ebook img

ERIC ED359686: Partnerships for Learning. PDF

45 Pages·1993·1.4 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED359686: Partnerships for Learning.

DOCUMENT RESUME ED 359 686 EC 302 246 AUTHOR Nathanson, Jeanne H., Ed. TITLE Partnerships for Learning. INSTITUTION Office of Special Education and Rehabilitative Services (ED), Washington, DC. PUB DATE 93 NOTE 45p. AVAILABLE FROM OSERS News in Print, Room 3129, Switzer Building, 330 C St., S.W., Washington, DC 20202-2524 (free). PUB TYPE Collected Works Serials (022) JOURNAL CIT OSERS News in Print; v5 n4 Spr 1993 EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Agency Cooperation; Ancillary School Services; *Cooperative Programs; *Disabilities; Early Childhood Education; Educational Change; Grading; Homework; Inservice Education; Mothers; National Surveys; Parent Child Relationship; Parent Education; Parent Participation; School Business Relationship; State Programs; Therapists; Vocational Rehabilitation IDENTIFIERS Medically Fragile ABSTRACT This journal issue focuses on cooperative partnership programs for the improvement of educational services to students with disabilities. The eight articles are: (1) "Partner-Based Prelinguistic Intervention: A Preliminary Report" by M. Jeanne Wilcox (which found the intervention procedures had a strong effect on mother-child dyads); (2) "Helping Parents to Help Their Children" by Beth Dohrn, James Bryan and Tanis Bryan (which describes two pilot studies to increase the effectiveness of parent involvement stressing attribution training); (3) "Homework, Grading, and Testing: National Surveys of School District Policies" (Michael H. Epstein et al.); (4) "A Green Mountain Challenge: Assessing the Impact of School Reform on Children with Disabilities" by Pam Kay et al. (a Vermont effort at systematic educational reform); (5) "Partnerships Between Therapists, Parents, and Children" by Mary C. Lawlor and Elizabeth A. Cada (describing a University of Illinois project); (6) "Inservice Programs for Related Services Teams Serving Medically Fragile Children" by Forrest C. Bennett et al. (describing a demonstration project in Oregon and Washington); (7) "From Vision to Reality: The Opened Door" by Ruby Frazier (describing "Projects with Industry" in Lansing, Michigan) and (8) "Effective Consumer-Service Provider Interactions in Vocational Rehabilitation" (Thomas Czerlinsky and Shirley K. Chandler). All articles include references. (Dii) *********************************************************************** Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. *********************************************************************** tvrt 4 4, s: ,& ktr s'111477117:7",rrr ,s 9R96qR(IR -.0 4 ?"-; Renata Schlegel (1988) Spring 1993 2 Partner-Based Pre linguistic Intervention: 4 A Preliminary Report Partnerships M. Jeanne Wilcox, Ph.D. for Learning 10 Children Helping Parents To Help Their Tanis Bryan Beth Dohrn Volume V, Number 4 James Bryan Spring 1993 Homework, Grading, and Testing: 15 Richard W. Riley Policies National Surveys of School District Secretary William D. Bursuck Michael H. Epstein United States Department of Carla Cumblad Edward A. Polloway Education Madhavi Jayanthi William L. Smith Assessing the A Green Mountain Challenge: Acting Assistant Secretary 22 Children with Disabilities Office of Special Education and . . . Impact of School Reform on Rehabilitative Services Martha Fitzgerald Pam Kay Amy Mellencamp Jeanne H. Nathanson Editor 27 Parents, and Children . . Partnerships Between Therapists, Elizabeth A. Cada, M.S. Mary C. Lawlor, Sc.D. Deborah Rhode Cover art Services Teams Inservice Programs for Related 31 Children Serving Medically Fragile Renata Schlegel (1988) Patrick Haley, M.S. Washington Very Special Arts Forrest C. Bennett, M.D. Janet Valluzzi, MBA Inside cover art Gerald Smith, Ed.D. 36 Opened Door From Vision to Reality: The OSERS News In Print is published quarterly and is available free of Ruby Frazier charge. Contributors of articles for this issue expressed their own points Provider Interactions Effective Consumer-Service of view, which may not necessarily 39 in Vocational Rehabilitation reflect the position or policy of the Shirley K. Chandler, M.S. U.S. Department of Education. The Thomas Czerlinsky, Ph.D. information in this publication may be reproduced without further permis- sion: a credit line would be appreciat- ed. Please address comments to: Edi- Room tor, OSERS News In Print, 3129, Switzer Building, 330 C Street, S.W. Washington, D.C. 20202-2524. Spring 1993 3 4 ()SERS News In Print Partner-Based Pre linguistic Intervention: A Preliminary Report M. Jeanne Wilcox, Ph.D. Infant Child Communication Research Programs Arizona State University Aprimary area of concern for many young children with developmen- "'" tal disabilities is initial communication or prelinguistic skills. Often, these young children demonstrate little or no language use. Although facilitation of language skills is an ultimate goal for many children. the more immediate intervention goal must focus on estab- lishment of basic communication skills. Nes Prior to use of first words. children have developed a rich system of nonsymbol- ic communication abilities. Further, it is apparent that these early communica- tion skills (e.g.. intentionally conveying a message to an interactive partner) for- mulate a necessary basis for initial lan- guage use. This is not to say that inten- tional communication abilities by " themselves represent a sufficient condi- tion of language acquisition. However. it is clear that we cannot expect a child to use language as a tool for communi- cating it he or she has not first learned ..4.11.1V to communicate. The first task facing many early interventionists providing acquisition of intentional communica- interactive context. Central to interven- services to young children with disabili- tion is widely acknowledged, both for tions designed to establish initial com- ties is often facilitation of intentional children developing typically and those munication skills is consideration of communication skills. with developmental disabilities. Within facilitative parameters within the inter- Communication. as the process by the last decade. it has been repeatedly active context. The importance of the which meaning is conveyed during suggested that most developmental skill interpersonal environment to children's interpersonal interactions, is tied to the 5 OSERS N'eu s In Print OAP Tft ` --te" *--4-'mo - It?, J `II. /.0 this purposeful ner. The emergence of intent of transmitting a specific mean- learning. including communication. can communication is marked by a child's ing (e.g.. request for something) to an child/adult be viewed as deriving from deliberate attention to an interactive the interactive partner. but nonetheless 1982: social interchanges (e.g., Ms. partner. For example. a child may behavior is often perceived as mean- Bruner, 1977: Marfo, 1988; Snow. "request assistance" by looking at an ingful by a partner. For example. a 1990: 1981; Wilcox. Kouri. & Caswell, interactive partner. shaking a box that child may be unsuccessful at opening a Wilcox, 1992). Of particular importance he or she cannot open. and looking container of small toys and an interac- is expo- to children's early development toward the partner again. Children's tive partner perceives this as a context in which a sure to social interchanges initial intentional or purposeful com- offered and in which assistance can be caregiver/adult responds to a child's munications are conveyed through though the opens the container even behavior in a responsive manner. nonsymbolic modes and include a rela- indi- child has done nothing that would Responsivity has been discussed by var- tively restricted range of meanings for assistance. cate a specific request Easter- ious authors (e.g.. Lamb. & for (e.g., request for assistance, request Thus, during the earliest phase of com- brooks. 1981: Rosenberg & Robinson. attention). Gradually, through exposure munication development. children's tendency 1988) and refers to an adult's children expand to responsive partners, communications are not purposeful behavioral cues to recognize children's the types of meanings they can convey Commu- from the child's perspective. and provide contingent, appropriate. modes and also start using symbolic nication that occurs is a result of a part- and consistent responses to those cues. of those (e.g.. speech) for expression attribute ner's ability and tendency to Responsive partners have an impor- meanings. and meaning to a child's nonsymbolic facilitating the tant role to play in The behavior of an interactive partner frequently unconventional behavior. initial com- emergence of children's ini- is central to facilitation of children's Children gradually acquire the abili- munication skills. Children are born key tial communication abilities, and a communica- ty to produce intentional that with basic communication abilities is sensitive aspect of partner behavior is pur- tive behavior. i.e.. that which partner-per- can be characterized as behavior. A part- responses to a child's posefully produced to transmit a child's ceived, which means that a recognize a ner must consistently specific message to an interactive part- the behavior is not produced with 5 Sprinq 1993 nrif7"1" 111.7.!!1! 6 . . . OSERS Nel,s In Print Development was also administered to with the task of learning basic commu- child's communicative or potentially all children, revealing delays in recep- nication skills. may often be exposed to communicative attempts and respond to tive and expressive communication situations in which they are receiving those attempts in a contingent, appropri- skills that ranged from 6 to 22 months. inconsistent responses (across their typi- ate. and consistent manner. In this way. Review of the children's medical cal interactive partners) to their poten- a child comes to learn that his or her records indicated auditory and visual tially communicative behaviors. When behavior has consistent and predictable acuity within normal limits. Baseline this is considered in conjunction with environmental effects, a knowledge the fact that young children with dis- base that is regarded as essential to a observations of the children while inter- abilities are typically enrolled in early acting in typical activities with each of child's ability to purposefully commu- intervention programs in which they the targeted interactive partners con- nicate (Snow, 1981; Wilcox et al., have contact with multiple interactive firmed that, while some children pro- 1990). As the importance of children's duced occasional behavior that was partners (e.g., teachers and related ser- interactive partners is increasingly rec- judged as purposeful communication, vices personnel), potential inconsisten- ognized, especially for children who are cies across these partners becomes a their communicative interactions were presymbolic, the behavior of partners of matter of concern. Wilcox et al. sug- predominantly preintentional in nature, children with disabilities has served as gested that a viable intervention strategy i.e.. the majority of communication that the focus of a variety of investigations might be one in which training focused occurred was partner-perceived. Thus. and communication programming (e.g., on increasing the consistency of recog- Brookes-Gunn & Lewis, 1984; Cun- all children were determined to be can- nition and responses to children's com- ningham, Reuter, Blackwell. & Deck. didates for intervention with a focus on municative or potentially communica- facilitating purposeful nonsymbolic 1981: Hanzlik & Stevenson. 1986: tive behavior by a child's typical Houghton, Bronicki. & Guess, 1987; communication skills. interactive partners. Therefore, the pur- Tannock, 1988; Wilcox et al., 1990). The children were enrolled in one of pose of this research was to develop and four different early intervention pro- Numerous aspects of partner's interac- test a partner-based intervention pro- grams in which they received educa- tive behavior have been examined with gram of this nature and in the following tional and therapeutic (e.g., speech/lan- children with disabilities including gen- sections of this article preliminary infer eral measures of interaction such as guage, physical therapy, occupational mation regarding the procedures and turn-taking and duration of joint atten- 'herapy) services. Two of the programs outcomes is presented. were designated as standard practice tion as well as more specific measures of partner behavior (e.g., positive control sites, the remaining two pro- Method compliant behavior. grams served as sites for initial imple- responses to mentation of the experimental inter- responses to child initiations) in relation Participants vention procedures. This report is to child behavior. Although there has Twenty-seven children, ranging in based on thirteen children and their been a fair amount of variation in results age from 16 to 38 months and their of such studies, an emerging concern is interactive partners who participated in familiar interactive partners (mothers the fact that young children with dis- the experimental intervention condition and their early intervention personnel abilities may often be exposed to less and fourteen children and their partners including early childhood special edu- than optimal responses from their com- who served as standard practice control cators. speech-language pathologists. municative partners. For example, in an participants. occupational therapists, and physical earlier investigation of partner respon- Intervention Procedures therapists) were recruited for participa- sivity to young children with develop- In recognition of the fact that commu- tion in this investigation. All children mental disabilities (Wilcox et al.. 1990), nication is a dynamic process, the inter- had a medical diagnosis of a general comparisons were made with respect to vention procedures included a focus on developmental delay and none were the tendencies of children's mothers. both child and partner behavior within using language at the time of recruit- early intervention teachers, and speech- typical interactive contexts. Central to ment for participation in the research. language pathologists to recognize chil- the design of the experimental program Standardized testing included adminis- dren's communicative or potentially was the assumption that communication tration of the Battelle Developmental Results behavior. communicative occurs during any interaction that may Inventory and all children scored at least revealed that the partners typically dis- take place with a young child, irrespec- 1.5 standard deviations below the mean agreed in the identification of children's tive of the specific focus of the interac- in all assessed developmental areas (i.e.. communicative behavior as well as their tive effort. In other words, it is not nec- motor, cognitive. communication, per- interpretations of potential meanings. It essary for there to be a specific adaptive). The sonal-social. and was concluded that many young chil- "communication" activity for a child to Sequenced Inventory of Communication dren with disabilities, who are faced Spring /993 6 OSERS News In Print complete. The training procedures included a combination of individual Program Components of the Partner Training and group meetings for the targeted partners Table 1 provides an overview Component One: Assessment and brief description of the program child's typical partners as Focus: Identification of behavior perceived by a components. As can be seen, the initial component was designed to gather communication. videotaped segment Method: Each partner individually watches the same information regarding the partner's per- from his or her perception, of caregiver-child interaction and specifies, ception of children's communicative behavior and its asso- when communication occurs as well as the child's behavior. This information served as ciated meaning. the basis for determination of areas of consistency and inconsistencies among Component Two: Sensitivity Training the partners and ultimately resulted in communication behavior and Focus: Increasing identification of targeted the identification of specific child identification of opportunities for communication. behaviors) that would be targeted for and one individual Method: Three group meetings with the partners facilitation efforts. The second compo- engaged in typical activities "coaching" sess;un with each partner while nent had a primary focus on increasing with the child. the partners' abilities to recognize tar- Component Three: Contingency Training geted child behavior as well as strate- seconds) and relatedness gies for creating opportunities for com- Focus: Increasing the timeliness (within five munication. The third component model appropriate behavior (attribute meaning matched to opportunity; child behavior. focused on establishing contingent part- and/or symbol) of responses to targeted behav- and two individual "coach- ner responses to targeted child Method: Two group meetings with the partners typical activities with ior. and the fourth component had a pri- ing" sessions with each partner while engaged in mary focus on consistency of partner the child. responses (both within and across part- Component Four: Consistency Training ners) as well as strategies for further of children's skills. Focus: Self-monitoring and further enhancement enhancement of children's emerging individual "coach- Method: Two group meetings with the partners and two communication skills. in typical activities with ing" sessions with each partner while engaged Pre- and Post-Intervention Measures the child. Videotaped samples were obtained immediately prior to and immediately following partners completion of the direct contact with the targeted children. learn basic communication skills. For intervention program for experimental Rather, experimental efforts focused on example, a mother may be Irving to get and standard practice participants. These training and monitoring of the children's her child dressed. During this routine samples at each measurement poir.t (i.e., typical interactive partners, defined as caregiving activity, communication per included two pre- and post-intervention) those who interacted with the children at communi- se is not the focus: however, 20-30 minute tapes of each child inter- had least three times per week and cation between mother and child is cer- acting with his or her participating part- six known the children for at least tainly taking place. and even though there were ner in typical activities. Thus. months prior to the research. dressing is the primary goal. there are observation a total of 50-60 minutes of The primary goal of the partner train- still numerous opportunities for the child for each child interacting with his or her ing was to increase the sensitivity, con- Similar to learn about communication. partner for each of the measurement of partner sistency. and contingency child- reasuning can be applied to early points. Teacher-child samples were and responses to children's actual hood education activities. Thus, the obtained during a classroom activity of potential communicative behavior procedures ex pen mental intervention the educators' choice, and samples of within the context of whatever caregiv- those which can he conceptualized as children interacting with their physical. ing, educational, or therapeutic activi- were "overlaid" upon existing program- occupational. and speech/language ther- ties the partners would typically con- ming efforts. No attempts were made to apists were obtained by recording a duct with the children. The intervention alter any aspect of the children's ongo- scheduled treatment session. Mother- protocol consisted of four major partner ing program plans. and the children con- child samples were obtained in the chil- training components that took approxi- tinued to receive educational and thera- dren's homes during a tree play activity, mately six months for the partners to peutic services. Research staff had no Spring /993 7 8 These tapes were reviewed for the Children's Purposeful Communcation with purpose of obtaining information Mothers & Professionals regarding child and adult behavior. Child behavior measures included 30 identification of all occurrences of pur- C poseful communication, defined as 25 0 those instances in which the child 20 looked at the interactive partner and 0.° produced additional behavior (e.g., reach, vocalization, alternating looks) for the purpose of requesting an object or activity or for the purpose of direct- 0 Prof.-Post Prof.-Pre Mothers-Post Mothers-Pre ing the partner's attention to a particu- lar event. In addition to purposeful Maternal Enhancing and Limiting Behavior communication, behavior that did not meet criteria for purposeful communi- cation but occurred in an "assistive 80 context" was also noted. Assistive con- texts were defined as those instances in 60 which there was an opportunity for the it adult to interpret a child's behavior as Ca 05 40 either a request for an activity. object. or attention. 20 Measures of partner behavior focused 0 on descriptions of adult reponses to the Limit-Post Limit-Pre Enhance-Post Enhance-Pre children's purposeful communication as well as behavior that was not purposeful Professional Enhancing and Limiting Behavior but occurred in an assistive context. Partner responses were categorized as 100 enhancing, limiting, or neutral. Enhanc- ing responses included those regarded 80 as facilitating for children's communi- a, 60 cation and were identified as instances in which the partners demonstrated (a) ct 40 compliance with purposeful requests. 20 (b) compliance with perceived child requests, i.e., those behaviors produced 0 Limit-Post Limit-Pre Enhance-Post Enhance-Pre in assistive contexts, or (c) acknowl- edgement (in the absence of compli- ance) for both purposeful and perceived NIP Experimental Control requests. Limiting responses were those regarded as less than optimal with Results And Conclusions in which the partners attempted to redi- respect to facilitation of children's com- rect child behavior they regarded as munication skills and included instances displays the results to Figure 1 inappropriate. Neutral behavior was in which the partners (a) ignored chil- changes in children's purposeful com- regarded as neither facilitative nor limit- dren's behavior. (b) attempted to redi- munication for the experimental and included predominantly and ing rect a child's attention to an activity or standard practice control groups. Due instances in which the partners could object that was not consistent with the to variances in observation, time and, discern no clear child intent or opportu- child's purposeful communication or therefore, opportunities to communi- nity for compliance and responded with the assistive context, and (c ) verbal cate, the frequency of the children's requests for additional information (e.g., directives that were not related to the purposeful communications was calcu- "What do you want?", "Huh?", "Could ongoing activity. Excluthd from the lated per 10 minute observation units at limiting category were those instances you show me?") 8 Sri ins; 1 g93 OSERS Mews iii Print tion programs are being evaluated for partner training procedures for the each measurement point. Data for the all participating children. i mother-child dyads. children while interacting with the pro- Unfortunately, similar trends were fessionals was collapsed across disci- References not observed for the professional part- pline groupings due to the fact that not ner-child dyads. The results indicated Ms. H. (1982). The unfolding of behavioral all children had all categories of profes- organization in the face of biological violation. minimal changes in child or partner sional partners. Statistical analyses In E. Tronick (Ed.). Social interchange in infan- behavior. Experimental partners demon- indicated that while experimental and cy: Affect. cognition. and communication (pp. strated essentially equal amounts of 125-160). Baltimore: University Park Press. control group participants demonstrated Brooks-Gunn, J. &I-eviis, M. (1984). Mater- enhancing and limiting behavior from equal abilities during pre-intervention nal responsivtty in interactions with handicapped and the pre- and post-intervention, measures with their mothers, the exper- infants. Child Development. 55. 782-793. experimental children demonstrated imental children demonstrated signifi- Bruner, J. (1977). Early social interaction and only a slight, nonsignificant increase in language acquisition. In H. Schaffer (Ed.), Stud- cantly greater amounts of purposeful ies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 271-289). their purposeful communications with communication post - intervention. No New York: Academic Press. these partners. Standard practice control significant changes were noted for Cunningham. C.. Reuter, E., Balckwell. J., decrease partners demonstrated a slight either group of children while interact- & Deck. J. (1981). Behavioral and linguistic in enhancing behavior and a slight developments in the interactions of normal and ing with their professional partners. retarded children with their mothers. Child increase in limiting behavior but both Figures 2 and 3 display information Development. 52. 62-70. changes were insignificant. Standard the partner reaarding changes in Hanzlik. J. & Stevenson, M. (1986). Interac- practice control children demonstrated enhancing and limiting behavior. tion of moaners with their infants who are men- communi- ially retard -d. retarded with cerebral palsy, or no change in their purposeful Aeain, data for the professional part- nonretarded. American Journal of Mental Defi- cations with their professional partners. disciplines. ners was collapsed across ciency. 90. 513-520. It would appear that the intervention Houghton, J., Bronicki, G., & Guess, D. Statistical analyses revealed significant impact on program had minimal to no (1987). Opportunities to express preferences and changes in the experimental mothers' make choices among students with severe dis- the professional partner-child dyads. behavior, and as can be seen in Figure abilities in classroom settings. Journal of the At this point, it is unclear why the 2. demonstrated an increase in enhanc- Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps. intervention procedures had such a ing behavior and a decrease in limiting 12. 18-27. minimal impact on the professional Lamb, M. & Easterbrooks. M. (1981). Indi- behavior. No significant changes were idual differences in parental sensitivity: Origin, the pro- partners. One possibility is that noted for the standard practice control components. and consequences. In M. Lamb & fessionals' levels of production of mothers. Similarly. no significant dif- R. Sherrod (Eds.). Infant social cognition: enhancing behavior were high at the Empirical and theoretical considerations (pp. ferences or changes were noted in the pre-intervention measurements. It may 127-153). Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum. behavior of experimental and standard Marto, K. (Ed.) (1988). Parent-child infrrac. be that there was little room for practice professional partners. non and developmental disabilities: Theory. improvement. However, the children When comparing pre- and post- research, and intervention. New York: Praeger. demonstrat- were certainly capable of Rosenberg, S. & Robinson. C. (1988). Inter- intervention data across the experimen- ing improvement and the patterns actions of parents with their young handicapped tal and standard practice mother-child children. In S. Odom & M. Karnes (Eds.), Early observed in changes in the maternal- dyads, it is apparent that the interven- with intervention for infants and young children child dyads suggest that the minimal handicaps (pp. 159-178). Baltimore: Paul H. tion procedures had a strong impact on change observed in the children's com- Brookes. these experimental dyads. While the Snow, C. (1981). Social interaction and lan- munication abilities may be in part standard practice dyads made essen- Ingram guage acquisition. In P. Dale & D. attributable to the minimal changes tially no change from pre- and post- (Eds.). Child language: An international per- made by the professionals in their University spective (pp. 195 -214). Baltimore: intervention. dramatic changes were responsivness to the children. Certainly Park Press. purposeful com- seen in the children's Tannock, R. (1988). Control and reciprocity in these are issues that continue to serve munication while interacting with their mothers interactions with Down syndrome and investigative as a focus for ongoing normal children. In K. Marto (Ed.). Parent-child mothers. In turn, mothers demonstrated efforts. Other ongoing efforts include Theo- interaction and developmental disabilities: their enhanc- a significant increase in New of ry. research. and intervention (pp. 163-180). further examination and validations ing behavior and a significant decrease York: Praeger. the intervention protocol for young This sug- in their limiting behavior. Wilcox, M., Kouri. T., & Caswell, S. (1990). children and their mothers and further relationship Partner sensitivity to communication behavior of important gests an disabilities. examination of potential impacts on young children with developmental between maternal enhancing behavior Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders.55. 679-693. professional partners' behavior through and children's emerging skills with Wilcox, M.11992). Enhancing initial commu- modifications of intervention proce- nonsymbolic respect to purposeful nication skills in young children with develop- dures. Additionally, the longer -term mental disabilities through partner programming. communication, and these preliminary interven- Seminars in Speech & Hearing, 13. 194-212. outcomes of the prelinguistic findings attest to the efficacy of the 9 Sprint; /993 1 0

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.