ebook img

EFFECTS OF RAPTORS ON THE ACTIVITY OF SANDGROUSE PDF

6 Pages·1994·2.2 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview EFFECTS OF RAPTORS ON THE ACTIVITY OF SANDGROUSE

, J Raptor Res. 28(4);236-241 © 1994 The Raptor Research Foundation, Inc. EFFECTS OF RAPTORS ON THE ACTIVITY OF SANDGROUSE Peter N. Ferns and Shelley A. Hinsley School ofPure and Applied Biology, University of Wales, College of Cardiff P.O. Box 915, Cardiff, CF1 STL, United Kingdom — Abstract. Raptors accounted for 55% ofthe disturbance incurred by two species ofsandgrouse (Pterocles alchata and P. orientalis) in breeding habitat in La Mancha, Spain, during spring and summer. The main species of raptors were the golden eagle {Aquila chrysaetos) in spring and Montagu’s harrier (Circus pygargus) in summer. During the breeding season, Montagu’s harriers carried out the majority of their foraging at those times of day when male sandgrouse collected water for their chicks. This presumably enabled the harriers to locate chicks more easily. Sandgrouse lost no more than 0.5 hr of feeding time per day as a result of disturbance by raptors, and yet were still able to rest for at least 3 hr a day at times when they could have been feeding. Predators as a whole were probably the major factor responsible for the different dispersion patterns adopted by sandgrouse at different times of year. Key Words: golden eagle; Montagu’s harrier; Pterocles alchata; Pterocles orientalis; raptor disturbance; sandgrouse; vigilance. — Resumen. -Aves rapaces fueron responsables por 55% de las perturbaciones sufridas por dos especies de gangas (Pterocles alchata and P. orientalis) en sus areas de reproduccion en la Mancha, Espana, durante la primavera y el verano. Las especies de aves de rapina mas prominentes fueron el aguila real (Aquila chrysaetos) en la primavera y el aguilucho cenizo (Circus pygargus) en el verano. Durante la estacion de reproduccion los aguiluchos cenizos llevaron a cabo la mayoria de su forrajeo en las horas del dia en que las gangas machos estaban recogiendo agua para sus polios. Esto supuestamente les permitio a los aguiluchos localizar los polios mas facilmente. Las gangas perdieron no mas de media hora del tiempo de alimentacion diario debido a las perturbaciones por aves de rapina sin embargo estuvieron inactivos por lo menos durante 3 horas al dia cuando hubieran podido estar alimentando. Las aves de rapina en general fueron probablemente el factor mas importante en determinar los distintos partrones de dispersion de las gangas en distintas epocas del ano. [Traduccion Autores] Raptors can exert a significant influence on the rowhawks attacked particular tit (Parus spp.) flocks flocking behavior of birds, both in the short term about once per day on average, and disturbed the and the long term. In response to attacks by spar- birds for only about 2 min. Longer disturbances can rowhawks (Accipiter nisus), redshanks (Tringa tot- be inferred from detailed studies ofmerlins and spar- anus) deserted their winter feeding territories for up rowhawks attacking wintering waders (Page and to an hour and formed flocks, while turnstones (Ar- Whitacre 1975, Boyce 1985, Cresswell and Whit- enaria interpres) reduced their individual spacing from field 1994), though no actual estimates are available three bird lengths to one bird length (Whitfield 1988). in these cases. Myers (1984) found that winter feeding territories In order to derive estimates of the total time lost were abandoned by sanderlings (Calidris alba) in by pin-tailed sandgrouse (Pterocles alchata) and black- years when merlins (Falco columbarius) were more bellied sandgrouse (P. orientalis) as a result of dis- abundant. These studies focused on the influence of turbance by raptors, we recorded the amount oftime raptors on the flocking and vigilance of potential the birds spend being vigilant, the number of inci- prey (see also Metcalfe 1989). The amount of time dents ofdisturbance and the factors responsible. This for which target organisms are prevented from car- was done as part of the compilation of time and rying out other activities has seldom been measured. energy budgets for these two species of sandgrouse In one early study, Morse (1973) found that spar- in Spain. 236 December 1994 Raptor Disturbance of Sandgrouse 237 Study Area and Methods Table 1. Number of raptors observed while compiling Observations were made in semi-arid farmland in the sandgrouse time budgets. province of La Mancha, Spain, in spring (29 March to 21 April) and summer (22 July to 10 August) 1986. Number of Sandgrouse occurred in flocks during the spring, com- prising on average 26 (SD = 23, N = 31) individuals in Sightings pin-tailed sandgrouse, with a maximum flock size of 75, OF Each Most daytime flocks ofblack-bellied sandgrouse contained Raptor 2-7 birds, though slightly larger groups (maximum 28) SUM- roosted together overnight. In summer, both species oc- Spring MER curred in scattered family groups. When compiling time N = N = budgets, we scan-sampled bird activity at 1 min intervals. In both seasons, the day was divided into 10 equal time Raptor Species 23 da 20 da periods. These time periods were longer in summer (92 Black kite {Milvus migrans) 4 min) than in spring (82 min). The field area and study Red kite (M. milvus) 6 methods are described elsewhere (Hinsley 1994, Hinsley and Ferns 1994). A total of over 46 000 bird observations Montagu’s harrier {Circus pygargus) 1 23 were made, involving on average 29 pin-tailed sandgrouse Common buzzard {Buteo buteo) 3 4 and 6 black-bellied sandgrouse each day in spring, and Imperial eagle {Aquila heliaca) 2 family groups of 2-5 pin-tailed sandgrouse in summer (at Golden eagle {A. chrysaetos) 11 least two different families were watched each day, and Booted eagle {Hieraaetus pennatus) 1 over 40 families were studied in total). Every potential Common kestrel {Falco tinnunculus) 1 predator that was observed close enough to the focal birds Peregrine falcon {Falco peregrinus) 1 2 to disturb them was recorded. If the same source of dis- turbance caused more than one group of birds to react, Little owl {Athene noctua) 1 this was treated as a single incident, even if the events occurred a considerable time apart (actual maximum = 7 sandgrouse adopted alert upright postures or min). The observations in the spring were undertaken at a crouched close to the ground (these behaviors were m range of 300-1000 (typically 700 m) to avoid disturbing also recorded as vigilance). the birds, and it was not possible to distinguish gender at The most common identified source ofdisturbance this time of year. Observations imn the summer were un- was raptors. During a total of 43 d of observations, dertaken at a range of 200-800 (typically 400 m). In the summer, groups split up into family parties in which the number of disturbances due to raptors was 60. A males, females and juveniles (1-2-mon-old at the time of further 49 disturbances were attributed to other the observations) were distinguishable. causes. The species of raptors involved (Table 1) To estimate the amount of time that sandgrouse spent differed significantly between spring and summer, being vigilant when disturbed, we performed stepwise re- though the number of incidents was very similar. gression on the percentage of time spent being vigilant (arcsine transformed) in each of the 10 daily time periods The most important species in spring was the golden against the number of records of disturbance caused by eagle. The most important species in summer was all factors that wewere able toidentify. These were divided Montagu’s harrier (golden eagles were absent from into five categories comprising the numbers of the most the area during the summer). Because the same in- common raptors in each season (golden eagle [Aquila chry- dividual raptor may have caused disturbance on more saetos] in spring, Montagu’s harrier [Circus pygargus] in summer); other raptors; other birds (mainly red-legged than one day, there may have been some unavoidable partridges [Alectoris rufa] and great grey shrikes [Lanius pseudoreplication. However, the proportion of dis- excubitor]); mammals (rabbits, sheep and dogs); and hu- turbances caused by the most abundant raptor (com- mans (including noises caused by human activities). This pared with all other birds) in the two seasons was method of measuring the affect of disturbance has the P < advantage of taking into account losses of feeding time significantly different (Fisher’s exact test, 0.005). A through increased alertness even when this lasted for some greater diversity of raptors was thus responsible time after the disturbance ended. for disturbance during the spring (Table 1). Red kites (Milvus migrans) and black kites (M. milvus) Results were recorded in spring but not summer, both being A In the absence of disturbance, sandgrouse vigi- migrants in the area. flock of 106 black kites that lance was typically restricted to short bouts with the passed over on migration on the evening of 3 August head up. The duration of these bouts was longest in was not included since it caused no disturbance. male pin-tailed sandgrouse accompanying chicks (.x Golden eagles were observed circling quite high = 6.5 sec, SD = 3.9, N = 47). When disturbed, (about 30-50 m) over the fields and hill slopes, and 238 Peter N. Ferns and Shelley A. Hinsley VoL. 28, No. 4 also perched on vantage points overlooking the fields. Both golden eagles and red kites regularly take birds the size of adult sandgrouse (Cramp and Simmons 1979). In spring, a juvenile imperial eagle {Aquila heliaca) took a taxidermic mount of a sandgrouse (used to measure operative temperature) from the ground and was only prevented from flying off with it by the attached data-logger. Montagu’s harriers and red kites quartered the fields at a height of only a few meters, and occasionally circled at a greater elevation. In summer, Montagu’s harriers were ob- served attacking young sandgrouse, but not adults. The young of ground-nesting birds form a consid- erable part of this harrier’s diet during the summer months (Cramp and Simmons 1979). budget Vigilance of adult sandgrouse in each time period (Fig. 1) peaked during the middle of the day in time spring, and during the morning and evening in sum- mer. The gap between the summer peaks coincided with a period of reduced activity of both raptors and sandgrouse sandgrouse in the middle of the day, induced by high temperature. The vigilance of juvenile sandgrouse reached a peak earlier in the morning than that of of adults. Adult males spent more time being vigilant in summer than in spring (19% of daylight hours percentage compared with 37o), presumably because of the vul- nerability of their ofTspring during the former pe- riod. This difference was statistically significant and H = P = (Kruskal-Wallis test, \\.?>, 0.001. While male and female vigilance in the 10 daily raptors, time periods were correlated significantly during summer = P < N = (r 0.69, 0.02, 10), juvenile of vigilance was not correlated with that of either par- ent (r = 0.46 with males and r = 0.12 with females, Number P > 0.10 in both cases). This is largely because juveniles spent most of the day feeding or resting, relying on their parents to act as sentinels (especially their fathers). They showed most alertness when he was away collecting water for them during the morn- ing and when he returned. Juveniles were less sen- sitive to disturbance by Montagu’s harriers in the evenings (Fig. 1), probably because the latter spent most oftheir time hunting rabbits during this period Figure 1. Selected raptor activity (numbers) and sand- of the day. grouse vigilance (percentage of time budget) throughout Sandgrouse responded to raptors differently in the daylight period. The total number of raptors is shown in spring (a), the total number ofraptors other than Mon- spring and summer, and this reflected the different tagu’s harriers (b), and number of Montagu’s harriers in dispersion pattern they adopted in the breeding sea- summer son because ofthe vulnerability oftheir young (Lima (c). and Dill 1990). In neither spring nor summer did the sandgrouse form denser groups when disturbed; if anything the flocks tended to split up. In spring. December 1994 Raptor Disturbance of Sandgrouse 239 they merely flew to another of the several fields in that caused by humans {t = 4.8, P = 0.003). The which they normally foraged during the course of larger size of this species apparently made it less the day. In summer, the first response of both adults vulnerable to disturbance by most raptors. and young to the presence of a raptor was to crouch and freeze. If a family group was located by a raptor, the adults usually mobbed it before flying off, while Discussion the young ran or flew into the nearest dense vege- While the spring flocks of sandgrouse may have tation. Consequently, their activity was affected been reasonably easy for golden eagles and other mainly by a direct loss of time available for feeding, raptors to locate, they did not provide a particularly We preening, drinking and other activities. saw three easy target as indicated by the rarity of the attacks direct attacks by Montagu’s harriers on juvenile we witnessed. Young sandgrouse during the breed- sandgrouse, all unsuccessful, and adults with young ing season were much more vulnerable, though they mobbed Montagu’s harriers on four additional oc- must have been more difficult to locate in the well- casions. Male vigilance showed no significant cor- dispersed family groups. It is thus not surprising relation with disturbance factors, mainly because that Montagu’s harriers concentrated most of their they spent such a high proportion of the time alert, foraging activity in the period when male sandgrouse even in the absence of any obvious disturbance (19% returned with water from the waterholes, since this of daylight hours spent alert, compared with 12% in provided them with a potential clue to the where- females). abouts of young. Such predation pressure may have Since disturbance often caused sandgrouse to fly led in turn (c.f. Lima and Dill 1990) to a wider in spring, there was significantly more sandgrouse spread ofwatering times amongst males in this area, alertness in time periods in which the birds spent including evening watering. The two species of more time flying (r = 0.80, P < 0.01, N ^ 10 in all sandgrouse we observed tended to forage in difTerent cases). In summer, reduced levels of alertness in both types ofhabitat (Hinsley 1994), but we do not believe sexes of pin-tailed sandgrouse were associated with that this choice was influenced by raptor activity. periods of resting (males, r = —0.68, P < 0.05; The pin-tailed sandgrouse was the more vulnerable females, r = —0.67, P < 0.05). Differences in flock species, but it foraged in open fields where it was sizes were not significant in different time periods, more visible than on the hill slopes favored by black- nor was alertness significantly correlated with flock bellied sandgrouse. As suggested by Lima and Dill size in either species. Therefore, alertness within (1990), vigilance costs were higher during the breed- seasons was not significantly correlated with any ing season. Adults with young were also prepared variable that might have confounded its correlation to run greater risks by mobbing raptors during the with disturbance, including flock size or distance to summer. cover (Elgar 1989). Predation of young sandgrouse probably repre- Stepwise regression analysis indicated that a high sents a major source of mortality (Maclean 1985). proportion of the time spent being vigilant was ac- However, young are likely to be even more vulner- counted for by the observed sources of disturbance, able to raptors at water holes (Cade 1965) and this despite the small sample size (TV = 10 in all cases). may explain why males continued to carry water to In spring, 48% of the variation in pin-tailed sand- them even after they had become capable of flying grouse vigilance between time periods was accounted to water holes for themselves. Raptors recorded at- = for by the overall number of raptors recorded {t tacking or consuming sandgrouse in other areas in- 3.2, P < 0.02) and 20% by human disturbances {t clude pallid harrier (Circus macrourus), golden eagle, = 2.4, P = 0.05). In summer, 46% offemale vigilance greater kestrel (Falco rupicohides), sooty falcon (F. was accounted for by raptors other than Montagu’s concolor), lanner falcon (F. biarmicus) and Barbary harriers {t = 2.5, P < 0.05) and 257o by mammals falcon (F.pelegranoides) (Ali and Ripley 1983, Cramp {t = 2.5, P < 0.05). Also in summer, 68% ofjuvenile 1985, Urban et al. 1986). vigilance was accounted for by the number of Mon- The amount oftime males with young spent being tagu’s harriers {t = 4.1, P = 0.003). Fifty-seven vigilant (19% of daylight hours) was much less than percent ofblack-bellied sandgrouse vigilance in spring that of male barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis) es- was accounted for by disturbance caused by dogs, corting their broods (48%) (Forsland 1993). In both sheep and rabbits (^ = 5.6, P = 0.001), and 19% by species, the males devoted more time to vigilance 240 Peter N. Ferns and Shelley A. Hinsley VoL. 28, No. 4 than the females. Walters (1990) found no difference Acknowledgments in the response of long-toed lapwings {Vanellus cras- We are most grateful to the Fundacion Jose Maria sirostris) and blacksmith plovers (V. armatus) ac- Blanc for allowing us to work on their estate in Spain and cording to whether or not they had young, but they tsoanDd.grHo.usTeh.oSmaAsHfowraslocsautpinpgorstuecdh baygoaodSEplRaCce rteossetaurdchy did respond more strongly to harriers than to eagles. studentship. We would also like to thank three referees The intensity of predatory disturbance at our study for numerous improvements to the manuscript. site was relatively low compared with some others. For example, at Bolinas Lagoon in California, a single merlin mounted about 19 attacks per day on Literature Cited shorebirds (Page and Whitacre 1975). However, the Ali, S. and S.D. Ripley. 1983. Handbook of the birds 5000 potential prey in this area were divided into of India and Pakistan. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, groups of at most several hundred, and therefore the U.K. disturbance incurred per bird was much lower. Cal- Boyce, D.A. 1985. Merlins and the behavior of win- vo (1993) recorded more than 10 disturbances per tering shorebirds. Raptor Res. 19:94-96. day (an order of magnitude more than that caused Cade, T.J. 1965. Relations between raptors and colum- biform birds at a desert water hole. Wilson Bull. 77: by raptors at our site) at breeding colonies of collared 340-345. pratincoles {Glareola pratincola) in southwest Spain. Calvo, B. 1993. Influences of agricultural land-use and Black kites and common kestrels {Falco tinnunculus) habitat modification on the breeding biology and con- were responsible for most pratincole disturbance, servation of collared pratincoles Glareola pratincola in though Montagu’s harriers were also seen taking SW Spain. Ph.D. dissertation, Univ. Glasgow, U.K. chicks. Cramp, S. [Ed.]. 1985. The birds of the western Pale- Most of the time sandgrouse spent being vigilant arctic. Vol. 4. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, U.K. as a result of the activities of raptors, humans, and AND K.E.L. Simmons. [Eds.]. 1979. The birds other mammals was time they would otherwise have of the western Palearctic. Vol. 2. Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, U.K. spent feeding. In the middle of the day, when birds Cresswell, W. AND D.P. Whitfield. 1994. The effects rested in the shade of shrubs, alertness and distur- of raptor predation on wintering wader populations at bance levels were relatively low and the small amount the Tyninghame estuary, southeast Scotland. Ibis 136: ofdisturbance that did occur then was usually caused 223-232. by red-legged partridges. Elgar, M.A. 1989. Predator vigilance and group size None of the results described here suggest that in mammals and birds: a critical review ofthe empirical disturbance by raptors presented sandgrouse with evidence. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 64:13-33. any difficulty in meeting their daily energy require- Forsland, P. 1993. Vigilance in relation to brood size ments. The time of the year when the highest pro- and predator abundance in the barnacle goose, Branta portion of the day was occupied by foraging was the leucopsis. Anim. Behav. 45:965-973. spring, and even then black-bellied sandgrouse were Hinsley, S.A. 1994. Daily time budgets and activity patterns of sandgrouse (Pteroclididae) in contrasting still able to devote over 3 hr, and pin-tailed sand- grouse over 4 hr, to resting at times when they could arid habitats in Spain and Israel. /. Arid Environ. 26: 373-382. presumably have been feeding. The above regres- AND P.N. Ferns. 1994. Time and energy bud- sions enable us to estimate that raptors in spring gets of breeding males and females in sandgrouse Pter- caused pin-tailed sandgrouse to lose about 24 min ocles species. Ibis 136:261-270. of foraging time per day. Mammals caused black- Lima, S.L. and L.M. Dill. 1990. Behavioral decisions bellied sandgrouse to lose 10 min per day at the same made under the risk of predation: a review and pro- time of year. In summer, Montagu’s harriers caused spectus. Can. J. Zool. 68:619-640. juvenile pin-tailed sandgrouse to lose 9 min per day. Maclean, G.L. 1985. Sandgrouse: models of adaptive Other raptors caused adult females to lose about 25 compromise. S. Afr. J. Wild! Res. 15:1-6. Metcalfe, N.B. 1989. Flocking preferences in relation min. Raptors may be the largest single source of to vigilance benefits and aggression costs in mixed- disturbance, but man and other mammals also caused species shorebird flocks. Oikos 56:91-98. some, even in this thinly populated area. Predators Morse, D.H. 1973. Interactions between tit flocks and as a whole were probably a major factor responsible sparrowhawks Accipiter nisus. Ibis 115:591-593. for the different dispersion patterns adopted by pre- Myers, J.P. 1984. Spacing behavior of nonbreeding breeding and breeding sandgrouse. shorebirds. Pages 271-321 in J. Burger and B.L. Olla December 1994 Raptor Disturbance of Sandgrouse 241 [Eds.], Behavior ofmarine animals. Vol. 6. Shorebirds. wings: field evidence of discriminative abilities. Wilson NY Migration and foraging. Plenum Press, U.S.A. Bull. 102:49-70. Page, G. and D.F. Whitacre. 1975. Raptor predation Whitfield, D.P. 1988. Sparrowhawks Accipiter msus on wintering shorebirds. Condor 77:73-83. affect the spacing behavior of wintering turnstone Ar- Urban, E.K., C.H. Fry and C.S. Keith. [Eds.]. 1986. enaria interpres and redshank Tringa tetanus. Ibis 130. The birds of Africa. Vol. 2. Academic Press, London, 284-287. U.K. Walters, J.R. 1990. Anti-predatory behavior of lap- Received 22 February 1994; accepted 30 July 1994

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.