(partic- minimalsensorineuralHLexhibitedmoredysfunctionthan ularlyonthestressandenergysubtests)showedthatCHL normalcontrolsonmanyofthesubtests—especiallystress experiencedmoredysfunction.Althoughthestudydidnot andenergy,twoconstructscloselyassociatedwithfatigue. parsethescoresforeachHLgroup,itisnoteworthythat Inapilotstudy,Hornsbyetal.(2014)assessedfatigue morethanhalfoftheparticipantsthatcompletedtheCOOP inasmallgroup(n=10)ofschool-ageCHLandanage- hadUHL.Additionalsupportcanbefoundinthestudy matchedgroup(n=10)ofchildrenwithoutHLusinga byHornsbyetal.(2014),inwhichthePedsQL-MFSwas generic,validatedsubjectivemeasureoffatigue,thePediatric administeredtoasmalldiversegroupofCHL(n=10) QualityofLifeInventoryMultidimensional Fatigue Scale andanage-matchedcontrolgroup.TwooftheCHLin (PedsQL-MFS; Varni, Burwinkle, Katz, Meeske, & thatstudyhadUHL,andtheirfatigueratingswereamong Dickinson, 2002; Varni, Burwinkle, & Szer, 2004). The themostsevereofanychildparticipant. PedsQL-MFS is a self-reportscaledesignedtoassessthree Recently,ourongoingresearchonlistening-related differentfatiguedomains(cognitive,sleep/rest,andgeneral) fatiguehasledustoholdinterviewsandfocusgroupsthat andhasbeencommonlyusedtoassessfatigueinchildren includedparentsofchildrenwithUHLandtheirteachers withavarietyofCHCs.Hornsbyandcoworkersfoundthat andclinicianstodiscussissuesaroundlistening-relatedfatigue. asmallgroupofschool-ageCHLreportedmorefatiguethan Reportsfromthesestakeholdershavebeeninsightful.Forex- anage-matchedcontrolgroupofchildrenwithoutHLacross ample,aparentofachildwithUHLstated,“Mydaughteris allthreedomains—althoughthedifferencesbetweengroups exhaustedmostdaysafterschoolorwhenshehastolisten werenotsignificantforthecognitivedomain.Theseverity foralongtime.”Aneducationalaudiologistofferedthese ofthefatigueinCHL,whencomparedtoexistingworkin comments:“OurkidswithUHLlossaresimilartochildren theliterature,wasequivalenttoorgreaterthanfatigueratings withmildtomoderatehearinglosses—theyrequireaudi- reportedbychildrenwithotherCHCs. torybreaksthroughoutthedayandstrugglemoreaca- In a follow-up to the aforementioned pilot study, demicallythanonewouldexpectgiventheirhearingloss.” Hornsby et al. (2017) collected PedsQL-MFS data on a Commentssuchasthesefromparentsandprofessionalsof- much larger population of school-age CHL (n = 60) with feradditionalsupportforthepremisethatchildrenwith mild to moderately severe hearing losses.Moreover, in UHLmaybecomefatiguedfollowingsustainedlistening additiontoobtainingdatafromthechildren,parentproxy demandsinschool—andsuchfatiguemayimpactonschool reportswerealsoobtained.Thesedatawerecomparedto performance. reportsfromacontrolgroup(n=43)ofchildrenwithout HL and theirparent proxies. Consistent withoutcomes APreliminaryExaminationofListening-RelatedFatigue fromthepilotstudy,resultsshowedthatCHLexperienced inCHLasReportedbytheChild,Parent,andTeacher significantlymoreoverallfatigueand cognitivefatigue Giventhescarcityofinformationrelatedtofatigue thanchildren withoutHL.Also,consistent withresults inchildrenwithUHL,weuseself-reportdatainthissection fromtheadult population (Hornsby & Kipp,2016),no toconductapreliminaryexplorationoflistening-related Bessetal.:Listening-RelatedFatigue 87 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions fatigueinchildrenwithunilateralandbilateralHL.Theself- thatshowtheseverityoflistening-relatedfatigueforagiven reportdatawerecollectedaspartofourongoingwork individual.Thus,anIRTscoreof0reflectsascoreequalto developingascaletoassesslistening-relatedfatiguein themeanscoreacrossallscalerespondents(i.e.,acrossall children.Someoftheinformationreportedhereisbased HLandnoHLgroups).Incontrast,anIRTscoreof–3and on a conferenceposter (Hornsby, Davis, Cho, & Bess, +3 reflects very low and very high ratings of listening- 2018). related fatigue, respectively. TheVFS:AToolforMeasuringListening-RelatedFatigue VFSDataCollectionandParticipants Althoughnearingcompletion,theVFSisstillbeing Potentialparticipantswererecruitedinpersonfollow- developed,andassuch,weonlybrieflydescribeherethe ingroutineaudiologicalappointmentsatourclinicaswell developmentprocessofthescalestodate.Currently,we asviasocialmediaandotheronlinerecruitmentportals.As areconductingvaliditystudiesonthreeversionsoftheVFS aresult,datacollectionwascompletedusingonlineversions —achild,aparent,andateacherversion.Todevelopthese oftheVFSandinpersonusinghardcopiesofthescales. scales,weusedinformationfrominterviews/focusgroups Parentsandteacherscompletedthescalesthemselves.Chil- (separategroupsofchildren,parents,andteacherspartici- drenalsocompletedthescalethemselveswhenpossible,al- pated)tocreatealargepoolofpotentialtestitems(157– thoughforyoungerchildren(<10yearsold)oranychild 212itemsdependingonthetargetrespondent).Usingbest whohadquestionsorneededassistance,theparent(orre- practicesandaniterativeprocess,includingfactoranaly- searchassistantifpresent)wasavailabletoreadthescale ses,classicaltesttheory,anditemresponsetheory(IRT), itemsaloudtothechildandansweranyquestions.Datafor weevaluateditemqualitytoreducethetotalnumberof allthreeversionsoftheVFSwerecollected,stored,and itemsdowntoeightto12,dependingonthescale.Factor managedusingResearchElectronicDataCapture(Harris analysesrevealedaunidimensionalstructureforthechild etal.,2009).ResearchElectronicDataCaptureisasecure, andteacherscales.However,theparentscalewasbestfit web-basedsoftwareplatformdesignedtosupportdatacap- asatwo-factormodelconsistingofaphysicalfactorand tureforresearchstudies.Allprocedureswerereviewedand asocial–emotional–cognitivefactor.IRTanalysesconfirmed approvedbyVanderbilt’sInstitutionalReviewBoard,and allscaleitemswerehighquality(i.e.,allitemshadhighin- allparticipantsprovidedinformedconsentorassentprior formationanddiscriminability).Theparentversioncontains tostartinganystudyprocedure. 12items,theteacherversionhaseightitems,andthechild Participantresponseswereobtainedfrom(a)399par- versionconsistsof10items.Allinstrumentsusea5-point entsofchildren(aged6–17years)with(n=263)andwith- Likertscaletoassessthefrequency(1=neverto5=almost out(n=136)HL,(b)137children(aged10–17years)with always)offatigue-relatedcomplaints.Examplesofitemsin- (n=98)andwithout(n=39)HL,and(c)363teachersof cludedinallthreeversionsoftheVFSandanadultversion students(aged6–17years)with(n=289)andwithout(n=70) (VFS-A)areshowninTable1.AllscalesallowforIRT HL.Notallparentsofthe137childrespondentscompleteda scoringandusingsummedLikert-scalescores.Inthisreview parentversionofthescale.Likewise,somechildrenofparents article,scaleresultswillbedescribedusingIRT,ratherthan whocompletedthescaledidnotcompleteachildversionof summedscores.AnIRTanalysisandscoringapproachhas thescale.Inthisreviewarticle,wedonotinvestigateparent severaladvantagescomparedtomoretraditionalscale proxyandchildagreementoragreementbetweenteacherre- assessmentmethods(seeHambleton&Swaminathan,2013, sponsesandparentorchild.Thosetopicswillbeexploredin forareview).Importantly,anIRTanalysismethodtakes futureworks.Thechild’sHLwasself-reportedbytheparent thesensitivityofeachtestitemintoaccountinthescoring andteacherasunilateralorbilateral.Degreeoflosswasself- process,providingamorepreciseestimateoftheunderlying reportedbythechild’sparentasmild/slight,moderate,severe, latentconstructbeingmeasured—inourcase,listening- orprofoundbasedontheirperceivedspeechunderstanding relatedfatigue(Embretson&Reise,2000).IRTscoresob- (WorldHealthOrganization,2019).Adetaileddemographic tainedusingtheVFSarestandardizedscores(likezscores) descriptionoftheparticipantsisprovidedinTable2. Table1.ExamplesofquestionnaireitemsfromtheVanderbiltFatigueScale(VFS)forlistening-relatedfatigueinchildren. VFS-A:Adultitems VFS-C:Childitems (cid:129) Ifeelwornoutfromeverydaylistening. (cid:129) Afterschool,I’msotiredIdon’twanttotalktoanyone. (cid:129) Ittakesalotofenergytolistenandunderstand. (cid:129) Listeningatschoolwearsmeout. VFS-P:Parentitems VFS-T:Teacheritems (cid:129) Itishardformychildtoconcentrateforalongtime. (cid:129) Whenthestudentgetstiredfromlistening,heorshe“checksout.” (cid:129) Mychildiscompletelywornoutafterlisteningfora (cid:129) Thestudentappearswornoutafterworkinghardtolistenallday. longtime. Note. ThethreeversionsoftheVFSforchildrenincludeachildself-report(VFS-C),aparentproxyreport(VFS-P),andateacherproxy report(VFS-T).SamplesfromtheadultversionoftheVanderbiltFatigueScale(VFS-A;Hornsbyetal.,2018)areprovidedasacomparison. Responsesareona5-pointLikertscalefromnevertoalmostalways. 88 Language,Speech,andHearingServicesinSchools (cid:129) Vol.51 (cid:129) 84–97 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Table2.ParticipantcharacteristicsforrespondentscompletingthevariousversionsoftheVanderbiltFatigueScaleforchildren. Variable Parentrespondents Childrespondents Teacherrespondents No.ofparticipants 399 137 363 Male/female/NR 209/189/1 73/64/0 186/169/8 Age(years) 10.7(3.4) 13.6(1.9) 10.4(3.3) HL/noHL/NR 263/136/0 98/39/0 289/70/4 UHL/BHL 64/199 28/70 40/249 Note. Gender,meanage(1SD),andhearingstatusofchildrenbeingreportedonareprovidedforallthreeparticipantgroups.NR=not reported;HL=hearingloss;UHL=unilateralhearingloss;BHL=bilateralhearingloss. VFSInitialAnalysesandResults reportedbyeachrespondentgroup(parents,children, Itisimportanttonotethatthedatausedintheanaly- andteachers).ThemeanIRTscoreforagroupofadults sesdescribedbelowwerenotcollectedforthepurposeofex- withHL,basedonresultsusinganadultversionofour aminingtheeffectsofunilateralorbilateralHLonlistening- listening-relatedfatiguescale,theVFS-A,isalsoprovided relatedfatigue.Rather,thesedatawerecollectedtoassistin asacomparison(Hornsbyetal.,2018). developingavalid,reliablescaleforassessinglistening- Amongparentrespondents,therewasasignificant relatedfatigue.Thus,theanalysesdescribedbelowareex- maineffectofgroup(F=24.5,p<.001).Posthoctesting, ploratoryinnature.Nonetheless,giventhepaucityofdata usingaseriesofBonferroni-adjustedttests,revealedsignifi- relatedtofatigueinCHL,wefeltsuchanalyseswouldbeof cantlyhigherIRTscores(higherfatigue)forbothHLgroups interesttocliniciansandcouldprovideusefulinformation compared to the control group without HL (t = –6.6, toguidefutureresearchinthisarea.Weusedasingle-factor p < .001 and t = –4.9, p < .001 for the bilateral adj adj analysisofvariance(ANOVA)tocompareIRTratingsof and UHL groups, respectively). Interestingly, parent rat- listening-relatedfatigueinchildrenwithself-reportedbilat- ings for children with unilateral and bilateral HL were eralandUHLtoacontrolgroupofchildrenwithnoHL. quite similar in magnitude and not significantly different SeparateANOVAswereconductedforeachrespondent (t = 0.019, p > .05). A significant main effect of group adj group—parent,teacher,andchildren.Inaddition,forthe wasalsoobservedforthechildratings(F=3.27,p<.05). two-factorstructurefortheparentscale,an“overall”fatigue Aswiththeparentdata,posthoctestingrevealedthatthe scorewascreatedbyaveragingtheIRTscoresofeach bilateralgroupreportedsignificantlyhigherlevelsoffatigue factor.Separateanalyseswereconductedoneachfactor thanthecontrolparticipants(t=–2.5,p <.05).However, adj scoreandontheoverallfatiguescore.Thestatisticalfind- thefatiguescoresforchildrenwithUHLfellbetweenthose ingswerethesameforeachanalysismethod,soonlythe ofthebilateralHLgroupandthecontrolgroupofchildren resultsforoverall(average)fatiguearereportedforthe withoutHL.Listening-relatedfatiguescoresforchildren parentdata. withUHLwerenotsignificantlydifferentthaneitherthe Figure1showsthemeanIRTscaledscoresforthose controlgroup(t=–0.9,p >.05)orthechildrenwith adj childrenwith(bilateralandunilateral)andwithoutHLas bilateralHL(t=–1.36,p >.05). adj Figure1.Meanitemresponsetheory(IRT)scalescoresofrespondentswithself-reportedunilateral,bilateral,andnohearingloss(HL) obtainedusingthreeversionsoftheVanderbiltFatigueScale(VFS)forassessinglistening-relatedfatigueinchildren(VFS-C,VFS-P,VFS-T). ThemeanscalescoresforadultswereobtainedusingtheVanderbiltFatigueScaleforAdults(Hornsbyetal.,2018)andareprovidedfor comparison.Solidlinesoverthebarsreflectsignificantdifferencesbetweengroups. Bessetal.:Listening-RelatedFatigue 89 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Finally,ananalysisofteacherratingsalsorevealeda Figure2.Percentageofchildrenwithunilateralandbilateral hearingloss(HL)exhibitingmoderate-to-severefatigue(item significantmaineffectofgroup(F=3.52,p<.05).How- responsetheoryscores>0.5)ascomparedtoagroupofchildren ever,posthoctestingusingaseriesofBonferroni-adjusted withoutHL.Solidlinesoverthebarsreflectsignificantdifferences ttestsrevealednosignificantdifferencesbetweenanyof betweengroups. thegroups(allp values>.05).Themaineffectappeared adj tobedrivenbythedifferenceintheteachers’fatiguerat- ingsforchildrenwithbilateralandUHL,notdifferences betweenCHLandthecontrolgroup.AsseeninFigure1, themeanfatigueratingsfromteachersforchildrenwith UHL were lower than that of the children without HL (control group) and the children with bilateral HL. We discuss this unexpected finding in more detail later in the reviewarticle. Additionally,recallthat HornsbyandKipp(2016) foundthat adults seeking help forHLweremuchmore likelytoreportseverefatigueandvigordeficitsthanexpected basedonnormativedata.Toexploretheissueofsusceptibility tomoreseverefatigueinchildren,weusedanIRTscore of>0.5asacut-pointtooperationallydefine“moderate- to-severe”listening-relatedfatigue.Wethencomparedthe prevalenceofmoderate-to-severefatigueinchildrenwith andwithoutHLacrossrespondentgroups.Todoso,we calculatedthepercentageofindividualswithandwithout HLineachrespondentgroupwhoreportedmoderate-to- severelistening-relatedfatigue.Althoughtheuseof0.5as acut-pointisarbitrary,forourpurposes,itseemsareason- ableestimatebecauseonlyabout31%ofthepopulation wouldbeexpectedtoreportgreaterfatigueproblems. Resultsforadults,parents(IRTscoreaveragedover Factors1and2),andchildrenareshowninFigure2.The teacherdataarenotplottedas,similartoourmeanresults, they showed no significant difference between groups. A series of chi-square analyses (see Table 3) showed that moderate-to-severe listening-related fatigue is much more common in adults and children with unilateral or bilateral HL when compared to those without HL. More specifi- cally, adults with bilateral and UHL were significantly (approximately nine times) more likely to report moderate- withUHL.Todoso,wereviewedanddiscussedexisting to-severe fatigue than adults without HL. A similar sig- workinthisarea.Ourreviewrevealedthatsystematicre- nificant finding, although smaller in magnitude, was searchonfatigueinchildrenwithUHLisscant.Themost observed for the parent proxy respondents. Parents of relevantinformationcomesfromstudiesexamininglistening- children with bilateral or UHL were approximately four relatedfatigueinchildrenandadultswithbilateralHL.While times more likely to report their children experienced limitedinscope,someresearchexamininglistening-related moderate-to-severe fatigue than parents of children with- fatigueinadultswithUHLandgeneralfatigueinchildren out HL. Finally, children with bilateral and UHL were withotherCHCsalsoprovidedrelevantinformation. approximately 2.5 and 2.0 times more likely, respectively, Giventhelimitedworkinthisarea,wealsoincluded to report moderate-to-severe fatigue than children without preliminaryanalysesofself-reportedlistening-relatedfatigue HL—although the difference for the children with UHL inCHL,includingUHL.Datafortheseanalyseswerecol- did not reach statistical significance. Importantly, there lectedaspartofaseriesofstudiestodevelopandvalidate were no significant differences in relative risk for moderate- asuiteofscalesforassessinglistening-relatedfatigue(the to-severe fatigue between children with unilateral and VFS)inchildren.Consideringtheavailableliteratureand bilateral HL (see Table 3). ourself-reportfatiguedataleadsustoconcludethatchil- drenwithunilateralandbilateralHLareatincreasedrisk forlistening-relatedfatigue.Moreover,themagnitudeof Discussion fatigueobservedinchildrenwithUHLappearstobesimilar Aprimarygoalofthisreviewarticlewastointroduce tothatexperiencedbychildrenwithbilateralHL—again theconstructoflistening-relatedfatigueandhighlightits highlightingthesignificantimpactofUHLinchildren.This importanceforCHL,withaparticularfocusonchildren reviewarticleisthefirsttoaddressthetopicofsubjective 90 Language,Speech,andHearingServicesinSchools (cid:129) Vol.51 (cid:129) 84–97 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Table3.Chi-squareanalysesofprevalenceof“moderate-to-severe”listening-relatedfatigueamongourhearingloss andnohearinglossgroupsaswellasbetweenrespondentswithbilateralandunilateralhearingloss. Variable χ2 p Prevalenceratio AdultswithnoHLvs.bilateralHL 106.1 <.001 9.2[5.1,16.6] AdultswithnoHLvs.unilateralHL 48.8 <.001 8.7[4.4,17.2] Adultswithbilateralvs.unilateralHL 0.1 .76 0.9[0.6,1.4] CNHLvs.CBHL 5.9 <.05 2.5[1.1,5.6] CNHLvs.CUHL 2.3 .13 2.0[0.8,5.0] CBHLvs.CUHL 0.5 .47 0.8[0.4,1.5] ParentsofCNHLvs.parentsofCBHL 32.1 <.001 3.8[2.2,6.4] ParentsofCNHLvs.parentsofCUHL 31.1 <.001 4.4[2.5,7.9] ParentsofCBHLvs.parentsofCUHL 1.2 .28 1.2[0.9,1.7] Note. Responseswereobtainedfromadults,children,andtheirparentsasproxyreporters.Valuesin[]showconfidence intervalsaroundtheprevalenceratio.Boldedvaluesshowsignificantdifferences.HL=hearingloss;CNHL=children withnohearingloss;CBHL=childrenwithbilateralhearingloss;CUHL=childrenwithunilateralhearingloss. fatigueinchildrenwithUHL.Clearly,additionalworkex- usedinthisanalysiswerebasedonresponsesfromall aminingthiscomplex,multifacetedconstructanditsrela- 60items.Incontrasttoourinitialanalysis,asingle-factor tionshiptoHLisneeded. ANOVAonthissmallsubsetofdata(comparingagiven Oneimportantareaofinquiryistobetterunderstand teachersIRTscoreforachildwithandwithoutHL) whyourcurrentteacherdatawerenotsensitivetotheeffects revealedasignificantmaineffectofgroup(F=10.7, ofHLonlistening-relatedfatigueinschool-agechildren p<.05).ThemeanIRTscoresprovidedbyteachersfor (seeFigure1).Thisdiscrepancycouldsimplysuggestthat theCHL(–0.10)weresignificantlyhigherthanthatof teachersarenotsufficientlyawareofthesignsandsymp- thechildrenwithoutHL(–0.71).Infact,theteacherIRT tomsoflistening-relatedfatigueinCHL.Suchanexplana- scoresforCHLwerehigherin16ofthe21cases. tion,however,isinconsistentwithanecdotalreportsand Theresult aboveisencouraging and suggests that commentsmadeduringourfocusgroupswithteachers, teachersmaybesensitivetotheproblemsoflistening-related educators,andsupportstaffwhoworkwithCHL.Inthese fatigueinCHL,atleastwhencomparingchildrenwithand discussions,reportsofthesubstantialdifficultieswith withoutHLwithintheirownclassroom.However,addi- listening-relatedfatigueamongCHLwerecommon—hence, tionalresearchisclearlyneeded.Forexample,itispossible theunderlyingreasonforthediscrepancyremainsunclear. thattheformatofthecurrentscalemaynotbeoptimalfor Althoughfocusgroupcommentssuggestotherwise, teacheruse.Scalemodifications,suchasspecifically ourreducedabilitytodetecttheeffectsofHLinourteacher instructingrespondentstofirstconsiderhowtheymight datacouldreflectalimitedinsightofthisgroupintothe respondiftheywereratingatypicallydevelopingchild fatigueoftheirstudents.Recallthatteachersmustprovide priortocompletingthescaleforanat-riskchild(e.g.,one proxyratingsofachild’sfatigue.Assuch,theirabilityto withHL),mayenhancethescale’sabilitytoidentifythose dosomaybelimitedbyhowmuchtheyinteractwiththe atmostrisk. childduringtheday.Unlikeparents,theteachermayonly Alternatively,factorsotherthanthescaleitselfcould seethechildforalimitedperiodoftimeeachday,poten- alsohaveinfluencedouroutcomes.Recallthatourteacher tiallylimitingtheirinsightintothefatigueofachild.Ifthis datawerecollectedforthepurposeofidentifyinghigh-quality werethecase,wemightexpectarepeated-measuresanaly- scaleitemsandvalidatingafinalversionofthescale—not sis,inwhichthesameteacherratesthefatigueofachild toempiricallyexaminetheeffectsofHLonself-reported withandwithoutHL,wouldbebettersuitedtodetecting fatigue.Tothisend,wespecificallycastawiderecruitment between-groupsdifferencesshouldtheyexist. netwithagoalofincludingrespondentswithawiderange Unfortunately,onlytwoteachersprovidedVFSratings offatigueratings.Itispossiblethatthesampleofchildren forachildwithandwithoutHLaspartofthevalidationdata ratedbyteachershadadditionalfactors,otherthanHL, collectiondescribedabove.Bothteachersreportedonchildren that may have affected their fatigue (e.g., some other withbilateralHL,andthoseoutcomesweremixedwith disability)—potentially masking the effects of HL. To oneteacherreportinggreaterfatigueforthechildwithHL be certain, we need to develop a better understanding of andtheotherreportingtheopposite.However,wewereable the factors responsible for the apparent limited sensitivity totestthishypothesisusingdatafromanearlierversion of the teacher scale. ofourscalethatwasobtainedfromasubsetofteachers whoprovidedratingsforbothchildrenwithandwithout HL(n=21).Thisearlyversionofthescalehadatotalof IdentificationandManagementofFatigueinCHL 60items.Althoughthescaleincludedthe10itemsused GiventhatchildrenwithUHLandbilateralHLare inourcurrentteacherversionoftheVFS,IRTscores atincreasedriskforlistening-relatedfatigue,anincreasingly Bessetal.:Listening-RelatedFatigue 91 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions importantroleforpediatricandeducationalaudiologistswill SuggestionsforFatigueInterventions betheidentificationandmanagementoflisteningeffortand Althoughevidence-basedinterventionprotocolsarenot fatigueinschool-agechildrenwithUHLaswellasthose presentlyavailable,someobviouscommonsensestrategies withbilateralHL.School-ageCHLareofteneligiblefor seemappropriateforimplementationintheschoolsetting. specializedinstructionundertheIndividualswithDisabil- Importantly,giventheriskforfatigueappearstobesimilar itiesEducationAct(Johnson&Seaton,2012). forthosewithunilateralorbilateralHL,thesuggested IfachilddoesnotqualifyforanIndividualizedEdu- intervention/managementstrategiesthatappearbelow cationProgram(IEP),theymaybeconsideredfora504plan couldbeappropriateforallCHL.Manyofthesestrategies undertheSection504oftheRehabilitationActof1973,an weresuggestedanddiscussedduringourfocusgroups antidiscriminationcivilrightsstatutethatrequiresthatthe withteachersandotherschoolserviceproviders. needs of students with disabilities be met as adequately astheneedsofchildrenwithoutdisabilities(Zirkel,2009). ClassroomStrategies Bothplansrequireeducationteammemberstoholistically Theclassroomisoftenadifficultlisteningenviron- reviewachild’sperformanceintheclassroomandconsider mentforCHL,asthespacesdonotmeetrecommended potentialaccommodationstoprovideequalcommunica- standardsfornoiselevelsandreverberationtimes(Gremp tionaccesstoclassroominstruction. &Easterbrooks,2018;Knecht,Nelson,White,&Feth, Asapartofspecializedinstruction,ateamofprofes- 2002).Evenwhenutilizingproperlyfittedamplification, sionals(educationalaudiologists,certifiedteachersofthe CHLmaystillstruggleinthissetting.CHLmaystruggle hearingimpaired,andspeech-languagepathologists)work tolistenandcomprehendinnoisysettingsandhavediffi- togetherwithCHLandtheirfamiliestosupportacademic cultylocalizingtalkersduringgroupdiscussions. success.Theycollaboratetodeterminewhetherthechild Ourfocusgroupdiscussionswithteacherswhowork should receivepreferentialseating,is meeting auditory withCHLrevealedthattheeducationalteammayconsider goals in their IEP,and is appropriatelyutilizinghearing- severalanecdotalinterventionswhenachildwithhearing assistivetechnology,ifrecommended.Inaddition,theteam lossdemonstratesbehaviorstheybelievetobefatiguerelated. reviewsthechild’seducationalprogressperiodicallyand Forexample,focusgroupparticipantsnotedthatCHLtend considersadditionalfactorsthatmaybeimpactingeduca- tobe“morezonedout”comparedtotypicalhearingpeers, tionalprogress. especially“towardtheendoftheschoolday.”Hence,itwas Insuchreviews,theconceptoffatigueisfrequently notsurprisingtolearnthatmanyteachersandclinicians discussedandoftenconsideredintheeducationalprogram- reportedarrangingtheircaseloadschedulestoaccommodate mingofCHL;however,systematicidentificationandman- CHLinthemorningtoreducethepossibleeffectsoffatigue agementprotocolsarenotavailable.Themostlogicalway onachild’stherapysessions.TohelpCHLcopewithfa- todetermineachild’sriskforfatigueisforeducational tigue,teachersreportedinterventions,suchasallowingthe audiologists,speech-languagepathologists,andteachers childtoremovetheirhearing-assistivetechnologyforshort tolookforsymptomsandbehaviorscommonlyassociated listeningbreaksorschedulingconsistent“movementbreaks,” withfatigueinchildren.Ourresultssuggestthatteachers wherethechildcouldmoveandstretch,intotheroutineof andsupportstaffmayneedtobetrainedtoidentifypromi- theclassroom. nentcharacteristicsoflistening-relatedfatigue(seeFatigue MostCHLareprovidedwithflexible,preferential Educationsectionbelow).Suchcharacteristicswouldinclude seating(i.e.,seatingthatallowsforthebestaccesstoboth reportsorbehaviorsindicativeoftiredness/weariness,mood visualandauditoryinformation)intheclassroomsetting. changes,sleepiness(especiallyinthemorning),adecrease ForchildrenwithUHL,preferentialseatingmayneedto instaminaorenergy,oralackofdesiretocontinuewitha bereviewedbasedontheactivityoftheclassroom;how- task.Iffatigueissuspectedforagivenchild,theaudiologist ever,thenormalhearingearshouldtypicallybefacedto- orspeech-languagepathologistwouldadministeravalidated wardthespeakerofinterest. fatiguemeasuretothechildand/orparentandteacher.This typeofmeasurecouldalsobeincorporatedintoexisting ListeningBreaks (ornew)self-advocacycompetencycheckliststobeused Toourknowledge,thereisnoresearchspecifically duringIEP/504meetings.Examplesoffatiguescalesfor focusedonusinglisteningorrestbreakstoreducefatiguein childrenincludethePedsQL-MFS(Varnietal.,2002),the school-ageCHL.Despitethisabsence,discussionsamongour ChildhoodFatigueScale(Hockenberryetal.,2003),andthe focusgroupparticipantssuggestitisacommonstrategy soon-to-be-availableVFS,whichspecificallyassesseslistening- ofcliniciansandteacherswhoworkwithCHL.Whilere- relatedfatigue.Thesestandardizedmeasuresprovidesup- searchonrestbreaksinCHLisnonexistent,thereisalarge portforanecdotalevidencethataCHLmaybestruggling relatedliteraturefromstudiesofdrivingbehavior,flight withfatigueintheclassroomsetting,animportantfactor simulation,andworkforce,suggestingrestbreaksarean whendeterminingspecializedservicesintheschoolsetting. effectivetoolforreducingfatigueanditsnegativeeffects. Whilethisisalogicalapproach,additionalresearchis Intheworkforce,forexample,restbreaksareimportantfor neededtoevaluatethesensitivityandbenefitsofsuchan increasingperformance,avoidingaccidents,andpromoting approachtoidentificationoflistening-relatedfatiguein goodhealth(Fritz,Lam,&Spreitzer,2011).Manystudies children. havereportedthatrestbreaks,especiallyshortrestbreaks, 92 Language,Speech,andHearingServicesinSchools (cid:129) Vol.51 (cid:129) 84–97 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions havemanypositivebenefits,includingdecreasesinfatigue, devices)toprovideaccesstoauditoryinformation.Forchil- stress,anddiscomfortandincreasesinvitalityandtimespent drenwithUHL,theseamplificationoptionsaswellasa ontask(Arlinghausetal.,2012;Henning,Jacques, Kissel, contralateralroutingofsignalmayberecommended.Personal Sullivan, & Alteras-Webb, 1997;Tucker,2003;Zacher, devicesaimtoimproveaudibilitysothatachildisbetterable Brailsford,&Parker,2014).Forexample,Zacheretal.(2014) tohearandcommunicatewiththosearoundhim.Clinical studiedtheimpactofmicrobreaksonfatigueandvitalityin audiologistsoftenmaketherecommendationfor“full- agroupofworkingadults(N=124).Usingadiarystudy timeuseofamplification,”which,foratypicalschool-age design,theyshowedthatmicrobreaksresultedinadecrease child,wouldrangefromapproximately10to14hrperday infatigueandanincreaseinvitality. (Walkeretal.,2013).Somecliniciansandteachersbelieve Consistentwiththisrelatedresearch,someteachers thatwell-fittedamplificationhelpstoreducefatigueeffects reportthatallowingachilda“listeningbreak”—theoppor- duetoalongdayoflisteningintheclassroom.However, tunitytostepawayfromtheclassroomforafewminutesto evidencesupportingsuchanassumptionislimited.Hornsby gototherestroomorrestinanotherteacher’sclassroom— (2013)istheonlystudytodatetoexaminethisempirically, allowsthechildtocomebacktotheactivityrefreshedand andparticipantsinthatstudywereadults.Participantswere preparedforre-engagement.However,thereisnoconsen- requiredtocompleteacognitivelydemandinglisteningtask susconcerningthenumberofrestbreaksorthedurationof forapproximately60minwithoutabreak.Testingwascom- breaksthatshouldbeused.Itisanticipatedthattherecov- pletedwithandwithouthearingaids.Resultsshowedasmall erytimeneededbyastudentmayvary,basedonthechild’s benefitofhearingaidsforreducinglisteningeffortandmen- age,additionaldisabilities,orotherfactors.Furthermore, talfatigue—definedasadecrementincognitiveprocessing thereisnovalidatedmethodforteachersandcliniciansto speedovertime.Specifically,whenwearinghearingaids, knowwhenachildrequiresinterventionorhowtosystem- participantswerebetterabletomaintainvigilantattention aticallyprovidetheintervention. duringthesustainedlisteningtask.However,subjective ratingsofeffortandfatiguewerenotsignificantlydifferent FatigueEducation intheunaidedandaidedconditions. Althoughfatigueanditsnegativeeffectsarepervasive, Inaddition,recentevidencefromfocusgroupsand notallindividualslivingorworkingwithCHLareaware interviewswithadults suggests that extended deviceuse ofthepotentialsideeffectsthatsustainedfatiguecanhave itself can befatiguing(Holman,Drummond,Hughes,& onanindividual’soverallwell-being.Inaddition,ourinter- Naylor,2019;Hughes,Hutchings,Rapport, McMahon, viewdatawithCHLsuggestthatmany(evenmiddleand & Boisvert,2018).Similarcommentsweremadebysome highschoolers)areunawareoftheirpresentationofcom- CHLduringfocus groupandinterviewsconducted as monfatiguesymptomsascomparedtotheirpeers.Infocus part of our fatiguescaledevelopmentproject.Themixed groupsandinterviews,someteenagersdeniedanyfatigue- findings related to benefitsfrom deviceusemay beduein relatedproblems,whiletheirparents,whoalsoparticipated parttothelimitationsofhearingaidsandcochlearimplants inseparatefocusgroups,reportedsignificantdifficultieswith aloneinnoisyenvironments.Moreover,notallCHLwear listening-relatedfatigueintheirCHL.Onechildreported theiraidswhileattendingschool—24%ofchildren(6.5– “neverfeelingwornoutfromlistening”whilehermother 12.9yearsold)withmild-to-moderatelossesdonotuse sharedthatthischildoften“camehomefromschool,took theirpersonaldevicesonaday-to-daybasis(Gustafson, offherimplants,”and“laiddownfor30minutesorsowith Davis,Hornsby,&Bess,2015). thelightsoff.”AyoungadultwithHLsharedthatitwas Inadditiontopersonalhearingaidsandcochlearim- not“untilthecaseloadofcollege”thatshewasabletolook plants,accesstoimportantclassroominformationcanbe backandrecognizehowmuchlistening-relatedfatigueaf- achieved byusing apersonalremotemicrophone(RM) fectedherthroughheradolescentyears. system.RMsystemseffectivelyimprovesthesignal-to-noise Thesefindings suggestthatadditionaleducation ratioandallowsthechildtoheartheteacher’svoicemore aboutlistening-related fatigueanditsnegativeeffects is clearlyinthepresenceofexcessiveclassroombackground requiredto informandequip CHL,parents,andschool noise(Wolfe,Morais,Schafer,Agrawal,&Koch,2015). personnelworkingwith this population(i.e.,teachersand ThebenefitsofRMtechnologyforCHLarewellrecog- therapists).Structured educationalprograms that target nized,andthesesystemsarewidelyacceptedintheeduca- theidentificationofcommonsymptomsassociated with tionalsetting(Anderson&Goldstein,2004).Incontrast, fatigueandprovideguidelinesforidentificationandman- researchexaminingthebenefitsofRMsystemsforreduc- agement,includinginterventionstrategies,arelikelytoim- ingfatigueislimited.However,arecentstudybyRance, proveoutcomesforCHL.Additionally,schoolprofessionals, Saunders,Carew,Johansson,andTan(2014)suggests audiologists,andparentsshouldworkwithCHLtohelp thatRMsystemsmayhelptoreducestressassociatedwith themidentifysymptomsassociatedwithlistening-related difficultlisteningconditions,thuspotentiallyreducingfa- fatigueandself-advocatefortheirlisteningneeds. tigue.Inaddition,someprofessionalsworkingwithCHL intheschoolsettingreportobservingadecreaseinfatigue HearingDevicesandAssistiveTechnology symptomswhenthestudentusesanRMsysteminthe CHLaretypicallyprescribedpersonalamplification classroom;however,systematicresearchinsupportofthese devices(i.e.,hearingaids,cochlearimplants,bone-anchored accountsislacking. Bessetal.:Listening-RelatedFatigue 93 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Vanderbilt University - Library, Peri Rcvng on 02/26/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions