Emotion ©2011AmericanPsychologicalAssociation 2012,Vol.12,No.2,326–337 1528-3542/12/$12.00 DOI:10.1037/a0025762 Emotion Differentiation Moderates Aggressive Tendencies in Angry People: A Daily Diary Analysis Richard S. Pond, Jr. Todd B. Kashdan UniversityofKentucky GeorgeMasonUniversity C. Nathan DeWall Antonina Savostyanova UniversityofKentucky GeorgeMasonUniversity Nathaniel M. Lambert Frank D. Fincham BrighamYoungUniversity TheFloridaStateUniversity Anger is commonly associated with aggression. Inefficient anger-coping strategies increase negative affectanddepletetheregulatoryresourcesneededtocontrolaggressiveimpulses.Factorslinkedwith betteremotionregulationmaythenweakentherelationshipbetweenangerandaggression.Thecurrent work explored one factor associated with emotion regulation—differentiating one’s emotions into discretecategories—thatmaybufferangrypeoplefromaggression.Threediarystudies(N(cid:1)628)tested thehypothesisthatemotiondifferentiationwouldweakentherelationshipbetweenangerandaggression. InStudy1,participantshighinemotiondifferentiationreportedlessdailyaggressivetendencieswhen angry,comparedtolowdifferentiators.InStudy2,comparedtolowdifferentiators,highdifferentiators reportedlessfrequentprovocationindailylifeandlessdailyaggressioninresponsetobeingprovoked and feeling intense anger. Study 3 showed that high daily emotional control mediated the interactive effect of emotion differentiation and anger on aggression. These results highlight the importance of considering how angry people differentiate their emotions in predicting their aggressive responses to anger. Keywords:emotion,affect,categorization,anger,aggression Thepotentialforangerpervadesdailylife.Driverspoundtheir become aggressive while others do not. The current research hornsinsteadofpolitelyhonking,coworkersmakebitingremarks focuses on one factor—the ability to differentiate one’s negative instead of offering constructive criticism, and close relationship emotional experiences into discrete categories—that might make partners criticize their loved ones instead of supporting them. people resilient to aggression following anger provocation. We Anger has long been associated with aggressive behavior propose that emotion differentiation improves coping and allows (Berkowitz, 1983, Berkowitz, 1990; Dollard, Doob, Miller, forgreaterregulatorycontroloverone’semotionalstate,duetoan Mower, & Sears, 1939). Moreover, angered people often aggress enhanced capacity for understanding, clarifying, and describing when doing so can change their mood (Bushman, Baumeister, & what one feels at any point in time. We predict that such better Phillips,2001).Despitewidespreadacceptanceoftheroleofanger emotional control would have implications for buffering angry in predicting aggression, it is unclear why some angry people peoplefromaggressivebehavior. Anger and Poor Emotion Regulation ThisarticlewaspublishedOnlineFirstOctober24,2011. Whenpeoplefeelangryorupset,theyaremotivatedtoimprove Richard S. Pond, Jr., Department of Psychology, University of Ken- their mood (Morris & Reilly, 1987). Indeed, emotion regulation tucky;ToddB.Kashdan,DepartmentofPsychology,GeorgeMasonUni- effortsoccursorapidlyafteranupsettingeventthatitisdifficult versity; C. Nathan DeWall, Department of Psychology, University of tokeepparticipantsinabadmood(Isen,1984,Isen,1987;Taylor, Kentucky; Antonina Savostyanova, Department of Psychology, George 1991;Worth&Mackie,1987).However,angerislinkedtopoor MasonUniversity;NathanielM.Lambert,DepartmentofFamilyScience, strategiesforemotionregulation.Forinstance,angrypeopleoften Brigham Young University; Frank D. Fincham, Family Institute, The ruminate over the experience that provoked them; they turn their FloridaStateUniversity. attention inward, either reliving the angry experience itself or The authors thank Gregory T. Smith and Peggy S. Keller for their focusingattentiononhowbadtheyfeel(Caprara,1986;Lyubomir- helpfulcommentsonpreviousdraftsofthisarticle.Theauthorsalsothank sky&Nolen-Hoeksema,1995).Angerruminationisnotaneffec- JohnB.Nezlekforhisstatisticaladvice. CorrespondenceconcerningthisarticleshouldbeaddressedtoRichard tive coping strategy because it ultimately increases aggression, S. Pond, Jr., Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Kastle evenagainstinnocentbystanders(Bushman,2002;Bushman,Bo- Hall0004,Lexington,KY40506-0044.E-mail:[email protected] nacci, Pedersen, Vasquez, & Miller, 2005; Denson, Pedersen, 326 EMOTIONDIFFERENTIATIONBUFFERSAGGRESSION 327 Friese, Hahm, & Roberts, 2011). For instance, anger rumination Taylor,2004).Peoplewhohavedifficultyidentifyingfeelingsare playsamajorroleindisplacedaggression(e.g.,roadrage;Denson, alsopronetoengageinimpulsiveaggression(Fossatietal.,2009; Pedersen, & Miller, 2006). Rumination processes also increase Teten, Miller, Bailey, Dunn, & Kent, 2008). However, this re- angryaffect,physiologicalarousal,andaggressivecognition(Ped- search is difficult to interpret due to the reliance on people’s ersenetal.,2011).Angerruminationalsodepletespreciousregu- responsestoglobalquestionnairesontheirmetacognitiveabilityto latoryresourcesneededtosuppressaggressiveimpulses(Denson, differentiate emotions. People asked to respond to broad state- 2009;Densonetal.,2011). ments (e.g., “I have feelings that I can’t quite identify”) may Venting is another poor emotion regulation strategy linked to provide answers based on limited insight, influenced by social anger.Ventinginvolvesactingoutaggressiveimpulsesonpeople desirability. Some novel approaches from neuroimaging (e.g., orinanimateobjects(e.g.,punchingapillow,screaming,hittinga Newetal.,2002)andinterviewtranscriptanalyses(Villemarette- punchingbag).Angeredpeopleexpecttofeelbetterafterventing. Pittman,Stanford,&Greve,2003)havealsoshownthatdeficitsin Infact,ifangeredpeoplebelievetheirmoodisunchangeable,they the ability to recognize and articulate emotional experiences also aremuchlesslikelytobehaveaggressively(Bushmanetal.,2001). predictedimpulsiveaggression. Although some people take pleasure in venting their frustrations, Emotion differentiation may be better captured with a skills- ventingincreasesaggressionbecauseitcuesaggressivecognition based measurement, such as observing how people discriminate and behavior (Bushman, 2002; Bushman, Baumeister, & Stack, their emotional experiences over multiple occasions (Dunning, 1999;Hornberger,1959).Ineffect,ruminationandventingrepre- Heath, & Suls, 2004). Only a few studies have examined daily sentpoorangerregulationstrategies,ifone’sgoalistoreduceor variations in emotions as they relate to daily experiences (e.g., evenpreventaggressivebehavior. Kashdan et al., 2010; Swendsen et al., 2000; Tennen, Affleck, Armeli, & Carney, 2000). To better understand how emotional experiencesinfluencethetendencytoaggress,aswellaspossible Emotion Differentiation as a Marker for Resiliency bufferstothisrelationship,itiscrucialtoemployamethodology The preceding research implies that anger is associated with thatcapturesfeelingsandeventsacrosstimeinaperson’snatural ineffectiveemotionregulationstrategiesthatincreaseaggression. environment.Fromthisperspective,emotiondifferentiationshows Traitsassociatedwitheffectiveemotionregulationmaytherefore promise as a component of resilience. For example, Ong and buffer angered people from behaving aggressively. Emotion dif- Bergeman (2004) found that higher levels of trait resilience pre- ferentiation is a promising candidate for an individual difference dicted greater emotion differentiation and a higher co-occurrence factor associated with effective emotion regulation, which may ofpositiveandnegativeemotionsovera30-daysamplingperiod. have implications for weakening the relationship between anger Kang and Shaver (2004) found high emotion differentiators had andaggression. better interpersonal relationships (both self- and partner-rated). Emotion differentiation, also known as emotional granularity, Ong, Bergeman, and Boker (2009) argued that “emotional com- refers to how much a person is aware of and able to classify plexity may be a resource that resilient people draw upon during experiences into discrete emotion categories (Barrett, Gross, times of challenge and adversity” (p. 1784). Consistent with this Christensen,&Benvenuto,2001;Kashdan,Ferssizidis,Collins,& argument,thecurrentinvestigationaddresseshowemotiondiffer- Muraven, 2010; Tugade, Fredrickson, & Barrett, 2004). People entiationmaybufferangrypeopleagainstaggressiveresponding. whoshowadiminishedcapacityforunderstanding,clarifying,and describingwhattheyarefeelingatagivenpointoftime(i.e.,low Emotional Control Underlies Emotion emotion differentiators) have difficulty discerning more detail Differentiation Effects about an emotional state beyond the presence of positive or neg- Whymightemotiondifferentiationbufferangeredpeoplefrom ative valence (i.e., feeling “good” or “bad”). This lack of under- aggression? Better emotional control is one possibility. People standingmayleadlowemotiondifferentiatorstomisinterpretand whoarehighinemotiondifferentiation,particularlythedifferen- ruminateaboutnegativearousalstates.Moreover,lowdifferentia- tiationofintensenegativeemotions,havemoreemotionalintelli- torsmaybecomesusceptibletobingeeating,substanceabuse,and gence, a greater sensitivity to internal and situational cues, and a other self-destructive strategies to down-regulate negative emo- largerinventoryofadaptiveemotionregulationstrategiesattheir tions (Kashdan et al., 2010; Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997). disposal (Barrett & Gross, 2001; Barrett et al., 2001; Kashdan & Parker, Taylor, and Bagby (1998) suggest that people with little Rottenberg,2010).Infact,peoplewhoarebetteratdifferentiating abilitytodistinguishtheiremotionshavelesscapacityforemotion their emotional experience also report less self-distraction and regulation.Ontheotherhand,peoplewhofarebetterindiscerning moreengagementduringtimesofstress,aswellasagreaterability emotionsaremoreadeptatregulatingemotionsandpossessfewer to think carefully about their behavioral options in the stressful negative attitudes and less distress about intense emotions; thus, situation(Tugadeetal.,2004).Thus,highemotiondifferentiators theymaybemoreresilientwhenconfrontingstress(Barrettetal., mayspendlesstimeandself-regulatoryresourcesoncounterpro- 2001;Kang&Shaver,2004;Kashdanetal.,2010;Tugadeetal., ductive anger-regulation strategies that ultimately increase angry 2004). affect and aggression (Bushman, 2002; Bushman et al., 1999; Preliminary evidence for the association between low emotion Densonetal.,2011). differentiation and aggression stems from the literature on alex- ithymia. People with alexithymia—who are less able to identify The Present Research and describe emotions, and less able to discern emotions from bodily sensations—report similar or more intense physiological The present research tested the dual hypothesis that emotion arousal to unpleasant stimuli (e.g., Luminet, Rime, Bagby, & differentiation moderates aggressive responding in angry people, 328 PONDETAL. andthatgreateremotionalcontrolmediatesthiseffect.Toprovide & Perry, 1992), which was modified for daily use. This measure converging evidence in support of our hypothesis, we conducted was used because self-reported propensities toward physical and threediarystudiesexaminingnegativeemotiondifferentiationand verbal aggression are strongly related to behavioral aggression aggression. We predicted that on days when people feel more (Giancola & Parrott, 2008). Responses across the items were intense anger, high emotion differentiators will report less daily summedtoformacompositemeasureofdailyaggressivetenden- aggression,comparedtolowdifferentiators.InStudy3,wetested cies.Highernumbersindicatedgreateraggression. whether greater emotional control mediates the interactive effect ofemotiondifferentiationandangeronaggression. Procedure Participants were given a URL to record their feelings and Study 1: Emotion Differentiation Predicts Less Daily behaviorsthreetimeseachweekfor25days,whichincludedthe Aggressive Tendencies in Angry People measuresofnegativeaffect,angerintensity,andaggression.Par- Study 1 provided an initial test of the hypothesis that emotion ticipantswereinstructedtocompletetheirdailysurveysattheend differentiation predicts less aggression in people who feel angry. ofeachdaybeforemidnight.Toincreasecompliance,researchers Participants completed daily measures of negative emotion and stressed that receiving full participation credit was contingent on aggressivetendenciesthreetimesaweekfora25-dayperiod(total timelyreporting,andthatatime-datestampwouldberecordedon of 10 waves). We predicted that high emotion differentiators each log. All information submitted via online survey was confi- would be less susceptible to aggression when they felt angry, dentialandstoredonasecureserver.Adebriefingfollowed. compared to people who were less adept at differentiating their negativeemotions. Results Method Preliminary Analyses Ourmainpredictionwasthatamongpeoplewhoreportedfeel- Participants ing more intense anger, those who were better at differentiating among their negative emotions would be less susceptible to ag- Participants consisted of 199 undergraduate students (76% gression. Because the data violated the assumption of indepen- women)atalarge,Southern-Atlanticuniversity.Withameanage of 19.42 (SD (cid:1) 1.54), 70.4% of participants were Caucasian, dence in ordinary least squares regression (i.e., daily measures nested within individual participants), we used multilevel model- 13.1% were Hispanic, 2.0% were Asian American, 18.2% were ing techniques to account for their nested structure, using HLM AfricanAmerican,and9.3%ofparticipantsreportedtheirraceas Version6.08(Nezlek,2001;Raudenbush&Bryk,2002;Rauden- “other.”Studentsreceivedresearchcreditforparticipation. bush,Bryk,Cheong,&Congdon,2000).Atotalof1,928daysof data were provided by 199 participants (M (cid:1) 9.69). Participants Measures followedproperprotocolfortimelydailyresponsesfor94.4%of Negative emotion differentiation. Participants completed the entries (i.e., completed entries at the end of each day before thenegativeaffectsubscaleofthePANAS(NA;Watson,Clark,& midnight). Tellegen,1988),whichassessedtheintensityofnegativeemotions Theaverageintraclasscorrelationforthedifferentiationofneg- (e.g.,nervous,distressed,scared,hostile)participantsexperienced ative emotions was .84 (SD (cid:1) .47), indicative of an acceptable eachday.Eachitemwasmeasuredona5-pointscale(fromvery levelofvariability.Toimprovetheinterpretationoffindings,we slightlyornotatalltoextremely).Anindexofnegativeemotion reverse-scoredthescaleofemotiondifferentiation(i.e.,multiplied differentiation was computed by calculating average intraclass scores by (cid:2)1), so that larger scores reflected greater emotion correlations with absolute agreement between the negative emo- differentiationinsubsequentanalyses. tion adjectives across the assessment period for each participant Ourinitialanalysesfocusedonthereliabilityofthedailymea- (Barrett,1998;Kashdanetal.,2010;Tugadeetal.,2004).Larger sures for anger intensity and aggressive tendencies. We followed correlation scores indicated less differentiation of emotions. A procedures recommended by Nezlek (2007, 2011) for assessing Fisherrtoztransformationwascomputedonallintraclasscorre- scalereliabilitywithnesteddata.Usingathree-levelunconditional lationsbeforeanyadditionalanalyses.1 modelwithitems(Level1)nestedwithindays(Level2)anddays Dailyintensityofangerexperience. Participantscompleted nestedwithinpeople(Level3)(seeRaudenbush&Bryk,2002for fouritemsmeasuringhowmuchwithinthepast24hoursaclose formal rationale), analyses showed that the four items for daily friendhasmadethemfeelangry,frustrated,provoked,andhostile, intensityofexperiencedangerandthefouritemsfordailyaggres- relativetootherdays.Theitemswereassessedona9-pointscale sive tendencies had adequate reliability (0.91 and 0.66, respec- from (cid:2)4 (Far less than usual) to (cid:3)4 (Far more than usual) and weresummedtoformacompositeofdailyangerintensity.Higher 1Studies 1 and 3 also included measures of positive emotion (PA: numbersindicatedgreaterfeelingsofanger. Watson,Clark,&Tellegen,1988),whichpermittedustocreatepositive Daily aggressive tendencies. Participants completed an ab- emotion differentiation indices. Neither Study 1 nor Study 3 showed breviated form of the physical (e.g., “Given enough provocation significant positive emotion differentiation (cid:4) daily anger interactions to today, I might hit another person”) and verbal (e.g., “If people predictaggressivetendencies(p’s(cid:1).30and.90,respectively).Addition- wereannoyingmetoday,IwouldtellthemwhatIthinkofthem”) ally, there were no main effects for positive emotion differentiation in aggression subscales of the Aggression Questionnaire (AQ; Buss Studies1and3(p’s(cid:1).07and.65,respectively). EMOTIONDIFFERENTIATIONBUFFERSAGGRESSION 329 tively). The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for daily ag- negative emotion differentiation did not reach significance, b (cid:1) gressive tendencies was 0.52, suggesting that 48% of the 0.27,t(197)(cid:1)0.52,p(cid:1).61.Themaineffectforangerintensity variabilityinaggressiverespondingwaswithin-person. was significant, such that, on average, participants who reported Next,priortoourprimaryanalyses,weexaminedwhetheranger feelingmoredailyangerfromaclosefriendalsoreportedgreater intensityvariedasafunctionofemotiondifferentiation(grand-mean dailyaggressivetendencies,b(cid:1)0.07,t(197)(cid:1)2.54,p(cid:1).01. centered).Theseanalysesconsistedofatwo-levelmodelwithdays Toevaluatethenatureofourinteractioneffect,weexaminedthe (Level1)nestedwithinpeople(Level2).Themaineffectfornegative association between daily anger intensity and daily aggressive emotiondifferentiationdidnotreachsignificance,b(cid:1)0.41,t(197)(cid:1) tendencies among participants relatively low ((cid:2)1 SD) and high 0.41,p(cid:1).68,suggestingthatdailyangerintensitydidnotvaryasa ((cid:3)1SD)inemotiondifferentiation(Aiken&West,1991).Among functionofemotiondifferentiation. participants low in emotion differentiation, anger intensity pre- We then constructed a multilevel model to test our primary dictedincreaseddailyaggressivetendencies,b(cid:1)0.20,t(cid:1)2.74, hypothesis, with negative emotion differentiation as a Level 2 p (cid:1) .007. However, among participants who expressed a greater predictor,dailyangerintensityasaLevel1predictor,across-level tendencytodifferentiatetheiremotions,therewasnotasignificant interaction term between negative emotion differentiation and association between anger intensity and aggression, b (cid:1) (cid:2)0.05, daily anger intensity, and daily aggressive tendencies as the out- t (cid:1) (cid:2)0.77, p (cid:1) .44. On days defined by extreme anger, people comeofinterest.Intheseanalyses,theLevel2predictor(negative high in emotion differentiation reported 17% fewer aggressive emotiondifferentiation)wasgrand-meancentered(Aiken&West, tendenciesthandidpeoplelowinemotiondifferentiation. 1991). Daily anger intensity was group-mean centered (i.e., person-centered),therebyeliminatingtheinfluenceofperson-level differences on parameter estimates of mean daily anger (Nezlek, Discussion 2001). This meant that in within-person analyses, an individual’s coefficientdescribedtherelationshipbetweendeviationsfromhis Study1offersinitialevidencethatidentifyingandrespondingto or her mean anger intensity level and deviations from his or her negative emotions in distinct ways is a protective factor against meandailyaggressionlevel. aggressivebehavior,evenwhenonereportsfeelingangry,acom- mon elicitor of aggression (Berkowitz, 1983, Berkowitz, 1990). Moderating Effect of Negative Emotion Differentiation Participants’ capacity for emotion differentiation weakened the on the Association Between Anger and Aggressive relationship between daily anger provoked by a close friend and Tendencies daily aggressive tendencies. That is, daily anger predicted fewer aggressive tendencies among participants high in emotion differ- Asexpected,analysesrevealedasignificantNegativeEmotion Differentiation (cid:4) Anger Intensity interaction, b (cid:1) (cid:2)0.13, entiation than participants low in emotion differentiation. Al- t(197) (cid:1) (cid:2)1.97, p (cid:1) .05 (see Figure 1). The main effect for thoughStudy1showsthatgreateremotiondifferentiationweakens the relationship between anger and aggression, it is unclear whether emotion differentiation moderates the effect of anger on aggression in response to direct provocation. We explore this effectinStudy2. Study 2: Emotion Differentiation Predicts Less Aggression in Response to Direct Provocation Study 1 showed that people who uniquely clarify and distin- guish among their felt emotional experiences are less susceptible todailyaggressionwhentheyfeelangry.Wesoughttoexpandon the association between emotion differentiation and aggressive behavior in Study 2 by exploring how emotion differentiation alters behavioral responses in circumscribed social contexts. Un- likeStudy1,Study2participantsreportedwhetherornotsomeone hurt their feelings (i.e., provocation) and if so, whether they behaved aggressively. This can be construed as an explicit, con- textualized approach to studying the potential value of emotion differentiation in understanding the link between anger and ag- gression. To provide a more sensitive measure of the association betweenemotiondifferentiationandaggression,participantscom- pletedmeasuresofnegativeemotionandaggressivebehavioreach day for a 3-week period (total of 21 waves). We predicted that, comparedtolowdifferentiators,highdifferentiatorswouldbeless Figure 1. Interactive effect between daily anger intensity and negative susceptible to aggression in response to being provoked to feel emotiondifferentiationondailyaggressivetendencies,Study1. angrybyanotherperson. 330 PONDETAL. Method were given a secure website link for an online survey to be completed nightly. For the next 21 days, they were instructed to complete the online survey after 6 p.m. To increase compliance, Participants researchers stressed that receiving full participation credit was Participants consisted of 186 undergraduate students (62% contingentontimelyreportingandwerethenshowntheautomatic women) at a large, mid-Atlantic university. With a mean age of time-and-datestampingofeachdayofdata.Allinformationsub- 23.99(SD(cid:1)9.14),53.7%ofparticipantswereCaucasian,11.8% mitted via online survey was confidential and stored on a secure were Hispanic, 11.3% were Asian American, 7.0% were African server.Adebriefingfollowed. American, 1.6% were Middle Eastern, 1.1% were American In- dian,and6.5%participantsreportedtheirraceas“other.”Students Results receivedresearchcreditforparticipation. Preliminary Analyses Measures Weagainusedmultilevelmodelingtoaccountedforthedata’s Negativeemotiondifferentiation. Participantsreportedhow nestedstructure(HLMVersion6.08,Raudenbush&Bryk,2002; muchtheyfeltembarrassed,disappointed,bored,anxious/nervous, Raudenbush et al., 2000). Participants provided a total of 3,790 and sad each day. Each item was measured on a 7-point scale days of data (M (cid:1) 20.38). Participants followed proper protocol (from not at all to very much). A measure of negative emotion fortimelydailyresponsesfor93.7%ofentries(i.e.,after6p.m.on differentiationwascomputedusingthesameprocedureasinStudy thesamedayandbefore9a.m.thenextday). 1.Largercorrelationscoresindicatedlessdifferentiationofemo- Theaverageintraclasscorrelationfordifferentiationofnegative tions.AFisherrtoztransformationwascomputedonallintraclass emotionswas.39(SD(cid:1).25),indicativeofanacceptablelevelof correlationsbeforefurtheranalysis. variability. To better interpret subsequent findings, we again Whereas Study 1 used a measure of negative affect circum- reverse-scored the scale (multiplying scores by (cid:2)1), such that scribedtohighenergynegativeemotionadjectives,Study2mea- largerscoresreflectedgreateremotiondifferentiation. sured negative emotion using adjectives that spanned both of the AswithStudy1,ourinitialanalysesfocusedonthereliabilityof negative valence quadrants in the circumplex model of emotion thedailymeasureforaggression.Usingathree-levelunconditional (i.e., low vs. high energy; e.g., Barrett, 1998; Russell, 1980; modelwithitems(Level1)nestedwithindays(Level2)anddays Watson & Clark, 1997). Several of the items were negative in nested within people (Level 3), analyses showed that the three valence and high in energy (embarrassed, anxious/nervous) and items for daily aggression had adequate reliability (0.81). The otheritemswerenegativeinvalenceandlowinenergy(i.e.,bored, intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for daily aggression was sad). For this reason, we expected that the average intraclass 0.33,indicatingthat67%ofthevariabilityinaggressivereactions correlationinStudy2wouldbelowerthanthatfromStudy1. iswithin-person. Dailyintensityofanger. Eachday,participantswereasked Next, prior to our primary analyses, we conducted analyses to if someone or something caused them to feel angry and then examinewhetherthepresenceofprovocationepisodesvariedasa participants responded to how angry they felt. Participants com- function of emotion differentiation (grand-mean centered). These pletedaone-itemmeasurethatassessedhowangrytheyfeltona analysesincludedpeoplewhoreportedepisodesofbeingprovoked 7-pointscale(fromnotatalltoextremely).Ifnothingcausedthem by another person and those who did not, with days (Level 1) to feel angry, they received the lowest possible response when nestedwithinpeople(Level2).Theprimedependentmeasurewas askedabouttheintensityoftheiranger(i.e.,scoreof1reflecting dichotomous: Did a participant have an episode where they were notatall). provoked by another person on a day or not? These data were Daily aggression. Participants were given a probe asking analyzed with a nonlinear (Bernoulli) multilevel model that is them if someone provoked them, hurting their feelings. If they sometimes referred to as multilevel logistical regression. These answered yes, subsequent questions were asked about their reac- analyses estimate two types of coefficients: a unit-specific and a tion. Participants completed three items each day that assessed population average. We used unit-specific estimates instead of their aggressive behavior when provoked (i.e., “if someone an- population averages, but it should be noted that the two sets of noyedme,Iwasaggressivetowardthem,”“Isaidnastyorcritical coefficientsprovidedverysimilarresults.Theanalysesestimated thingswhenIwasupset,”and“Ididthingslikeslammedthedoor alog-oddsforeachpersonthatwasconvertedtoaprobability.We when I was upset”). Each item was measured on a 7-point scale found that greater emotion differentiation was associated with a (from not at all to very much). Responses across the three items lower probability of being provoked by another person in daily were summed to form a composite measure of daily aggression, life. withhighernumbersindicatinggreateraggressivebehavior. Theseanalysesresultedinaninverserelationshipbetweenneg- ativeemotiondifferentiationscoresandfrequencyofprovocation Procedure episodes (b (cid:1) (cid:2).16, t(184) (cid:1) (cid:2)2.15, p (cid:1) .03). This provided a startingpointtoexaminingwhetheremotiondifferentiationaltered Participants were given a URL to record their feelings and behavioralresponsestoprovocation. behaviors each day for three weeks. Following an initial training Next, following procedures in Study 1, we employed a multi- sessioninwhichparticipantslearnedhowtocompletetheirdaily levelmodeltotestourprimaryhypothesis,withnegativeemotion questionnaires, participants completed self-report questionnaires differentiation as a Level 2 predictor, daily anger intensity as a reflecting demographic information and personality traits, and Level1predictor,across-levelinteractiontermbetweennegative EMOTIONDIFFERENTIATIONBUFFERSAGGRESSION 331 emotiondifferentiationanddailyangerintensity,anddailyaggres- 43% less daily aggression when they were angry, compared to sion as the outcome of interest. In these analyses, the Level 2 peoplewithlowemotiondifferentiation. predictor(negativeemotiondifferentiation)wasgrand-meancen- tered (Aiken & West, 1991). Daily anger intensity was group- Discussion mean centered (i.e., person-centered), thereby eliminating the in- fluenceofperson-leveldifferencesonparameterestimatesofmean The results of Study 2 replicate and extend our previous find- dailyanger(Nezlek,2001). ings,suggestingthatpeoplewhodifferentiateamongtheirnegative emotionsarerelativelyresilienttotheriskofprovocation.People withgreaterprecisionindescribingandclarifyingfeltexperiences Moderating Effect of Negative Emotion Differentiation report less frequent provocation by other people in daily life. on the Association Between Anger and Aggression Moreover, people who fail to adequately differentiate their emo- Our main prediction was that among people who feel greater tionalstatesarepronetoaggressioninthespecificsituationwhere anger, being more adept at differentiating negative emotional theyareprovokedbyanotherpersonandexperienceintenseangry stateswouldreducetheiraggressivebehavior.Asexpected,anal- emotions. yses revealed a significant Negative Emotion Differentiation (cid:4) Although Studies 1 and 2 showed that emotion differentiation AngerIntensityinteraction,b(cid:1)(cid:2)0.38,t(184)(cid:1)(cid:2)2.68,p(cid:1).009 moderated the association between anger and aggression, it re- (see Figure 2). Main effects for negative emotion differentiation, mainsunclearwhatmaymediatethismoderatingeffect.Emotion b (cid:1) (cid:2)1.29, t(184) (cid:1) (cid:2)2.56, p (cid:1) .01, and anger, b (cid:1) 0.84, differentiation is associated with adaptive coping strategies, such t(184)(cid:1)17.94,p(cid:5).001,werealsosignificantsuchthatpartici- as better regulation of intense emotions (Barrett et al., 2001; pants showing greater differentiation among their negative emo- Kashdanetal.,2010).Highemotiondifferentiatorsmaythenhave tional states reported behaving less aggressively when provoked morecontroloftheiremotionalandbehavioralresponsesinstress- on average, and participants showing greater anger reported fulsituations.Ourfinalstudyexaminedthispossibility. greateraggressionwhenprovokedonaverage. Next, we examined the association between anger and self- Study 3: Emotional Control Mediates the Moderating reported aggressive behavior among participants relatively low Effect of Emotion Differentiation on Aggressive ((cid:2)1 SD) and high ((cid:3)1 SD) in emotion differentiation (Aiken & Tendencies West, 1991). Among participants low in emotion differentiation, therewasastrongpositiveassociationbetweenangerandaggres- InStudy3,weexploredthemechanismunderlyingthemoder- sion, b (cid:1) 1.28, t (cid:1) 7.14, p (cid:5) .001. Among participants who atingeffectofemotiondifferentiationontherelationshipbetween expressed greater emotion differentiation, there was a weaker, anger and aggression. Specifically, we examined whether rela- positiveassociationbetweenangerandaggression,b(cid:1)0.40,t(cid:1) tively high levels of emotional control mediated the interactive 2.21, p (cid:1) .03. People with high emotion differentiation reported effectofemotiondifferentiationandangerintensityonaggression. Becausehighemotiondifferentiatorsaremoreeffectiveatemotion regulation, compared to low differentiators, they may experience greater control of their emotions under conditions of high anger. Participantscompleteddailymeasuresofnegativeemotion,emo- tionalcontrol,andaggressivetendenciesthreetimesaweekfora 25-dayperiod(totalof10waves).Wepredictedthatpeoplewho differentiated more among their negative emotions would be less susceptible to aggression when they felt angry, compared to low differentiators, and that this interaction would be mediated by higherlevelsofdailyemotionalcontrol. Method Participants Participants consisted of 243 undergraduate students (78% women)atalarge,Southern-Atlanticuniversity.Withameanage of19.34(SD(cid:1)2.27),79%ofparticipantswereCaucasian,13.8% were Hispanic, 2.0% were Asian American, 14.2% were African American, .4% were American Indian or Alaskan Native, and 4.4% of participants reported their race as “other.” Students re- ceivedresearchcreditforparticipation. Measures Figure 2. Interactive effect between daily anger and negative emotion Negative emotion differentiation. As in Study 1, partici- differentiationondailyaggression,Study2. pantscompletedthenegativeaffectsubscaleofthePANAS(Wat- 332 PONDETAL. son et al., 1988). The same procedure for calculating negative cies had adequate reliability (0.55). The intraclass correlation emotiondifferentiationinStudies1–2wasused.Smallerintraclass coefficient (ICC) for daily aggressive tendencies was 0.68, sug- correlationsamongdiscreteaffectsindicatedgreateremotiondif- gesting that 32% of the variability in aggressive tendencies is ferentiation. A Fisher r to z transformation was computed on all within-person. intraclasscorrelationsbeforeanyadditionalanalyses. Next, prior to our primary analyses, we conducted analyses to Daily anger intensity. Participants completed a one-item examine whether anger intensity varied as a function of emotion measure that assessed how angry they felt that day on a 7-point differentiation(grand-meancentered).Theseanalysesconsistedof scale(fromnotatalltoextremely). atwo-levelmodelwithdays(Level1)nestedwithinpeople(Level Dailyemotionalcontrol. Participantscompletedaone-item 2). The main effect for negative emotion differentiation signifi- measureassessinghowmuchcontroltheyhadovertheiremotions cantlypredicteddailyangerintensity,b(cid:1)(cid:2)0.37,t(242)(cid:1)(cid:2)4.00, thatday.Participantswereaskedtoindicatehowmuchtheyagreed p (cid:5) .001, such that participants showing greater differentiation with the statement “My emotions got the best of me today” on a among their negative emotional states generally reported less 7-point scale (from not at all to extremely). Responses were intensedailyanger. reverse-scoredsuchthatlargernumbersindicategreaterlevelsof FollowingproceduresinStudies1–2,weemployedamultilevel emotionalcontrol.2 model to test our primary hypothesis, with negative emotion dif- Daily aggressive tendencies. Participants completed the ferentiationasaLevel2predictor,dailyangerintensityasaLevel same abbreviated form of the physical and verbal aggression 1predictor,across-levelinteractiontermbetweennegativeemo- subscales of the Aggression Questionnaire (AQ; Buss & Perry, tiondifferentiationanddailyangerintensity,anddailyaggressive 1992) that was used in Study 1. The items needed rescaling at tendenciesastheoutcomeofinterest.Intheseanalyses,theLevel Level 1, in order to achieve approximately equal error variances 2 predictor (negative emotion differentiation) was grand-mean (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Thus, the items were standardized centered(Aiken&West,1991).Dailyangerintensitywasgroup- priortoformingthecompositeofaggressivetendencies. mean centered (i.e., person-centered), thereby eliminating the in- fluenceofperson-leveldifferencesonparameterestimatesofmean Procedure dailyanger(Nezlek,2001). Participants were given a URL to record their feelings and Moderating Effect of Negative Emotion Differentiation behaviorsthreetimeseachweekfor25days,whichincludedthe negativeemotion,emotionalcontrol,andaggressionitems.Partic- on the Association Between Anger and Aggressive ipantswereinstructedtocompletetheirdailysurveysattheendof Tendencies each day before midnight. To increase compliance, researchers Asexpected,analysesrevealedasignificantNegativeEmotion stressed that receiving full participation credit was contingent on Differentiation (cid:4) Anger Intensity interaction, b (cid:1) (cid:2)0.22, timelyreporting,andthatatime-datestampwouldberecordedon t(241) (cid:1) (cid:2)2.56, p (cid:1) .01 (see Figure 3). The main effect for each log. All information submitted via online survey was confi- negative emotion differentiation was marginally significant, b (cid:1) dentialandstoredonasecureserver.Adebriefingfollowed. (cid:2)0.58, t(241) (cid:1) (cid:2)1.89, p (cid:1) .06, such that participants showing greater differentiation among their negative emotional states re- Results portedfeweraggressivetendenciesonaverage.Themaineffectfor daily anger intensity was significant, b (cid:1) 0.58, t(241) (cid:1) 10.62, Preliminary Analyses p (cid:5) .001, such that participants reporting greater anger also reportedmoreaggressivetendenciesonaverage. Ourmainpredictionwasthatpeoplewhoshowmoredifferen- We next examined the association between anger and daily tiation among their negative emotional states, compared to low aggressive tendencies among participants relatively low ((cid:2)1 SD) differentiators, will be less susceptible to aggressive tendencies and high ((cid:3)1 SD) in the tendency to differentiate their emotions whentheyareangry,whichwouldbemediatedbyhigherlevelsof (Aiken&West,1991).Amongparticipantslowinemotiondiffer- emotionalcontrol.Weagainusedmultilevelmodelingtoaccount forthedata’snestedstructure(Nezlek,2001;Raudenbush&Bryk, 2002; Raudenbush et al., 2000). Participants provided a total of 2We conducted additional analyses to establish the validity of the 1,864 days of data (M (cid:1) 7.67). Participants followed proper emotioncontrolmeasureinStudy3.Participantscompletedadailymea- protocol for timely daily responses for 93.3% of the entries (i.e., sure of cognitive reappraisal, which is one type of emotion regulation completedentriesattheendofeachdaybeforemidnight). strategy(Gross,2008).Participantswereasked,“Howmuchdidyouthink Theaverageintraclasscorrelationfordifferentiationofnegative aboutthepositiveaspectsofnegativeeventsorsituationsinyourlifesince emotionswas.84(SD(cid:1).62),whichindicatesanacceptablelevel the last log?” This item was measured on a 1 (Not at all) to 5 (I was ofvariability.AswithStudies1–2,wereverse-scoredthescaleof constantly thinking these thoughts) scale. Our emotional control item emotion differentiation (i.e., multiplied scores by (cid:2)1), so that significantly related to this type of cognitive reappraisal, b (cid:1) 0.05, t (cid:1) 2.84, p (cid:1) .005, such that participants who reported greater control over largerscoresreflectedgreateremotiondifferentiation. theiremotionsalsoreportedthinkingmoredailyaboutthepositiveaspects As in Studies 1 and 2, our initial analyses focused on the of the negative events in their life. Additionally, a subsample of 164 reliabilityofthedailymeasureforaggressivetendencies.Weagain participantscompletedtheBriefCOPE(Carver,1997)beforetakingpartin usedathree-levelunconditionalmodelwithitems(Level1)nested Study 3. Negative coping strategies at the trait level, such as venting, within days (Level 2) and days nested within people (Level 3). self-distraction,anddenial,werenegativelyrelatedtoourdailyemotional Analyses showed that the four items for daily aggressive tenden- controlitem(p’s(cid:5).0001). EMOTIONDIFFERENTIATIONBUFFERSAGGRESSION 333 Aspredicted,analysesrevealedasignificantNegativeEmotion Differentiation(cid:4)AngerIntensityinteraction,b(cid:1)0.15,t(241)(cid:1) 2.68, p (cid:1) .008 (see Figure 4). Main effects for negative emotion differentiation,b(cid:1)0.34,t(241)(cid:1)3.48,p(cid:1).001,andanger,b(cid:1) (cid:2)0.45,t(241)(cid:1)(cid:2)12.85,p(cid:5).001,werealsosignificantsuchthat participants showing greater differentiation among their negative emotional states reported greater emotional control, and partici- pantsshowinggreaterangerreportedlessemotionalcontrol. We next examined the association between anger and daily emotionalcontrolamongparticipantsrelativelylow((cid:2)1SD)and high ((cid:3)1 SD) in the tendency to differentiate their emotions (Aiken&West,1991).Amongparticipantslowinemotiondiffer- entiation, anger intensity predicted less daily emotional control, b(cid:1)(cid:2)0.55,t(cid:1)(cid:2)11.60,p(cid:5).0001.Whereas,amongpeoplewho expressed a greater tendency to differentiate their emotions, the association between anger and emotional control was a weaker, negativeassociation,b(cid:1)(cid:2)0.36,t(cid:1)(cid:2)6.82,p(cid:5).0001. Wenexttestedwhetherdailyemotionalcontrolpredicteddaily aggressive tendencies, controlling for the main and interactive effects of emotion differentiation and daily anger. As predicted, theassociationbetweendailyemotionalcontrolanddailyaggres- sive tendencies was significant, such that people who reported greater daily emotional control also reported fewer aggressive Figure 3. Interactive effect between daily anger and negative emotion tendenciesonaverage,b(cid:1)(cid:2)0.30,t(242)(cid:1)(cid:2)5.59,p(cid:5).001. differentiationondailyaggressivetendencies,Study3. Last,wetestedformediatedmoderationbyestimatingthe95% confidence interval of the indirect effect using the empirical-M test.Traditionalapproachesfortestingmediationareoftenunder- powered and suffer from inflated Type-I error rates because they entiation,angerintensitypredictedincreaseddailyaggression,b(cid:1) incorrectlyassumethattheproductofcoefficients(operationalized 0.80, t (cid:1) 7.69, p (cid:5) .0001. Among participants who expressed a asab)comprisingtheindirecteffectisnormallydistributed(Bol- greater tendency to differentiate their emotions, the association len & Stine, 1990; MacKinnon, Lockwood, & Williams, 2004; betweenangerandaggressionwasaweaker,positiveassociation, Preacher&Hayes,2008;Shrout&Bolger,2002).Moreover,such b (cid:1) 0.37, t (cid:1) 3.76, p (cid:1) .0002. Participants high in emotion methods are inappropriate for testing mediation with nested data differentiationreported16%lessdailyaggressivetendenciescom- pared to participants low in emotion differentiation, when they wereangry. Mediated Moderation Analyses Next, we investigated whether higher levels of emotional con- trolmediatedtheinteractiveeffectofemotiondifferentiationand angerintensityondailyaggressivetendencies.Becausewealready showedasignificantmoderatingeffectofemotiondifferentiation on the relationship between anger and aggressive tendencies, our next step was to show that emotion differentiation also signifi- cantly moderated the relationship between anger and emotional control (Baron & Kenny, 1986). We used multilevel modeling procedurestoaccountforthenestedstructureofthedata(Nezlek, 2001;Raudenbush&Bryk,2002;Raudenbushetal.,2000). To examine the mediating effect of daily emotional control on the moderating effect between negative emotion differentiation andangeronaggressivetendencies,wefirstemployedamultilevel modelwithnegativeemotiondifferentiationasaLevel2predictor, dailyangerintensityasaLevel1predictor,across-levelinterac- tiontermbetweennegativeemotiondifferentiationanddailyanger intensity,anddailyemotionalcontrolastheoutcomeofinterest.In these analyses, negative emotion differentiation was grand-mean centeredanddailyangerintensitywasgroup-meancentered(i.e., Figure 4. Interactive effect between daily anger and negative emotion person-centered). differentiationondailyemotionalcontrol,Study3. 334 PONDETAL. (Bauer, Preacher, & Gil, 2006). On the other hand, the bitionsagainstviolence(Anderson&Bushman,2002;Berkowitz, empirical-Mtestgeneratesconfidenceintervalsfortheindirect 1983,Berkowitz,1990).Angerisaparticularlyhazardousemotion effectfromcriticalvaluesobtainedbyempiricallysimulatinga because people often regulate it through rumination and venting, series of distributions for the product of two normal random whichultimatelyleadstomoreaggression(Bushman,2002;Bush- variables (MacKinnon et al., 2004). The empirical-M test does man et al., 2001). People require considerable self-regulatory not impose the assumption of normality on the distribution of resources to regulate anger in a way that avoids conflict, which the product. Thus, compared to traditional methods, the may leave them particularly vulnerable to aggressive retaliation empirical-M test provides more power and more accurate (Denson,2009).Therefore,individualdifferencefactorsassociated Type-I error rates for single-level (MacKinnonetal.,2004)and with effective emotion regulation may weaken the link between multilevel (Pituch, Stapleton, & Kang, 2006) designs, and it is angerandaggression. recommendedfortestsofmediationfornestedmodelsinwhichat In this vein, we explored a novel factor that may increase leastonevariablewasassessedattheupperlevel(level2)(Pituch resiliency to aggression when people are provoked to feel anger. &Stapleton,2008).Toconducttheempirical-Mtest,weusedthe Specifically,wehypothesizedthatangrypeoplewhotendtoiden- computer program PRODCLIN, which provided the confidence tify and clarify their discrete negative emotional experiences in interval of the indirect effect (MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams, & nuanced ways will be more resilient to aggression, compared to Lockwood,2007). angry people who differentiate among their negative emotions Aspredicted,theindirectpaththroughdailyemotionalcontrol relativelyless.Priorworkhasshownthatpeoplewhoarebetterat wasstatisticallysignificant,asthe95%confidenceintervaldidnot differentiating their emotions are also better at regulating those includezero((cid:2)0.09to(cid:2)0.01).Theinteractiveeffectofemotion emotions(Barrettetal.,2001;Kashdanetal.,2010;Tugadeetal., differentiationandangeronaggressionwaspartiallymediatedby 2004).Becausepeoplewhoarebetteratdifferentiatingtheirneg- daily emotional control (see Figure 5). Thus, participants who ativeemotionsaremoreemotionallyintelligent,moresensitiveto tendedtodifferentiatemoreamongtheirnegativeemotionsexhib- internal and situational cues, and have a greater inventory of ited a weaker relationship between anger and aggression, in part effective emotion regulation strategies (Barrett & Gross, 2001), becauseoftheirgreatercontrolovertheirdailyemotionalexperi- theymayhavebettercontroloftheiremotionsandtheirbehavior ences. intheheatofstressfulsituations,comparedtolowdifferentiators. They may be less likely to use maladaptive strategies for coping Discussion with anger, compared to low differentiators, which may have implicationsfortheirlowerlevelsofaggression. Study 3 provides converging evidence that negative emotion The current work extends previous research by showing that differentiation moderates aggressive responses to anger. In addi- theseregulatoryadvantagesofemotiondifferentiationhelpbuffer tion, Study 3 offers direct evidence in support of our conceptual angry people from potentially harmful conflicts. The results of modelthatemotiondifferentiationpredictsfewerdailyaggressive three diary studies provided converging evidence that emotion tendencies because people who are better differentiators have differentiation moderates the relationship between anger and ag- better emotional control under anger-provoking conditions. gression. In Study 1, higher dfferentiation of negative emotions Whereaspeoplewhoarelessadeptatdifferentiatingtheirnegative served as a resilience factor against aggressive tendencies for emotionssufferfromlessemotionalcontrolinagivenday,which angry participants. Study 2 replicated this effect, using different puts them at an increased risk of aggressing against others when measuresofemotiondifferentiationandaggressioninresponseto theyfeelangry. provocation. Study 3 replicated and extended Studies 1–2 by giving support to a mechanism underlying this effect. Emotion General Discussion differentiationmoderatedaggressiveresponsesinangrypeople,in part,becauseofitsrelationtobetteremotionalcontrol.Thismay Angerisintimatelylinkedtoaggressionbecauseofitsabilityto have freed up regulatory resources that would normally be used increase arousal and hostile cognitions, while also reducing inhi- toward venting, anger-rumination, and other ineffective coping strategiesforimprovingone’smood.Insupportofourconceptual model,Study3showedthatpeoplewhotendtodifferentiatetheir negativeemotionsreportedgreatercontroloftheirdailyemotions when they were angry, compared to low emotion differentiators. Thisgreateremotionalcontrolplayedasignificantmediatingrole in reducing aggression in angered people who are high emotion differentiators. Self-control processes are important for inhibiting aggression (DeWall&Anderson,2011;Finkel,2007;Slotter&Finkel,2011). Internal characteristics and situational factors that bolster self- controlprotectthenecessarycognitiveresourcesneededforover- riding aggressive impulses. People with low internal self-control, orwhosufferfromregulatorydepletion,aremorelikelytoimpul- Figure5. Mediatingeffectofdailyemotionalcontrolontheinteractive sivelyactouttheiraggressionbecausetheyhavefewerresources effectbetweendailyangerandnegativeemotiondifferentiationondaily forthecognitivereappraisalprocessesrequiredforignoringthose aggressivetendencies,Study3. aggressive urges (DeWall, Finkel, & Denson, 2011). Our results EMOTIONDIFFERENTIATIONBUFFERSAGGRESSION 335 builduponrecenttheoriesbyexposingemotiondifferentiationas oftenincludeillegalbehaviors(e.g.,assaultingorharmingothers one potential internal factor that reduces aggression, in part, by with a weapon), which pose problems in getting institutional protectingthosenecessaryregulatoryresources. reviewboard(IRB)approvaltomeasurethosebehaviors.Aswell, measuringtheincidenceofviolenceposesethicalityissues,suchas whethertoreportparticipantswhoareacleardangertoothersto Limitations and Future Directions a law-enforcement agency. Nonetheless, the dynamics between The current investigation provided consistent support for the generalaggressionandviolencearesimilar,andwewouldexpect hypothesis that emotion differentiation moderates aggressive re- asimilarmoderatingeffectofemotiondifferentiationandangeron sponding among angry people. Despite the consistency of our violence. findings, several limitations may prove beneficial in generating A final direction for future research concerns how emotion futureresearchideas.First,weshowedthatemotiondifferentiation differentiationmayinfluenceprosocialoutcomes.Angerislinked moderatesthelinkbetweenangerandaggression,inpart,because toaggressionthroughthemaladaptivestrategiespeopleuse(e.g., itisassociatedwithincreasesinemotionalcontrol.However,this venting, rumination) to deal with anger, which exhaust precious leaves room for other potential mediators. One possibility is that regulatoryresources(Bushman,2002;Bushmanetal.,2001;Den- people who tend to identify and respond to their emotions in son, 2009). The current work posits that emotion differentiation uniquewaysarealsomoremindfuloftheirconsciousstate.Mind- buffers angry people from aggression because greater emotion fulnessislinkedtoagreaterawarenessofone’semotions,which differentiation is linked to the availability of adaptive coping hasimplicationsforemotionregulationandself-controlprocesses strategies, which decreases the probability of using maladaptive (Brown & Ryan, 2003), particularly in response to threats to the ones.Yet,inthepursuitofmoodenhancementeffects,peoplewho self(Niemiecetal.,2010).Peoplewhoaregoodatdifferentiating feel bad may be motivated to act prosocially. For instance, the theirnegativeemotionsmaybemoremindfulatthetraitlevel.As researchliteratureonhelpingindicatesthatsometimessadpeople well,theprocessofdifferentiatingone’semotionsonaday-to-day willhelpothersinanefforttomakethemselvesfeelbetter(Cial- basismayalsohaveimplicationsforexercisingdailymindfulness. dini, Darby, & Vincent, 1973; Manucia, Baumann, & Cialdini, Futureresearchwouldbenefitfromexploringtraitanddailymind- 1984).Ifhighemotiondifferentiatorsengageinfewerbehaviorsto fulness as a mediator of the interactive effect of emotion differ- change their mood in response to upsetting events, they may entiationandangeronaggression. engageinlessprosocialbehaviorwhendistressed.Incontrast,low Asecondpossiblemediatormightberelatedtoincreasedwork- differentiators,whoexperiencedistressingeventsassimply“bad,” ingmemorycapacity.Barrettandcolleagues(2001,2004)suggest may be most likely to help when they believe that doing so may thatpeoplewhohaveagreatercapacityforworkingmemoryhave improve their mood. Thus, emotion differentiation may predict more executive resources for attending to and identifying emo- less aggressive responses when people are angered, but emotion tional cues, managing emotional knowledge, and coping with differentiation may predict less prosocial behavior when people stressful emotions. Recent neuroimaging studies lend initial sup- aredistressed.Thispossibilityawaitsfutureresearch. port to this hypothesis, showing that brain areas associated with workingmemoryareinvolvedinemotionregulationandpsycho- Conclusion logicalwell-being(Ochsneretal.,2004;Urryetal.,2004).Thus, futureresearchwouldalsobenefitfromexploringworkingmem- Feeling angry is an inevitable part of life. Yet how people ory capacity as a mediator of the interactive effect of emotion behavewhileangrydifferswidelybetweenindividuals.Thepres- differentiationandangeronaggression. entinvestigationexaminedtheutilityofemotiondifferentiationin A second limitation relates to our study being correlational in buffering aggression in people’s naturalistic environment on a nature.Astrengthofourmethodologywasthatwecouldexamine daily basis. This research builds on prior work by helping us to people’sfeelingsacrosstimeandwithintheirownenvironments. understandwhysomeangrypeopledonotlashoutatothers,while However,ourfindingsmaynotrevealcausalrelationships.More- others are at an increased risk of such behavior. Aggression is over,duetoourdailydiarymethod,ourstudiesexhibitedaheavy costly in terms of the toll it takes on our social relationships, relianceonsingle-itemmeasures,whichrisklowreliability.Future psychological health, and even our own safety. This research experimentalworkwouldbenefitfrommanipulatingemotiondif- program sheds light on a novel internal factor that may help to ferentiation and anger in the laboratory and then measuring ag- preventthenegativeconsequencesofaggression. gressionbehaviorally. Athirdlimitationinvolvesthedifferencebetweenviolenceand References aggression.Ourstudyconcernedthetendencytowardgeneraldaily aggression (i.e., harming someone who would be motivated to Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and avoid that harm; Baron & Richardson, 1994). Violence, on the interpretinginteractions.London,UK:Sage. other hand, is intended to cause extreme physical harm (e.g., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych injury, death). All violent acts are aggressive, but not all acts of .53.100901.135231 aggression are instances of violence (Bushman & Huesmann, Baron,R.A.,&Richardson,D.R.(1994).Humanaggression(2nded.). 2010).Thebaserateofviolenceislow,duetoitsextremenature; NewYork:PlenumPress. therefore, we chose general daily aggression as our outcome of Baron,R.M.,&Kenny,D.A.(1986).Themoderator-mediatorvariable interest. We expected more variability in reports of daily aggres- distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and sion across each diary period, which would make it a more statisticalconsiderations.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology, sensitive measure. Second, items that constitute violent behavior 51,1173–1182.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Description: