ebook img

ERIC ED602955: The Tip-of-the-Tongue State and Curiosity PDF

2017·0.69 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED602955: The Tip-of-the-Tongue State and Curiosity

Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Cognitive Research: Principles DOI10.1186/s41235-017-0065-4 and Implications ORIGINAL ARTICLE Open Access The tip-of-the-tongue state and curiosity Janet Metcalfe1* , Bennett L. Schwartz2 and Paul A. Bloom1 Abstract Theories of study time allocation and of curiosity suggest thatpeople are most engaged withand want to devote their time to materials that are not completely mastered but also are not so difficult that theymight be impossible. Their curiosityis thought to be triggered by items that are almost known, or are in what is sometimes called the region ofproximal learning. Answers thatare onthetip-of-the-tongue (TOT)—not immediately recallable but nevertheless evoking a feeling of imminent recall—seem, intuitively, to be materials that have this characteristic of being almost, but not quite, fully known. We therefore,hypothesized that people would be particularly curious to seethe answers to questions for which theanswers were on thetips of their tongues. Totest the TOT curiosity hypothesis, wegave participants 82 general information questions and quickly asked whether theanswerswere or were not on thetips of their tongues and whether they wanted to see theanswers later. Overwhelmingly, items that were accompanied by a TOT feeling were those which evoked participants’curiosity, regardless of whether the feeling occurred in conjunction with anerror of commission, an error ofomission, or even with thecorrect answer. Keywords: Tip-of-the-tongue,Curiosity, Google, Metacognition, Region ofproximal learning Significance feeling states may be epiphenomenal—having no func- This article investigates metacognitive conditions that tion in human cognition. By contrast, our results indi- induce people’s need to know. An mTurk survey re- cate that the special feeling of mild torment that is the vealed that people think they are most curious—opera- hallmark of theTOTstate is a goad to epistemic action. tionalized, in the survey, as having the strongest urge to Ittriggerspeople’sneedtoknow. Google the answer—when they do not know the answer but not when they know the answer. However, they think they are indiscriminate among ‘not known’ states: Background when they are sure they do not know, when they have a Most studies of the TOT state focus on either the cha- feeling that they might be able to recognize the answer, racteristics of the information pertaining to the not-yet- and when they are in a genuine ‘tip-of-the-tongue’ retrieved items—such as the first letter, the number of (TOT) state. To ascertain whether they were correct syllables, the gender of the word in certain languages, or that all ‘don’t know’ states were equal, or whether there incorrect words called ‘blockers’ (Brown, 1991, 2012; is a special metacognitive state associated with curiosity, Gollan & Brown, 2006; Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006a; we gave participants 82 general information questions, Miozzo & Caramazza, 1997)—or they focus on the com- asked them to give the answers, asked them whether pelling phenomenology of the state (Cleary & Claxton, they were in aTOTstate, and then asked them whether 2015; Schwartz & Cleary, 2016). With respect to the they wanted to see the answer. They overwhelmingly latter, researchers often include William James’ (1890, wanted to see the answer when they were in a TOT p. 251) description “The state of our consciousness is state. This occurred regardless of what their response peculiar. There is a gap therein; but no mere gap. It was, including when they had been correct, had made a is a gap that is intensely active. A sort of wraith of commission error, or had made an omission error. Phi- the name is in it, beckoning us in a given direction, losophers have sometimes suggested that metacognitive making us at moments tingle with the sense of our closeness and then letting us sink back without the longed-for term,” R. Brown and McNeill’s (1966, p. 326) *Correspondence:[email protected] comment that people who are “seized by a TOTstate … 1ColumbiaUniversity,NewYork,NY,USA Fulllistofauthorinformationisavailableattheendofthearticle would appear to be in mild torment, something like the ©TheAuthor(s).2017OpenAccessThisarticleisdistributedunderthetermsoftheCreativeCommonsAttribution4.0 InternationalLicense(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/),whichpermitsunrestricteduse,distribution,and reproductioninanymedium,providedyougiveappropriatecredittotheoriginalauthor(s)andthesource,providealinkto theCreativeCommonslicense,andindicateifchangesweremade. Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page2of8 brink of a sneeze,” or A. Brown’s (1991) acknowledgment most effective learning algorithm involved having people of “personal introspections of inner turmoil when grap- selectively study the ‘T’ items. Metcalfe and Kornell plingforanelusiveword.”Normally,whenindividualsare (2003, 2005) called T items those in the Region of engaged in nonpathological cool cognition, they are not Proximal Learning (RPL). In addition, they found that seized by ‘inner turmoil,’ or by any kind of ‘torment.’ But college students selectively chose these particular items TOTstates are different. They arise into consciousness as for further study. Loewenstein (1994), like Berlyne a distinct emotional configuration that appears to be (1954), also zeroed in on such items in his theory of universally recognizable (Schwartz, 1999). The question curiosity, pointing to them as being the items people that underlies the research in the present article is why found particularlyinterestinganddeservingofattention. this particular metacognitive feeling state surfaces into The items that one finds on the tip of one’s tongue fit consciousnessasadistinctiveandemotionalstate:whatis the characterization of ‘T’ items or ‘RPL’ items very well. itsfunction? They are not so easy that they can be readily recalled, Schwartz and Cleary (2016; and see Metcalfe & but they are not so difficult that they are completely un- Schwartz, 2015; Schwartz & Metcalfe, 2011) have known and have no resonance. Indeed, some partial and suggested that the TOT state may have an adaptive, semantic information appears to be retrievable for these evolutionary purpose. They have focused on the finding items, making them ideal candidates for optimal lear- that people persevere in their efforts at recall when they ning. It is possible that the feeling associated with being are in a TOTstate: the metacognitive feeling appears to in aTOTstate is a marker of these special items and has prompt people to continue trying. Furthermore, most a function of provoking curiosity and inducing the per- TOTstates are eventually successfully resolved, although sontoepistemicaction. with obvious difficulty. Even so, the difficulty of the Theaction involved ininformationseeking variesfrom retrievalitself isthought toplayarole inenhancinglater situation to situation. A person may just keep trying to memory (see, e.g., Bjork & Linn, 2006)—a consequence retrieve rather than giving up, may look up an answer in that has beneficial effects for the individual’s later a dictionary, or may ask a friend or teacher. Often performance. The present study takes this idea a step Google is the option of choice. To investigate whether further in suggesting that the TOTstate prompts curio- theTOTstate isperceivedashavingsome special role in sity, which could entail more than just a retrieval inducingsuchaction,we asked 127 participants(61 male, attempt—the feeling state may entail general informa- 66 female; mean age=40.6, SD=11.6) on MTurk, the tion seeking behavior. followingquestion: Weiner (2006), indescribing the tenants of his attribu- “You have the strongest urge to Google the answer to tion theory, argued that consciously experienced feelings aquestionthathasbeen posedtoyouwhen: (i.e., emotions) are ‘goads to action.’ Consistent with this view, we propose that the conscious emotional instan- (a)youaresureyouknowtheanswer,andcanproduceit, tiation of the TOT state has a function of inducing the (b)youaresure youdon’tknowthe answer andcan’t experiencer to the action of trying and persisting in produceit, efforts to gain the sought-for information. It induces an (c)youhavea‘feelingofknowing,’thatis,youthink you eagerdesiretoknow:curiosity. couldprobablyrecognizethe answer ifitwere Several precursors in the literature provide convergent shown toyou., support for the idea that the TOT state may be related (d)youareina‘tip-of-the-tongue’state, thatis,youare to curiosity and information seeking. First, going back as unable tothink oftheanswer,now,butfeelsure that far as Berlyne’s (1954) theory of epistemic curiosity, the- youknow itandthat itisontheverge ofcoming orists have proposed that people’s need to know is ig- back toyou.” nited by materials that are neither completely unknown nor completely known, but are, instead, in an intermedi- Only 25% of our respondents chose the TOT option ate zone (Atkinson, 1972; Kornell & Metcalfe, 2006b; ‘d’; 39% chose ‘b’ and 31% chose ‘c’. Very few people Litman, 2009; Loewenstein, 1994; Metcalfe, 2002). These (5%)wantedtoknowtheanswerwhentheythoughtthey theories indicate that people are best off, as far as learn- already knew it. Not knowing was associated with infor- ing goes, if they study items that are at an intermediate mation seeking. But people did not appear to prioritize state of learning. For example, Atkinson (1972) among ‘not known’ states. They said they had the urge employed an early computerized learning program to Google when they did not know at all and when they which sorted items into those that were reliably remem- had a feeling of knowing, as frequently as when they bered, intermediate items that were somewhat learned were in aTOTstate. The mTurk survey, of course, may but not permanently (which he called ‘T’ or transitional or may not reflect what people choose to do when items),andthose thatwereunlearned. Hefound thatthe actuallyinsuchstates. Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page3of8 The only empirical investigation that we were able to low or high. The materials were also divided into two find of the relation between people’s curiosity and their sets, because some questions are more likely to evince TOT states was that by Litman, Hutchins, and Russon TOTstates than others. (2005). They gave participants 12 general-information Finally, we asked participants to report TOTstates for questions to answer in a self-paced manner, and had all responses, even those that were correct. Although them indicate for each whether they knew the answer, most studies look only at TOT experiences when the they did not know the answer, or they were in a TOT person has made an omission error, TOT states some- state. They then asked for intensity or confidence ratings. times occur even when people have made a commission Personality questionnaires were administered, allowing error (they may be in what is sometimes called a examination of trait curiosity. For some participants a ‘blocked’ state, and retrieve the blocker as an error) or forced-choicerecognitiontestwasgiven.Attheendofthe even when they were correct (presumably if they had experiment, the participants were provided with sealed low confidence about their answer). We were interested envelopes with the 12 questions written on the outside in all of these TOT states. Our primary question, andtheanswersenclosed.Theresearchersinvitedpartici- throughout, was the relation of TOTstates to curiosity: pants to open any envelopes they wished. They reported did people want to see the answers more when they that people were more likely to open the envelopes of were inaTOTstatethan whentheywere not? questionstowhichtheyhadbeeninaTOTstate. Although the results of this experiment are supportive Experiment of a link between TOTstates and curiosity, by the time Method the choices were made many, if not most, TOT expe- Participants riences would probably already have subsided (see The 46 participants (20 male, 25 female, one who did Brown & Croft, 2014). To overcome this problem, in the not answer) were Columbia University and Barnard experiment that follows we asked participants whether College undergraduates, ranging in age from 18 to 29 they wanted to see the answer to the questions the mo- years (M=19.96; SD=2.32). They received partial ment after they gave their TOT judgments for each course credit for participating in the experiment. All question, while they were still in the state. Furthermore, procedures were in accordance with the ethical princi- TOTstates are rare—occurringin thenaturalflow of life ples of the APA, and were approved by the Columbia (see Brown, 2012) only about once per week, and in a UniversityInternalReviewBoard(IRB-AAAD4902). laboratory setting about 10–20% of the time for omis- sion errors. With only 12 questions, there probably were not many TOT experiences per person in Litman et al.’s Materials The stimuli were 82 general information questions taken (2005) study. The frequency was not reported. To accu- from the Nelson and Narens (1980) norms, and updated mulate enoughTOT experiences to allow more detailed and corrected (see, Metcalfe, Casal-Roscum, Radin, & analyses, we used 82 questions rather than just 12. We Friedman, 2015). For example, one question was “What also speeded responding to each question. TOT expe- is the name of the ancient warrior who was dipped in riences often exist in the first few seconds of attempted the River Styx?” The questions were displayed on an retrieval, but then, upon successful retrieval, they dissi- iMac desktopcomputer withSuperLab4.5software. pate. Instead of waiting for this to happen, we pushed people to give aTOTjudgment quickly, while they were still in the unresolved state. This meant that the experi- Design menter had to run each participant in person, and The experiment was 2 x 2 mixed design, with factors enforce timing of the responses. He or she read each Memory Load (Load or No Load, within participant) question aloud and then allowed only 5 s before the and Group (1 or 2, between participants). The questions participant had to say whether or not he or she was in a (Set A) that were in the No Load condition in Group 1 TOT state. Our procedure resulted in a considerably were in the Load condition in Group 2; the questions higher proportion of TOT states than is typically ob- that were in the No Load condition in Group 2 (Set B) served in self-paced experiments. In addition, we used a were in the Load condition in Group 1. Twelve ques- load manipulation on half of the questions because tions were reserved to be in the same load conditions working memory load, in some but not all experiments in both groups, allowing us to look at overall group (e.g., Schwartz’s, 2008, Experiments 1, 2 and 3 showed differences. On these 12 questions, no significant the effect whereas Experiment 4 did not), has been between-group differences in the probability correct, shown to influence the probability of TOT experiences. the probability of commission errors, or the probability We wanted to see whether curiosity was evoked by TOT of TOT experiences were found. The order of presenta- states regardless of whether their overall probability was tion of questions was randomized for each participant. Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page4of8 Procedure and the probability of an omission error was 0.54 Participants were instructed that they would be given a (SD=0.16). The overall probability of being in a TOT series of general information questions to try to answer state was 0.24 (SD=0.15) and all participants exhi- and, while trying, to say (a) whether they were in aTOT bited TOT states, but not in all treatment combina- state or not and (b) whether they wanted to see the tions (which will account for differences in degrees of answer later or not. They also were informed that they freedom shown in some of the following analyses). would later be able to see the correct answer for up to 10% of the questions. We included (but did not enforce) Omissionerrors this restriction because in pilot testing with no restriction When we examined the probability of TOT experiences many participants had stated that they wanted to see the givenomissionerrors,whichisthestandard analysis, there answers to all of the questions. The 10% ‘rule’allowed us wasnoeffectofeitherGrouporofMemoryLoad,butthere to investigate differences depending upon whether people wasaninteractionbetweenthem,F(1,44)=9.19,p=0.004, wereinaTOTstateornot.Participantsweretoldthaton η2 ¼0:173, power=0.84. This interaction appeared to be p halfofthequestionstheywouldseea stringoffourdigits duetoadifferenceinTOTexperiencesforSetAandSetB. before the question itself was presented, whereas half of Set A was in the No Load condition for Group 1 (with a the time they would see asterisks. If a string of numbers TOT probability of 0.36) and in the Load condition for appeared they would be required to recall those digits Group 2 (p=0.35). Set B was in the Load condition for after answering the general information question and Group1(p=0.28)andintheNoLoadconditionforGroup giving their judgments. When asterisks appeared, they 2 (p=0.30). This same Group×Memory Load interaction wouldlaterhavetosayithadbeenasterisks. wasshowninthedataonthesimpleprobabilityofwanting Participants nearly always knew what aTOTstate was toseelater,F(1,44)=4.87,p=0.03, η2 ¼0:1,power=0.58. (also see Schwartz & Metcalfe, 2011). If, however, a par- p Peopletendedtowanttoseetheanswersmoreinthecon- ticipant did not, the experimenter explained it as a state ditionsinwhichthereweremoreTOTexperiences. in which “you feel sure you know the answer and think you can get it—it is imminent—but you cannot think of itatthemoment.”Participantscompletedapractice trial ChoicetoseelaterwheninaTOTstateornot(omission errors) before theexperimentbegan. We treated being or notbeing inaTOTstate as if it was First, the asterisks or numbers appeared, followed by a an independent variable and computed the conditional 5-sdelay.Then,thegeneralinformationquestionappeared probability of choosing to see later as the dependent onscreen and was read aloud by the experimenter, who variable in a 2 (TOT or not)×2 (Group)×2 (Memory immediately upon finishing hit a key to start the 5-s re- Load) design. Neither Group nor Memory Load, nor the sponse period. During this period,the experimenter typed in the participant’s answer, if any. At the end of the 5 s, interaction, was significant. Indeed, the only thing that wassignificantwaswhetherthepersonwasinaTOTstate participantsindicatedwhethertheywereinaTOTstateor or not, F(1, 43)=42.68, p=0.001, η2 ¼0:50, power=1.0. not. The computer then immediately asked whether they p wanted to see the answer later. The participant then People were about twice as likely to want to see the recalled the four digits or that it had been asterisks, and answer later when they were in a TOT state (0.45) than theprogramthenwentontothenexttrial. when they were not (0.24). This difference, as shown in At the end of the experiment, after filling out a demo- Fig. 1, was evident for every group and for every load graphics sheet, participants were thanked, debriefed, condition/questionset. given course credit for participation, and allowed to see theanswers toallofthegeneralinformation questions. Commissionerrors Commission errors are interesting because TOT expe- Results riences sometimes occur when people are in a ‘blocked’ In this experiment, people wanted to see the answers state, or when they have only partial information, such more when they were in a TOTstate (0.44) than when as the first letter. Such responses would have been clas- they were not in a TOT state (0.18), t(45)=8.06, p< sified as commission errors here. There were no effects 0.001. This is the main result of interest, and was ob- of either Group or of Memory Load, nor was there an tainedinallconditionsthatwewereabletoexamine,as interactionbetweenGroup andMemory Load. will be illustrated shortly. There were other differences, The simple probability of wanting to see answers later, however, as indicated in the following data are provided however, indicated an interaction between Group and intheAdditionalfile1. Working Memory Load (which may have been a bias to- Theprobability ofa correct answer was0.26 (SD=0.15), ward Set A—as noted earlier), F(1, 44)=3.92, p=0.054, theprobabilityofacommissionerrorwas0.20(SD=0.09), η2 ¼0:08,power=0.49. p Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page5of8 Fig.1Probabilityofanswerseekinggiventhattheresponsewasanomissionerror.Errorbars:standarderrorofthemean.TOT:tip-of-the-tongue ChoicetoseelaterwheninaTOTstateornot ChoicetoseelaterwheninaTOTstateornot(onall (commissionerrors) errors) For this analysis, we looked at the subset of data in As would be expected, because the separate error-type whichtheinitialresponsetothequestion wasacommis- analyses were significant, when we collapsed over all sion error—a response that was either partial or wrong errors there was a significant main effect for being in a (note that ‘don’t know’ responses were counted as omis- TOTstateonchoicetoseelater,F(1,44)=54.65,p<0.001, sion errors)—and computed the conditional probability η2 ¼0:55, power=1.0. No other main effects or interac- p of wanting to see later as a function of whether the per- tionsweresignificant. son was in a TOT state or not, conducting a 2×2×2 ANOVA with factors TOT, Group, and Memory Load. Correctresponses There was a significant effect of TOTstate: people more In this experiment, participants were not given feedback often chose to see the answer later when they were in a about whether their responses were correct or incorrect, TOT state than when they were not, F(1, 19)=8.44, and we observed a number of cases in which people p=0.009, η2 ¼0:308, power=0.78. No other effects expressed that they were in a TOT state even though p or interactions were significant. The conditional pro- theyhad,infact,giventhe correctanswer.Toourknow- bability of choice to see later for each of the eight ledge, this phenomenon has not been observed pre- cells for commission errors is shown in Fig. 2. viously. The dynamics of uncertainty resolution and the Fig.2Probabilityofanswerseekinggiventhattheresponsewasacommissionerror.Errorbars:standarderrorofthemean.TOT:tip-of-the-tongue Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page6of8 feeling of TOT are not well understood. However, it is TOTstate. Thus, although theTOTstate affects people’s plausible that a person might feel they were in a TOT pursuit of the answers, people are not aware that it does state if they were uncertain about their correct response. so. Interestingly, the effect of theTOTstate on informa- In that case, these responses, like TOT responses made tion seeking occurred at a variety of different levels of after either omission or commission errors, should be TOT experiences, for both groups, and regardless of indicative thatpeople were in their RPL—close to having whether people were in a memory load condition or not. the right answer—and might evoke curiosity. Accor- It occurred regardless of the type of error people had dingly, weexaminedthe correctresponses. made—omission or commission. Indeed, it even oc- There was an effect of Working Memory Load, F(1,44) curred when people had been correct. We conclude that =8.89, p=0.005, η2 ¼0:17, power=0.83, showing that thenagging feeling of beinginthisparticularmetacogni- p people were more likely to be in aTOTstate when they tive stateisagoadtoepistemic action. had been in the Load condition than in the Asterisks There remain many unaddressed complexities and un- condition. There was also an interaction between Group answered questions. Curiosity is often taken to be a and Memory Load, F(1, 44)=35.18, p<0.001, η2 ¼0:44, pleasant experience—the happy state of an enquiring p mind. This perspective, concerning the positive emo- power=1.0,whichwas,again,probablyduetosetdifficulty. tional quality of curiosity, contrasts starkly with the nag- ging, tormented quality so frequently ascribed to TOT ChoicetoseelaterwheninaTOTstateornot(correct states. Interestingly, Litman, Crowson and Kolinski responses) (2010; and see Litman, 2009) have proposed that there As found earlier, the only effect that was significant may be two kinds of curiosity, which they call interest- was whether the person was in a TOT state or not, based curiosity, which is largely positive, and F(1, 12)=8.24, p=0.014, η2 ¼0:407, power=0.749. p deprivation-type epistemic curiosity, which is not. The People were more likely to want to see the answer need to know induced by the TOTstate examined here later when they had been correct but were in a TOT appears to be of the latter type, although more studies state (0.24) than when they were correct but not in a are needed to investigate the nuances of this emotional TOT state (0.03). This difference, as shown in Fig. 3, distinction. To complicate matters even further, the tor- was evident for every group and for every load condi- mented feeling that accompanies the TOTstate prior to tion/question set for which we had data. resolution may be modulated by anticipation of the highly positive emotionally charged feelings that arise Conclusion upon solution—feelings of pleasure that may be similar The results of this experiment indicated that when to the delight experienced when a person solves an people were in aTOTstate they wanted to see the cor- insight or a magic problem (see, e.g., Danek, Fraps, von rect answer more than when they were not in a TOT Muller, Grothe, & Ollinger, 2014; Hedne, Norman, & state. The results of our mTurk survey indicated that Metcalfe, 2016), or experiences a scientific epiphany. people do not realize there is any difference in their own The motivation to overcome the torment may go curiosity between merely not knowing and being in a hand in hand with the anticipation of the intense Fig.3Probabilityofanswerseekinggiventhattheresponsewascorrect.Errorbars:standarderrorofthemean.TOT:tip-of-the-tongue Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page7of8 pleasure felt when the state is resolved. Both may Received:25November2016Accepted:25April2017 contribute to people’s curiosity. Sometimes metacognitive states have been thought to be epiphenomenal—having no ramifications for cogni- References Atkinson,R.C.(1972).Ingredientsforatheoryofinstruction.American tion. The TOT data presented in this article contravene Psychologist,27,921–931. this conjecture: the presence of the TOT feeling state Berlyne,D.E.(1954).Atheoryofhumancuriosity.BritishJournalofPsychology, has implications for people’s choice to seek more infor- 45,180–191. mation—a propensity with deep consequences for our Bjork,R.A.,&Linn,M.C.(2006).Thescienceoflearningandthelearningof science:Introducingdesirabledifficulties.AssociationforPsychologicalScience curiosity, for our engagement in learning, and, ulti- Observer,19,1–2. mately, forourunderstanding. Brown,A.S.,&Croft,K.(2014).Thereitisagainonmytongue:Trackingrepeat TOTs.InB.L.Schwartz&A.S.Brown(Eds.),Tipofthetonguestatesand relatedphenomena(pp.32–49).Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. Additional file Brown,A.S.(1991).Areviewofthetip-of-the-tongueexperience.Psychological Bulletin,109,204–223. Brown,A.S.(2012).Tipofthetonguestate.Hove,UK:PsychologyPress. Additionalfile1:Data.(XLSX264kb) Brown,R.,&McNeill,D.(1966).The‘tipofthetongue’phenomenon.Journalof VerbalLearningandBehavior,5,325–337. Cleary,A.M.,&Claxton,A.B.(2015).Thetip-of-the-tongueheuristic:Howtip-of- Abbreviation the-tonguestatesconferperceptibilityoninaccessiblewords.Journalof TOT:Tip-of-the-tongue ExperimentalPsychologyLearningMemoryandCognition,41,1533–1539. Danek,A.H.,Fraps,T.,vonMuller,A.,Grothe,B.,&Ollinger,M.(2014).It’sakindof Acknowledgements magic—Whatself-reportscanrevealaboutthephenomenologyofinsight TheauthorsthankTealEich,RachelBurris,KarenKellyandJudyXu.They problemsolving.FrontiersinPsychology,5,1408. haveallowedustothankthem. Gollan,T.H.,&Brown,A.S.(2006).Fromtip-of-the-tongue(TOT)datato theoreticalimplicationsintwosteps:WhenmoreTOTsmeansbetter Funding retrieval.JournalofExperimentalPsychology:General,135,462–483.doi:10. TheauthorsaregratefultotheInstituteofEducationScience(R305A150467) 1037/0096-3445.135.3.462. andJamesS.McDonnellFoundationCollaborativegrant(ledbyRoddy Hedne,M.R.,Norman,E.,&Metcalfe,J.(2016).Intuitivefeelingsofwarmthand Roediger,#484800),withJM,asColumbiaPI,fortheirsupport.Theauthors confidenceininsightandnoninsightproblemsolvingofmagictricks. aresolelyresponsibleforthecontentofthisarticle. FrontierinPsychologySectionCognitiveScience,7(1314),1–13.doi:10.3389/ fpsyg.2016.01314. Availabilityofdataandmaterials James,W.(1890).Principlesofpsychology.NewYork:Holt. DataforindividualsubjectsandinsummaryformareavailableinanExcel Kornell,N.,&Metcalfe,J.(2006a).‘Blockers’donotblockrecallduringtip-of-the- filesupplementarytothisarticle.Rawdata,intextfiles,fromthecomputer- tonguestates.MetacognitionandLearning,1,248–261. generatedsubjectfilesaswellassourceprogramsareavailableonrequest Kornell,N.,&Metcalfe,J.(2006b).Studyefficacyandtheregionofproximal fromtheauthors. learningframework.JournalofExperimentalPsychologyLearningMemoryand Cognition,32,609–622. Authors’contributions Litman,J.A.,Hutchins,T.L.,&Russon,R.K.(2005).Epistemiccuriosity,feeling-of- knowing,andexploratorybehaviour.CognitionandEmotion,19,559–582. JMandBLSconceived,planned,anddesignedtheexperiment.JMoversaw Litman,J.A.(2009).Curiosityandmetacognition.InC.B.Larson(Ed.), theconductoftheexperimentwhichwasruninherlaboratory,trainedthe Metacognition:NewResearchDevelopments(pp.105–116).Hauppauge,NY: experimenters,supervisedthecodingofthedataandtheanalysis,and NovaSciencePublishers. wrotetheinitialdraft.BLSrevisedtheinitialdraftandcontributedtothe Litman,J.A.,Crowson,H.M.,&Kolinski,K.(2010).Validityoftheinterest-and programmingandanalysisoftheexperiment.PABranthesubjectsforthe deprivation-typeepistemiccuriositydistinctioninnon-students.Personality secondhalfoftheexperiment(Group2),codedandcheckedthedatafrom andIndividualDifferences,49,531–536. therawcomputeroutputs,contributedtotheanalyses,createdthefigures, Loewenstein,G.(1994).Thepsychologyofcuriosity:Areviewand andwroteadraftofthemethodssection.Dataanalysiswasconductedby reinterpretation.PsychologicalBulletin,116,75–98. TealEich(seeAcknowledgements).Allauthorsreadandapprovedthefinal Metcalfe,J.(2002).Isstudytimeallocatedselectivelytoaregionofproximal manuscript. learning?JournalofExperimentalPsychology:General,131,349–363.doi:10. 1037/0096-3445.131.3.349. Competinginterests Metcalfe,J.,Casal-Roscum,L.,Radin,A.,&Friedman,D.(2015).Onteachingold Theauthorsdeclarethattheyhavenocompetinginterests. dogsnewtricks.PsychologicalScience,26,1833–1840. Metcalfe,J.,&Kornell,N.(2003).Thedynamicsoflearningandallocationofstudy Consentforpublication timetoaregionofproximallearning.JournalofExperimentalPsychology: Notapplicable. General,132,530–542.doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.530. Metcalfe,J.,&Kornell,N.(2005).Aregionofproximallearningmodelofstudy Ethicsapproval timeallocation.JournalofMemoryandLanguage,52,463–477. AllprocedureswereinaccordancewiththeethicalprinciplesoftheAPA, Metcalfe,J.,&Schwartz,B.L.(2015).Theghostinthemachine:Self-reflectiveconsciousness and were approved by the Columbia University Internal Review Board andtheneuroscienceofmetacognition.InJ.Dunlosky&S.K.Tauber(Eds.),TheOxford (IRB-AAAD4902). Participants signed informed consent. HandbookofMetamemory(pp.407–424).Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress. Miozzo,M.,&Caramazza,A.(1997).Retrievaloflexical–syntacticfeaturesintip-of- the-tonguestates.JournalofExperimentalPsychologyLearningMemoryand Publisher’s Note Cognition,23,1410–1423. SpringerNatureremainsneutralwithregardtojurisdictionalclaimsinpublished Nelson,T.O.,&Narens,L.(1980).Normsof300general-informationquestions: mapsandinstitutionalaffiliations. accuracyofrecall,latencyofrecall,andfeeling-of-knowingratings.Journalof VerbalLearning&VerbalBehavior,19,338–368. Authordetails Schwartz,B.L.,&Cleary,A.M.(2016).Tip-of-the-tonguestates,déjàvuandother 1ColumbiaUniversity,NewYork,NY,USA.2FloridaInternationalUniversity, metacognitiveoddities.InJ.Dunlosky&S.Tauber(Eds.),OxfordHandbookof Miami,FL,USA. Metamemory(pp.95–108).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress. Metcalfeetal.CognitiveResearch:PrinciplesandImplications_#####################_ Page8of8 Schwartz,B.L.(1999).Sparklingattheendofthetongue:Theetiologyand phenomenologyoftip-of-the-tonguestates.PsychonomicBulletin& Review,6,379–393. Schwartz,B.L.(2008).Workingmemoryloaddifferentially affectstip-of-the- tonguestates and feeling-of-knowing judgment. Memory & Cognition, 36, 9–19. Schwartz,B.L.,&Metcalfe,J.(2011).Tip-of-the-tongue(TOT)states:Retrieval, behavior,andexperience.Memory&Cognition,39,737–749. Weiner,B.(2006).Socialmotivation,justice,andthemoralemotions:Anattributional approach.Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaum. Submit your manuscript to a journal and benefi t from: 7 Convenient online submission 7 Rigorous peer review 7 Immediate publication on acceptance 7 Open access: articles freely available online 7 High visibility within the fi eld 7 Retaining the copyright to your article Submit your next manuscript at 7 springeropen.com

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.