ebook img

ERIC ED438185: Student Expectations in Introductory Physics. PDF

19 Pages·2000·0.38 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED438185: Student Expectations in Introductory Physics.

DOCUMENT RESUME SE 063 214 ED 438 185 Redish, Edward F.; Saul, Jeffery M.; Steinberg, Richard N. AUTHOR Student Expectations in Introductory Physics. TITLE 2000-00-00 PUB DATE 19p.; Originally published in the American Journal of NOTE Physics, v66 p212-24, 1998. Tests/Questionnaires (160) Reports Research (143) PUB TYPE MF01/PC01 Plus Postage. EDRS PRICE *Classroom Environment; Higher Education; *Physics; Science DESCRIPTORS Education; Scientific Principles; *Student Attitudes; Student Interests; *Student Reaction; Student Surveys; *Teacher Expectations of Students; Teacher Student Relationship ABSTRACT Students' understanding of what science is about and how it is done and their expectations as to what goes on in a science course play a powerful role in what they can get out of introductory college physics. This is particularly true when there is a large gap between what the students expect to do and what the instructor expects them to do. This paper describes the Maryland Physics Expectations (MPEX) Survey, a 34-item Likert-scale survey that probes student attitudes, beliefs, and assumptions about physics. The results of pre- and post-instruction delivery of this survey to 1500 students in introductory calculus-based physics at 6 colleges and universities are presented. Findings indicate a large gap between the expectations of experts and novices and a tendency for student expectations to deteriorate rather than improve as a result of a semester of introductory physics. (Contains 36 references.) (Author/WRM) Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. Student Expectations in Introductory Physics Edward F. Redish, Jeffery M. Saul, and Richard N. Steinberg Department of Physics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742 Students' understanding of what science is about and how it is done and their expectations as to what goes on in a science course play a powerful role in what they can get out of introductory college physics. This is particularly true when there is a large gap between what the students expect to do and what the instructor expects them to do. In this paper, we describe the Maryland Physics Expectations (MPEX) Survey; a 34- item Liken-scale (agree-disagree) survey that probes student attitudes, beliefs, and assumptions about phys- ics. We report on the results of pre- and post-instruction delivery of this survey to 1500 students in introduc- tory calculus-based physics at six colleges and universities. We note a large gap between the expectations of experts and novices and observe a tendency for student expectations to deteriorate rather than improve as a result of a semester of introductory physics. Other questions, such as what happens over the longer I. INTRODUCTION term and how items of various clusters correlate with each other, are left for future publications. What students expect will happen in their introductory We begin by reviewing previous work on the subject in calculus-based (university) physics course plays a criti- section II. The structure and validation of the survey is It affects cal role in what they learn during the course. described in section III. Section IV contains the results what they listen to and what they ignore in the firehose of the survey for five calibration groups, ranging from of information provided during a typical course by in- novice to expert. The results of our survey with students It af- structor, teaching assistant, laboratory, and text. are presented in section V, and section VI discusses the fects which activities students select in constructing implications of our work. their own knowledge base and in building their own un- derstanding of the course material. II. BACKGROUND AND REVIEW OF This paper explores student attitudes and beliefs about PREVIOUS WORK university physics and how those attitudes and beliefs change as a result of physics instruction. In this paper, Maryland Physics Expectations (MPEX) we present the A. Recent Progress in Physics Education: Survey, a Likert-style (agree-disagree) questionnaire we Teaching Concepts have developed to probe some aspects of student expec- In the past fifteen years, there has been a momentous tations. We have used this survey to measure the distri- change in what we know about teaching and learning in bution of student attitudes at the beginning and end of the introductory calculus-based physics course. In about the first semester of calculus-based physics at six col- 1980, research began to show that the traditional class leges and universities. Our survey is included as an ap- leaves most students confused about the basic concepts pendix. of mechanics.I Subsequent work extended those obser- Because so little is known about the distribution, role, vations to other areas including optics, heat and thermo- and evolution of student expectations in the university In studying dynamics, and electricity and magnetism.2 physics course, many questions can be asked. To limit student understanding of the basic concepts of physics, the scope of this paper, we restrict ourselves to three much has been revealed about what students know and questions. how they learn. The crucial element is that students are not "blank slates." Their experience of the world (and Q1. How does the initial state of students in univer- of school) leads them to develop many concepts of their sity physics differ from the views of experts? own about how the world functions. These concepts are often not easily matched with those that are being taught Q2. To what extent does the initial state of a class in physics courses, and students' previous conceptions vary from institution to institution? may make it difficult for them to build the conclusions the teacher desires. However, it has been demonstrated Q3. How are the expectations of a class changed as that if this situation is taken into account, it is often pos- the result of one semester of instruction in vari- sible to provide activities that induce most of the stu- ous learning environments? dents to develop a good functional understanding of many of the basic concepts.3 PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS Office of Educational Research and Improvement BEEN GRANTED BY EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION 1 CENTER (ERIC) midis document has been reproduced as received from the person or organization BESTCOPYAVAILABLE originating it. Minor changes have been made to improve reproduction quality. TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES 2 INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC) Points of view or opinions stated in this 1 document do not necessarily represent official OERI position or policy. Success in finding ways to teach concepts is an excellent memorizing long lists of uninterpreted facts or perform- start (even though the successful methods are not yet ing algorithmic solutions to large numbers of problems without giving them any thought or trying to make sense widespread), but it does not solve all of our teaching of them. Although some students consider this efficient, problems with physics. We want our students to develop it is only efficient in the short term. The knowledge a robust knowledge structure, a complex of mutually supporting skills and attitudes, not just a patchwork of thus gained is superficial, situation dependent, and ideas (even if correct). We want them to develop a quickly forgotten. Our survey is one attempt to cast light on the hidden curriculum and on how student ex- strong understanding of what science is and how to do it. We want them to develop the skills and confidence pectations are affected by instruction. needed to do science themselves. Previous Research on Cognitive Expectations C. Student Expectations B. There are a number of studies of student expectations in science in the pre-college classroom that show that stu- It is not only physics concepts that a student brings into the physics classroom. Each student, based on his or her dent attitudes towards their classroom activities and own experiences, brings to the physics class a set of atti- their beliefs about the nature of science and knowledge affect their learning. Studies by Carey6, Linn, and oth- tudes, beliefs, and assumptions about what sorts of things they will learn, what skills will be required, and ers have demonstrated that many pre-college students what they will be expected to do. In addition, their view have misconceptions both about science and about what they should be doing in a science class. Other studies at of the nature of scientific information affects how they interpret what they hear. In this paper, we will use the the pre-college level indicate some of the critical items phrase expectations to cover this rich set of understand- that make up the relevant elemerits of a student's system ings. We focus on what we might call students' cogni- of expectations and beliefs. For example, Songer and expectations about their under- Linn studied students in middle schools and found that tive expectations standing of the process of learning physics and the they could already categorize students as having beliefs structure of physics knowledge rather than about the about science that were either dynamic (science is un- derstandable, interpretive, and integrated) or static (sci- content of physics itself. ence knowledge is memorization-intensive, fixed, and Our model of learning4 is a growth model rather than a not relevant to their everyday lives)." Alan Schoenfeld knowledge-transfer model. It concentrates on what has described some very nice studies of the assumptions happens in the student, rather than what the teacher is high schools students make about learning mathemat- doing. We therefore have chosen to focus our study on ics.9 He concludes that "Student's beliefs shape their cognitive attitudes that have an effect on what it is stu- behavior in ways that have extraordinarily powerful dents choose to do, such as whether they expect physics (and often negative) consequences." to be coherent or a loose collection of facts. The specific Two important large scale studies that concern the gen- issues our survey covers are discussed in detail in the next section. Other issues, such as students' motivation, eral cognitive expectations of adult learners are those of Perry") and Belenky et al. (BGCT)" Perry tracked the preferences, feelings about science and/or scientists, etc. attitudes of Harvard and Radcliffe students throughout are important but have been probed extensively else- their college career. Belenky et al. tracked the views of where.5 women in a variety of social and economic circum- Although we don't often articulate them, most physics stances. Both studies found evolution in the expectations instructors have expectation-related goals for their stu- of their subjects, especially in their attitudes about dents. In our university physics course for engineers knowledge.I2 Both studies frequently found their young and other scientists, we try to get students to make con- adult subjects starting in a "binary" or "received knowl- nections, understand the limitations and conditions on edge" stage in which they expected everything to be true the applicability of equations, build their physical intui- or false, good or evil, etc., and in which they expected to tion, bring their personal experience to bear on their learn "the truth" from authorities. Both studies ob- problem solving, and see connections between classroom served their subjects moving through a "relativist" or physics and the real world. We refer to this kind of "subjective" stage (nothing is true or good, every view a goal not listed in the course's syllabus learning goal has equal value) to a "consciously constructivist" stage. or the textbook's table of contents as part of the In this last, most sophisticated stage, the subjects ac- course's "hidden curriculum." We are frustrated by the cepted that nothing can be perfectly known, and ac- tendency many students have to seek "efficiency" to cepted their own personal role in deciding what views achieve a satisfactory grade with the least possible effort were most likely to be productive and useful for them. often with a severe unnoticed penalty on how much they learn. They may spend a large amount of time Redish, Steinberg, and Saul Student Expectations 2 3 mester of their university physics course, tracking their Although these studies both focused on areas other than progress through detailed problem-solving interviews.16 science," most professional scientists who teach at both Each student was interviewed for approximately 10 the undergraduate and graduate levels will recognize a hours. The interviews were taped and transcribed, and binary stage, in which students just want to be told the students were classified according to their statements "right" answers, and a constructivist stage in which stu- and how they approached the problems.I7 However, dents take charge of building their own understanding. conducting interviews with large numbers of students Consciously constructivist students carry out their own would be prohibitively expensive, and they are unlikely evaluation of an approach, equation, or result, and un- to be repeated at many institutions. Interviews therefore derstand both the conditions of validity and the relation cannot yield information about the distribution of stu- to fundamental physical principles. Students who want dent expectations in a large population. In order to to become creative scientists will have to move from the study larger populations, a reliable survey is needed binary to the constructivist stage. This is the transition which can be completed by a student in less than half an that we want to explore. hour and analyzed by a computer. We developed the An excellent introduction to the cognitive issues in- Maryland Physics Expectations (MPEX) survey to meet volved is given by Reif and Larkin" who compare the this need. spontaneous cognitive activities that occur naturally in everyday life with those required for learning science. B. The Development of the MPEX Survey They pinpoint differences and show how application of We began to develop the MPEX survey in the Autumn everyday cognitive expectations in a science class causes difficulties. Another excellent introduction to the cogni- of 1992 at the University of Washington. Students in the introductory calculus-based physics class were given tive literature on the difference between everyday and in-school cognitive expectations is the paper by Brown, a variety of statements about the nature of physics, the Collins, and Duguid, who stress the artificiality of much study of physics, and their relation to it. They rated these statements on a five point scale from strongly dis- typical school activity and discuss the value of cognitive agree (1) to strongly agree (5). Items for the survey apprenticeships." were chosen as a result of a detailed literature review, All the above-cited works stress the importance of ex- discussions with physics faculty, and our combined 35 pectations in how teens and young adults make sense of years of teaching experience. The items were then vali- their world and their learning. If inappropriate expecta- dated in a number of ways: by discussion with other fac- tions play a role in the difficulties our students com- ulty and physics education experts, through student in- monly have with introductory calculus-based physics, we terviews, by giving the survey to a variety of "experts", need to find a way to track and document them. and through repeated delivery of the survey to the same group of students. III. Constructing the Survey The MPEX survey has been iteratively refined and im- plemented through testing in more than 15 universities A. Why a Survey? and colleges during the last four years. The final ver- sion of the survey presented here has 34 items and typi- Our interactions with students in the classroom and in cally takes twenty to thirty minutes to complete. We re- informal settings have provided us with preliminary in- port here on the results of the MPEX survey given at six sights into student expectations. As is usual in physics colleges and universities to more than 1500 students. A education research, repeated, detailed, taped and tran- list of the institutions that have participated is shown in scribed interviews with individual students are clearly Table 1. All students were asked to complete the survey the best way of confirming or correcting informal obser- during the first week of the term'" (semester or quarter) vations and finding out what a student really thinks. and at the end of the term. The education literature contains particularly valuable transcripts of student interviews, especially in the work In the rest of this section, we describe how we chose the of David Hammer. In his Ph.D. thesis at Berkeley, items of the survey and how we validated it. Hammer followed six students throughout the first se- Student Expectations Redish, Steinberg, and Saul 3 4 whether it is useful to think about them together; Choosing the Items of the MPEX Survey The cognitive structures that we have referred to as beliefs about the role of mathematics Math Link 5. "student expectations" clearly are complex and contain in learning physics whether the mathematical many facets. We decided to focus on six issues or di- formalism is used as a way of representing informa- mensions along which we might categorize student atti- tion about physical phenomena or mathematics is tudes towards the appropriate way to do physics. Three just used to calculate numbers; of these are taken from Hammer's study and we have added three of our own. Effort beliefs about the kind of activities and 6. Building on the work of Perry and Songer and Linn work necessary to make sense out of physics cited earlier, Hammer proposed three dimensions along whether they expect to think carefully and evaluate which to classify student beliefs about the nature of what they are doing based on available materials learning physics:I9 and feedback or not. Independence beliefs about learning physics 1. The extreme views associated with each of these vari- whether it means receiving information or involves ables are given in Table 2. We refer to the extreme view an active process of reconstructing one's own un- that agrees with that of most mature scientists as the ex- derstanding; pert or favorable view, and the view that agrees with that of most beginning students as the novice or unfa- beliefs about the structure of physics Coherence 2. vorable view. The survey items that have been selected as a collection of isolated pieces or as knowledge to probe the six attitudes are given in the right hand col- a single coherent system; umn of the table. We refer to the collection of survey items designed to probe a particular dimension as a clus- Concepts beliefs about the content of physics 3. ter. Note that there is some overlap, as these dimen- as formulas or as concepts that knowledge sions are not independent variables.20 underlie the formulas. Although we believe the attitudes that we have defined as expert correspond to those attitudes needed by most In the MPEX survey, we seek to probe three additional creative, intuitive, and successful scientists, we note that dimensions: they are not always predictors of success in introductory beliefs about the connection be- Reality Link 4. physics classes. In an earlier study, Hammer studied whether physics is un- tween physics and reality two students in the algebra-based physics course at related to experiences outside the classroom or 2t One student possessed many novice charac- Berkeley teristics but was doing well in the course. The other stu- dent possessed many of the characteristics preferred by experts but was having trouble. The second student's Instructional Institution N desire to make sense of the physics for herself was not Characteristics supported and she did not begin to succeed until she switched her approach to memorization and pattern 445 traditional lectures, some University of Maryland, College Park (UMCP) classes with group-learning matching. In this case the course supported an attitude tutorial instead of recitation, and an approach to learning that most physics instruc- no lab tors would not endorse and one which certainly would 467 traditional lectures, with University of Minnesota, cause her trouble if she were to try to take more ad- Minneapolis (UMN) group-learning research de- vanced science courses.22 signed problem-solving and labs One can imagine exploring a wide variety of character- traditional lectures, group- 445 Ohio State University, istics ranging from whether the students like physics to Columbus (OSU) learning research designed whether they are intimidated by physics to whether they problem-solving and labs think they should take notes in lecture. In creating the Dickinson College (DC) Workshop Physics 115 MPEX survey, we have chosen to focus on issues that have an effect on how students interpret and process the a small public liberal arts Workshop Physics 12 university (PLA) physics in the class. We have not considered the stu- dent's feelings about physics, its value or its importance. 44 a medium sized public traditional two-year college (TYC) Table I: Institutions from which first semester or quarter pre- and post-instruction survey data was collected. All data is matched, i.e., all students included in the reported data completed both the pre- and post-instruction surveys. Student Expectations Redish, Steinberg, and Saul 4 BEST COPY AVAILABLE 5 perception of the learning characteristics described in Validating the Survey: Interviews Table 2 differs from the way that student actually func- We conducted more than 100 hours of videotaped stu- tions, the self-perception has a strong tendency to be dent interviews in order to validate that our interpreta- closer to the side chosen by experts. We therefore feel tion of the survey items matched the way they were read that while survey results for an individual student may and interpreted by students. We asked students (either be misleading, survey results of an entire classroom individually or in groups of two or three) to describe might understate unfavorable student characteristics. their interpretations of the statements and to indicate why they responded in the way that they did. In addi- tion, students were asked to give specific examples from IV. EXPERT EXPECTATIONS: class to justify their responses. THE CALIBRATION GROUPS From these interviews, we have found that students are In order to test whether the survey correctly represents not always consistent with their responses and ap- elements of the hidden curriculum, we gave it to a vari- proaches to what appear to us to be similar questions ety of students and physics instructors. We defined as and situations. We feel that this does not represent a "expert" the response that was given by a majority of failure of the survey, but properly matches these stu- experienced physics instructors who have a high con- dents' ill-defined understanding of the nature of physics. cern for educational issues and a high sensitivity to stu- One reason for this was described by Hammer. He ob- dents. We conjectured that experts, when asked what served that some students in his study believed that pro- answers they would want their students to give, would fessional physicists used the favorable conditions, but respond consistently. that it sufficed for them to behave in the unfavorable fashion for the purposes of the course. He referred to A. The Calibration Groups this by adding the marker "apparent" to the characteris- We tested the response of a wide rangeof respondents tic. This is only one aspect of the complex nature of by comparing five groups: human cognition. We must also be careful not to as- sume that a student exists in one extreme state or an- Group 1: engineering students entering the calcu- 1. other. A student's attitude may be modified by an addi- lus-based physics sequence at the U. of Maryland, tional attitude, as in Hammer's observations, or even ex- ist simultaneously in both extremes, depending on the Group 2: members of the US International Physics 2. situation that triggers the response.23 One must there- Olympics Team fore use considerable care in applying the results of a limited probe such as our survey to a single student. Group 3: high school teachers attending the two- 3. week Dickinson College Summer Seminar on new We are also aware that students' self-reported percep- approaches in physics education tions may not match the way they actually behave.24 However, the interviews suggest that if a student's self- MPEX Items Favorable Unfavorable independence takes responsibility for constructing takes what is given by authorities 1, 8, 13, (teacher, text) without evaluation own understanding 14, 17, 27 believes physics can be treated as 12, 15, 16, coherence believes physics needs to be consid- 21, 29 ered as a connected, consistent unrelated facts or "pieces" framework 4, 19, 26, stresses understanding of the under- focuses on memorizing and using concepts 27, 32 lying ideas and concepts formulas reality link believes ideas learned in physics believes ideas learned in physics 10, 18, are relevant and useful in a wide has little relation to experiences 22, 25 variety of real contexts outside the classroom math link considers mathematics as a conven- views the physics and the math as 2, 6, 8, independent with little relationship ient way of representing physical 16, 20 between them phenomena effort does not attempt to use available in- 3, 6, 7, makes the effort to use information available and tries to make sense of formation effectively 24, 31 it Table 2: Dimensions c f student expectations Redish, Steinberg, and Saul Student Expectations 5 6 sity teachers attending the two-week summer seminar D D D D 28 19 10 (N=56), and group 5 college and university teachers A D 20 D D 29 2 11 implementing Workshop Physics in their classroom D D A (N=19). The teachers in group 5 were committed to im- 30 3 21 12 plementing an interactive engagement model of teach- D D D A 4 22 31 13 i ing in their classroom. We asked the three groups of in- A D D A 23 32 5 14 structors to respond with the answer they would prefer We expected these five groups to their students to give. D 24 1 A D D 6 33 15 show an increasing level of agreement with answers we (A) (A) D A 25 34 7 16 preferred. = D A D 26 8 17 B. The Responses of the Calibration Groups A (D) 9 27 18 1 I The group we expected to be the most sophisticated, the Table 3: Prevalent responses of our expert group. Where the group 5 instructors, agreed strongly as to what were the respondents did not agree at the >80% level, the item is responses they would like to hear from their students. shown in parentheses and the majority response is shown. On all but three items, 80% or more of this group The response "A" indicates agree or strongly agree. The re- agreed with a particular position . Three items, num- sponse "D" indicates disagree or strongly disagree. bers 7, 9, and 34, had a strong plurality of agreement, Group 4: university and college teachers attending 4. but between X and X of the respondents chose neu- the two-week Dickinson College Summer Seminar tral. We define the preferred response of group 5 as the on new approaches in physics education We define a response in agreement expert response. with the expert response as and a response in favorable Group 5: college faculty who are part of a multi- 5. disagreement with the expert response as unfavorable. university FIPSE-sponsored project to implement For the analysis in this paper, the agree and strongly Workshop Physics at their home institutions. agree responses (4 and 5) are combined, and the dis- agree and strongly disagree responses (1 and 2) are The University of Maryland students are a fairly typical combined. A list of the favorable responses to the sur- diverse group of engineering students at a large research vey items is presented in Table 3. university. The entering class average on the FCI is around 50%, comparable to the average for introductory To display our results in a concise and easily interpret- The number of students in university physics classes.2 able manner, we introduce an (A-D) agree-disagree the sample is N=445. In this plot, the percentage of respondents in each plot. group answering favorably are plotted against the per- The US International Physics Olympics Team (USIPOT) centage of respondents in each group answering unfa- is a group of high school students selected from appli- vorably. Since the fraction of students agreeing and dis- cants throughout the USA. After a two week training agreeing must add up to less than or equal to 100%, all session, five are chosen to represent the US in the Inter- points must lie in the triangle bounded by the corners national Physics Olympics. In 1995 and 1996, this (0,0), (0,100), (100,0). The distance from the diagonal group trained at the University of Maryland in College line is a measure of the number of respondents who an- Park and we took the opportunity to have them complete swered neutral or chose not to answer. The closer a survey forms. The total number of respondents in this point is to the upper left corner of the allowed region, group is N=56. Although they are not teachers, they the better the group's agreement with the expert re- have been selected by experts as some of the best high 26 sponse. school physics students in the nation. Our hypothesis was that they would prove to be more expert than the The results on the overall survey are shown in Fig. 1. In average university physics student, but not as expert as this plot, the percentages are averaged over all of the our groups of experienced instructors. items of the survey, using the preferred responses of calibration group 5 as favorable. The groups' responses The physics instructors who served as our test groups are distributed from less to more favorable in the pre- were all visiting Dickinson College. Attendees came dicted fashion.27 from a wide variety of institutions. Many have had con- siderable experience in teaching, and all of them were Although the overall results support our contention that sufficiently interested in educational development to at- our survey correlates well with an overall sophistication tend a workshop. We separated them into three groups: of attitudes towards doing physics, the cluster results high school teachers attending a two-week group 3 show some interesting deviations from the monotonic ordering. These deviations are quite sensible and sup- summer seminar (N=26), group 4 college and univer- Redish, Steinberg, and Saul Student Expectations 6 7 port our use of clusters as well as overall results. In or- pect their students to have. der to save space and simplify the interpretation of re- sults, we present the data in Table 4. Displayed in this V. STUDENT EXPECTATIONS: table are the percentages of each group's favorable and DISTRIBUTION AND EVOLUTION unfavorable responses (in the form favorable / unfavor- able). The percentage of neutrals and not answering can In this section, we discuss the results obtained from giv- be obtained by subtracting the sum of the favorable and ing the MPEX survey at the beginning and end of the unfavorable responses from 100. first term of introductory calculus-based physics at six different institutions. In each case, the subject covered From the table we see that most of the fraction of re- was Newtonian mechanics. The schools involved in- spondents agreeing with the favorable response tends to clude the flagship research institutions of three large decrease monotonically from group 1-5 with a few inter- state universities: the University of Maryland (UMCP), esting exceptions. The high school teachers (group 3) Ohio State (OSU), and Minnesota (UMN); plus three are farther than their average from the favorable corner smaller schools: Dickinson College (DC), a small public in the coherence and math clusters, while the Physics liberal arts college (PLA), and a public two year college Olympics team is closer to the favorable corner in those (TYC). At the named colleges, we have data from mul- categories than their average. These results are plausi- tiple instructors. In the case of the last two institutions, ble if we assume that high school teachers are less con- data was only collected from a small number of instruc- cerned with their students forming a coherent and a tors and students. These are included in order to dem- mathematically sophisticated view of physics than are onstrate how the MPEX survey can be used as a diag- university teachers. The results also agree with our per- nostic tool, but are kept anonymous to protect the iden- sonal observations that the members of the USIPOT are tity of the instructors and institutions involved. unusually coherent in their views of physics and excep- tionally strong in their mathematical skills. At Maryland, Ohio State, and Minnesota, classes were presented in the traditional lecture-lab-recitation frame- Note also that the Olympics team results are very far work with some modifications. At Maryland, there is no from the favorable corner in the effort cluster. The main laboratory in the first semester and some of the recita- discrepancies are in items 3 and 7. We suggest that the tion sections were done with University of Washington reader peruse the survey items of that cluster (3, 6, 7, style tutorials.28 At Minnesota, the laboratory and reci- 24, 31). These items represent highly traditional meas- tations involve carefully designed group work.29 At ures of effort (reading the textbook, going over one's lec- Ohio State, lectures are traditional but are enhanced by ture notes) which we conjecture are not yet part of the use of various interactive elements, while recitation and normal repertoire of the best and brightest high school laboratory are done in a group problem-solving format physics students before they enter college. We also con- similar to that developed at Minnesota. At Dickinson jecture that most of them will have to learn to make College and at the public liberal arts institution, the these kinds of efforts as they progress to increasingly so- classes surveyed were done in the Workshop Physics en- phisticated materials and the level of challenge rises. vironment which replaces lectures with a combined lab This analysis of both the overall responses of the cali- and class discussion.3d The two-year college used a bration groups and the variations in the ordering con- purely traditional lecture-recitation framework. Like firms that the MPEX survey provides a quantitative Maryland, they have no lab in the first semester. The measure of characteristics which experts hope and ex- schools involved, the structure of their courses, and the number of students in our sample are summarized in Table 1. 100% 00% In order to eliminate the confounding factor of differen- tial drop-out rates, we only include students who com- 80% +Experts pleted the survey both at the beginning and at the end of + 70% College 789050,6 the term. We say that the data is matched. Our results 60% X show some differences among different classes at the HS Taw hers 50% same institution, but the variation is statistically consis- +US1POT 40% tent with the sample size. Therefore, we have combined 30% X UsACP pre results for similar classes at a given institution. 20% The overall survey results for the six schools are pre- 10% - sented in an A-D plot in Fig. 2. In order to simplify the 0% 1 reading of the graphs, we have displayed the results 0% 80% 30% 40% 60% 90% 20% 70% 100% 50% 10% from the three large research universities in one part of Fig. I: A-D plot for Calibration Groups. Average of all items Redish, Steinberg, and Saul Student Expectations 7 8 an MIR (gAtuage. Dm Gra I depen- Overall Reality lkita3 Experts 87/6 93/3 92/4 89/6 85/12 93/3 85/4 80/10 College 80/8 80/12 80/8 94/4 82/6 84/9 Teachers HS Teachers 75/16 62/26 95/2 73/15 71/18 68/13 67/21 USPOT 68/18 64/20 81/12 79/8 50/34 73/13 85/8 I UMCP pre 61/14 53/24 54/23 42/35 67/17 54/25 67/13 1 UMCP post 48/27 44/32 49/25 49/27 59/20 58/18 48/27 UMn pre 45/27 59/19 72/9 59/18 57/20 i 72/11 72/11 UMn post 69/10 58/20 57/20 61/17 46/28 72/12 63/16 - , OSU pre 51/24 65/10 53/23 37/36 66/16 65/13 52/21 OSU post 46/28 46/26 44/30 45/28 54/17 55/20 35/35 j DC pre 62/14 76/4 58/17 47/23 70/10 61/15 75/7 DC post 60/19 67/14 72/9 71/12 57/26 66/18 58/23 PLA pre 57/27 57/26 57/23 38/46 71/13 72/8 74/11 -----4/31 PLA post 45/34 48/30 -----2722 47/33 52/25 54/19 ! TYC pre 41/29 69/16 55/22 30/42 58/17 50/21 80/8 TY----C--P-o-si----- 49/26 -----ii/3i-T- 48/29 58/17 58/18 35/41 65/21 Table 4: Perm rages of students giving favorable/unfavorable responses on overall and clusters of the MPEX survey at the beginning (pre) and end (post) of the first unit of university physics. the figure (Fig. 2a) and those from the smaller schools age deterioration rather than an improvement in another (Fig. 2b). The pre-course results are shown of student expectations. by filled markers and the post-course results by open markers. The results of the expert group are shown by a The overall survey includes items that represent a vari- cross. ety of characteristics, as displayed in Table 2. In order to better understand what is happening in the classes ob- We make two observations. served, let us consider the initial state and the change of The initial state of the students at all the student expectations in the various clusters. The results 1. schools tested differs substantially from the ex- are presented in Table 4. pert results. The expert group was consistent, A. The Independence Cluster agreeing on which survey responses were desir- able 87% of the time. Beginning students only One characteristic of the binary thinker, as reported by agreed with the favorable (expert) responses Perry and BGCT, is the view that answers come from an about 40-60% of the time, a substantial dis- authoritative source, such as an instructor or a text, and crepancy. What is perhaps more distressing is it is the responsibility of that authority to convey this that students explicitly supported unfavorable knowledge to the student. The more mature students positions about 20-30% of the time. understand that developing knowledge is a participatory process. Hammer classifies these two extreme views as In all cases, the result of instruction on the 2. "by authority" and "independent." Survey items 1, 8, overall survey was an increase in unfavorable 13, 14, 17, and 27 probe students' views along this di- responses and a decrease in favorable re- mension. On this cluster, students' initial views were sponses. Thus, instruction produced an aver- Redish, Steinberg, and Saul Student Expectations 8 9 16, 21, and 29 have been included in order to probe stu- favorable in a range from 41% (TYC) to 62% (DC). All dent views along this dimension. groups showed essentially no significant change as a re- sult of one term of instruction. For comparison, the Our expert group was in agreement as to what responses USIPOT showed favorable views on these items 81% of were desirable on the elements of this cluster 85% of the the time. time. The initial views of students at our six schools were only favorable between 50% and 58% of the time. Survey items 1 and 14 are particularly illuminating and Most classes showed a small deterioration on this clus- show the largest gaps between experts and novices. ter, except for UMN (slight improvement from 57% to 61% favorable responses) and DC (improvement of 58% #1: All I need to do to understand most of the basic to 66% favorable responses). ideas in this course is just read the text, work most of the problems, and/or pay close attention in class. Two specific items in this cluster are worthy of an ex- plicit discussion. #14 Learning physics is a matter of acquiring new knowledge that is specifically located in the laws, #21: If I came up with two different approaches to a principles, and equations given in the textbook and problem and they gave different answers, I would in class. not worry about it; I would just choose the answer that seemed most reasonable. (Assume the answer The expert group was in 100% agreement that students is not in the back of the book.) should disagree with item 1 and in 84% agreement that they should disagree with item 14. Disagreeing with #29: A significant problem in this course is being able these items represents a rather sophisticated view of to memorize all the information I need to know. learning; but favorable shifts on these items are exactly the sort of changes that indicate the start of a transition Item 21 is a touchstone. Coming up with two different between-a binary and a more constructivist thinker. The answers using two different methods indicates some- interviews strongly support this view. Students who thing is seriously wrong with at least one of your solu- disagreed with these items were consistently the most tions and perhaps with your understanding of the phys- vigorous and active learners. ics and how to apply it to problems. Our expert group This cluster of items, and items 1 and 14 in particular, appear to confirm that most students in university phys- 100% ics enter with at least some characteristics of binary 90 learners, agreeing that learning physics is simply a mat- 80% ter of receiving knowledge in contrast to constructing + Evens 70 one's own understanding. We would hope that if a uni- .osu Pre 60% X UMCP pre versity education is to help students develop more so- UMN pre 50% phisticated views of their own learning, that the intro- ,,OSU peel MUMCP post 40% ductory semester of university physics would begin to UMN post 30% move students in the direction of more independence. 20% - Unfortunately, this does not appear to have been the 10% - case. In the touchstone items of 1 and 14, the only sig- nificant improvement was DC on item 14 (26% to 53%), 0% 90% 70% 60% 30% 40% 100% 50% 60% 0% 20% 10% and overall, only DC showed improvement. 100% B. The Coherence Cluster 90 Most physics faculty feel strongly that students should 80 %- see physics as a coherent, consistent structure. A major + Evens 70% strength of the scientific worldview is its ability to de- 0 41 TYC po ir 60% DC we scribe many complex phenomena with a few simple laws % W PLA pro 50 - and principles. Students who emphasize science as a ()roc post 0 CC post 40% collection of facts fail to see the integrity of the struc- VPLA post ture, an integrity that is both epistemologically convinc- 30% - ing and useful. The lack of a coherent view can cause 20%- students many problems, including a failure to notice er- 10% rors in their reasoning and an inability to evaluate a re- 0% 90% 80% 50% 30% 40% 100% 60% 70% 0% 20% 10% called item through cross-checks. Survey items 12, 15, Fig. 2a: (top) A-D plot for large schools. Average of all items. Fig. 2b: (bottom) A-D plot for small schools. Average of all items Student Expectations 9 Redish, Steinberg, and Said

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.