JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp Effects of contextual support on preschoolers’ accented speech comprehension Sarah C. Creela,⇑, Dolly P. Rojob, Angelica Nicolle Paulladaa aUniversityofCalifornia,SanDiego,LaJolla,CA92093,USA bUniversityofTexasatAustin,Austin,TX78712,USA a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Articlehistory: Young children often hear speech in unfamiliar accents, but Received24November2015 relativelylittleresearchcharacterizestheircomprehensioncapac- Revised29January2016 ity. The current study tested preschoolers’ comprehension of Availableonline4March2016 familiar-accentedversusunfamiliar-accentedspeechwithvarying levelsofcontextualsupportfromsentenceframes(fullsentences Keywords: vs.isolatedwords)andfromvisualcontext(foursalientpictured Accent alternativesvs.theabsenceofsalientvisualreferents).Thefamiliar Accentedspeech Speechcomprehension accent advantage was more robust when visual context was Wordrecognition absent,suggestingthatpreviousfindingsofgoodaccentcompre- Adverselisteningconditions hension in infants and young children may result from ceiling Eyetracking effects in easier tasks (e.g., picture fixation, picture selection) relativetothemoredifficulttasksoftenusedwitholderchildren andadults.Incontrasttopriorworkonmispronunciations,where mosterrors were novelobject responses, children inthe current study did not select novel object referents above chance levels. Thissuggeststhatsomepropertyofaccentedspeechmaydissuade childrenfrominferringthatanunrecognizedfamiliar-but-accented word has a novel referent. Finally, children showed detectable accentprocessingdifficultydespitepresumedincidentalcommu- nityexposure.Resultssuggestthatpreschoolers’accentedspeech comprehension is still developing, consistent with theories of protracteddevelopmentofspeechprocessing. (cid:1)2016ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved. ⇑ Correspondingauthor. E-mailaddress:[email protected](S.C.Creel). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.018 0022-0965/(cid:1)2016ElsevierInc.Allrightsreserved. S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 157 Introduction Withincreasingglobalization,listenersoftenencounterspeechinregionalorforeignaccentsdif- ferentfromtheirown.Theselistenersincludeyoungchildren.Althoughthereisawealthofresearch on the effects of unfamiliar accents on speech comprehension in adults, much less is known about young children’s accented speech comprehension, as observed by Cristià et al. (2012) in a recent reviewarticle.Thisisasignificantgapinknowledgegiventhataccentedspeechprovidesachallenge to young children’s developing abilities in speech perception and word recognition (Bent, 2014; Nathan,Wells,&Donlan,1998).Moreimportant,asCristiàetal.(2012)alsonoted,itpreventsthefield from having a full developmental picture of accented speech comprehension from infancy through childhoodthroughyoungandolderadulthood.Thecurrentstudyisanattempttodecreasethisgap. Littleresearchcharacterizesaccentedspeechcomprehensioninyoungchildren(with,asdiscussed shortly,afewexceptions),andexistingfindingsarespreadacrossparadigmsanddonotalwayspro- duce identical conclusions. Therefore, it is not clear how well children comprehend unfamiliar- accentedspeakers who theyencounter in everyday environmentsor what situationscreate greater orlessereaseincomprehension.Thecurrentstudysoughttocharacterizeaccentedspeechprocessing inpreschool-aged(3-to5-year-old)children.Inparticular,weaimedtoassesstheroleofdifferent typesofcontextualsupport—sententialsemanticcontextandvisualscenecontext—andthepossibility thatchildrenwillmisidentifyaccentedfamiliarwordsasnovelwords. Accentedspeechmaybemorechallengingforchildreninthattheyaregenerallymoresusceptible thanadultstoadverselisteningconditionsinspeechcomprehension(e.g.,Fallon,Trehub,&Schneider, 2000,2002).Thus,perceptualdifficultiesintroducedbyspeechinunfamiliaraccentsmaybeespecially challengingforchildren.Anadditionalreasonwhyaccentedspeechmaybeespeciallychallengingfor childrenisthat,becausepreschool-agedchildrenarerapidlyacquiringvocabulary,theyneedtoiden- tify potentially novel word forms. This means that if childrenare overly sensitive to pronunciation deviationsfromfamiliarwords,theymaymisidentifyfamiliarbutaccentedwords(e.g.,‘‘peeg”/pig/ forpig/pɪg/)asnovelwords.Ontheotherhand,ifchildrenacceptlargevariationsinpronunciation, theyriskfailuretodistinguishsimilarwords(e.g.,pick/pɪk/vs.peek/pik/).Thenextsectionreviews whatisknownaboutaccentedspeechrecognitionininfants,youngchildren,andadults. Accentedspeechprocessingduringdevelopment Infants’andtoddlers’accentedspeechprocessing Althoughwedonothavespaceforanexhaustivereview,itshouldbenotedthatmostdevelopmen- talresearchonwordorwordformrecognitioninunfamiliaraccents,oraccent-likevariabilitysuchas deliberate mispronunciations, has focused on children up to around 2years of age (accents: Best, Tyler, Gooding, Orlando, & Quann, 2009; Mulak, Best, Tyler, Kitamura, & Irwin, 2013; Schmale, Cristià, Seidl, & Johnson, 2010; Schmale & Seidl, 2009; van Heugten & Johnson, 2014, in press; van Heugten, Krieger, & Johnson, 2015; mispronunciations: Swingley, 2003; Swingley & Aslin, 2000, 2002;White&Aslin,2011;White&Morgan,2008),withmuchlessresearchonolderchildren(with theonlyexceptionsbeing,tothebestofourknowledge,thefollowing:accents:Bent,2014;Bent& Atagi, 2015; Nathan et al., 1998; mispronunciations: Creel, 2012; Newton & Ridgway, in press). Cristiàetal.(2012),inreviewingaccentprocessingstudiesinvariousagegroups,notedthatresearch onchildren’saccentprocessing,comparedwiththatofinfants/toddlersandyoungadults,is‘‘lesswell- documented”(p.7).(Notethatthereismoreresearchwitholderchildrenonrecognizingorrespond- ingtothepresenceofaccents:Floccia,Butler,Girard,&Goslin,2009;Girard,Floccia,&Goslin,2008; Kinzler & DeJesus, 2013; Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2007; Kinzler, Shutts, DeJesus, & Spelke, 2009; Wagner,Clopper,&Pate,2014). Infants,bysomeaccounts,showgoodwordorwordformrecognitionafter12or13monthsoflife. Schmaleetal.(2010)andSchmaleandSeidl(2009)testedinfants’recognitionoffamiliarizedword forms and found that infants recognize words despite unfamiliar regional accents by 12 months (Schmaleetal.,2010)andforeignaccentsby13.5months(Schmale&Seidl,2009).Thesewordform recognitionstudiesfitwithSwingleyandAslin’s(2000,2002)findingsfromalooking-while-listening paradigmthatchildrenaged15monthsandoldershowsomedegreeofrecognitionofnon-canonically 158 S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 pronouncedwords(intheircasedeliberatelymispronouncedwords);whenhearing‘‘Lookatthetog!” childreninSwingleyandAslin(2002)lookedmoreatapictureddogthanapicturedshoe—although they looked at the dog even more when dog was correctly pronounced. This result suggests that infantsareatleastpartlyawareofthesimilaritybetweencorrectlypronouncedandmispronounced forms(see White& Morgan,2008, for furtherexploration). Withadaptation,infants mayrecognize accented forms of familiar words; van Heugten and Johnson (2014) showed accented word form recognition in 15-month-olds following exposure to an accented reading of a familiar book, and WhiteandAslin(2011)foundthat19-month-oldswhowereexposedtoavowelshiftaccent(‘‘socks” pronounced as ‘‘sacks”) visually fixated the correct pictures when hearing artificially accented ver- sionsoffamiliarwordssuchas‘‘bottle”(pronouncedas‘‘battle”).By2yearsofageatthelatest,chil- drencanrecognizefamiliarwordsinunfamiliaraccentswithoutexposuretotheaccent(25months forCanadiantoddlershearingAustralian-accentedEnglish:vanHeugtenetal.,2015;19monthsfor AustraliantoddlershearingJamaican-accentedEnglish:Mulaketal.,2013)andnewlylearnedwords if theyhave brief exposure to the accent (Schmale,Cristia, & Seidl, 2012). By 2.5years,childrenno longer need accent exposure for recognizing familiar words (van Heugten & Johnson, in press) or newly learned words (Schmale, Hollich, & Seidl, 2011). Interestingly, children in van Heugten and Johnson’s(inpress)studyweremoresuccessfulatrecognizingtheaccentedwords(i.e.,theyfixated thenamedpicturemorerobustly)whenwordswereembeddedinfull-sentencecontexts,hintingthat at least one sort of context—acoustic–phonetic sentential context—may facilitate accent comprehension. Finally,arecentstudybySchmale,Seidl,andCristia(2015)suggeststhattoddlerscanbeinducedto recognizeaccentedtokensofnewlylearnedwordsbysimplyexposingthemtoawiderangeoftalkers (or even pictures of talkers) in their own accent. In that study, 2-year-olds were exposed to four American-accented talkers either reading passages or silently waving and gesturing. Then children werepresentedwithanovellabelingeventinafamiliaraccent,followedbyatwo-alternativechoice wherethesameobjectwaslabeledinanunfamiliaraccent.Inbothexposureconditions,childrengen- eralized word recognition to the unfamiliar accent. This differed from previous findings (Schmale etal.,2012),where2-year-oldswithoutexposuretotheunfamiliaraccentfailedtogeneralize.This implies that very young children, given a minimal amount of speech variability (or even visually impliedspeechvariability),appeartobroadentherangeofpronunciationstheywillacceptforaword. Whetherthisbroadeningoverextendstoacceptingaccent-atypicalpronunciations,orconfusingpre- viouslydistinctwords(e.g.,/pɪk/and/pik/),isasyetunknown.Takentogether,thesestudiesofinfants andtoddlersimplythatveryyoungchildrenarewellontheirwaytocomprehendingspeechinunfa- miliaraccents. Accentedspeechprocessinginpreschoolandprimaryschoolchildren Whereas the work reviewed above suggests excellent accented speech comprehension by 2 to 2.5years ofage,studies ofolderchildrenaremore equivocal. Becauseinfantresearchand research onolderchildrentendtousesomewhatdifferentparadigms,itisdifficulttocompareresultsacross ages(seeCreel&Quam,inpress,forfurtherdiscussion).Ontheonehand,preschool-aged(3-to5- year-old) children hearing mispronounced words in their native language phonological system are fairlyaccurateinselectingthematchingitemoutoffourpicturesinadisplay;Creel(2012)foundthat whenchildrenhearda single-phonological-featuremispronunciation(‘‘fesh”/feʃ/forfish /fɪʃ/),they selectedthetargetpicture(thefish)nearly90%ofthetimefromanarraycontainingpicturesofafish, two other familiar objects, and a novel object. Even when mispronunciations were more egregious (/fɑʃ/),theystillselectedthetargetpictureapproximately60%ofthetime.Thissuggeststhat,similar to Swingley and Aslin’s (2000, 2002) findings with toddlers, children readily detect the similarity betweenthemispronouncedversionandthewordrepresentation.Justasinterestingwaswhathap- penedwhenchildreninCreel’s(2012)studydidnotselectthetargetpicture(keepinginmindthat targetpictureselectionswerethepredominantresponseoverall);lookingonlyatthesenontargettri- als,childrenchoseanovelobjectroughly70%ofthetime,muchhigherthanthechancerateof33% (nontargetchoiceswere1novelpicturevs.2familiarpictures).Thislatterfindingimpliesthatwhen childrendonotrecognizeamispronouncedwordasfamiliar,theyaremostlikelytoassumethatitisa S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 159 novelform(e.g.,Markman&Wachtel,1988;seealsoHalberda,2003).Thisraisesthepossibilitythat unfamiliar-accentedwords,ifunrecognized,willbetreatedasnovelwords. AlthoughCreel’s(2012)studysuggeststhat3-to5-year-oldscomprehendunfamiliarpronuncia- tions fairly well, research with word or sentence repetition tasks, largely using real accents, paints a different picture (Bent, 2014; Bent & Atagi, 2015; Nathan et al., 1998; Newton & Ridgway, in press).Nathanetal.(1998)tested4-and7-year-oldBritishEnglishspeakers’abilitiestorepeatiso- latedwordsspokeninafamiliaraccent(LondonEnglish)versusanunfamiliaraccent(GlasgowEng- lish).Theyfoundthatbothgroupsperformedlesswellontheunfamiliaraccent(57%vs.88%forthe familiar accent), with improvement from the 4-year-olds (43%) to the 7-year-olds (71%). More recently,Bent(2014)extendedalineofresearchonaccentedspeechrecognitioninadultstoencom- passyoung(4-to7-year-old)children.Familiar-accented(AmericanEnglish)orunfamiliar-accented (Korean-accented English) words were presented at a +5dB signal-to-noise ratio with speech- shapednoise.Bentfoundthatchildrenweremuchlessaccurateatrepeatingwordsthanadultsand were far less accurate for foreign-accented words (44% accuracy) than native-accented words (80% accuracy).NewtonandRidgway(inpress),usingasentencerepetitioninnoisetaskwith6-and7- year-olds,foundmuchhighersignal-to-noisethresholdsforaccuraterepetitionofartificiallyaccented speechthanfamiliar-accentedspeech. Thus, in contrast to studies of infants and toddlers, studies of older children (with Creel, 2012, being an exception) suggest that they still have greater difficulty than adults in comprehending accented speech. These findings concur with other research suggesting that children are less adept than adults at comprehending speech in ideal (Hazan & Barrett, 2000; Ohde & Haley, 1997) and non-ideal(Fallonetal., 2000, 2002) listeningconditions.Findingsof non-adult-like accent compre- hension in young children are also consistent with accounts of protracted perceptual development (e.g.,Creel&Jimenez,2012;seealsoBent&Atagi,2015),whichpositthatfullyadult-likespokenlan- guageprocessingrequiresmanyyearsofperceptuallearning. Remainingquestions Currentevidenceismixedwithregardtothecircumstanceswherechildrensucceedinrecognizing accentedspeech.Onepotentialexplanationfordifferencesacrossagesandtasksisthattasksthatpro- videmorecontextualsupportleadtobetterperformance.Forexample,hearinganaccentedwordand selectingitsreferentfromasmallsetofpictures(Creel,2012;vanHeugtenetal.,2015)islikelymuch easierthanrecognizinganaccentedwordintheabsenceofanyvisualcontext(e.g.,Bent,2014;Nathan etal.,1998).Similarly,itislikelyeasiertorecognizeanaccentedwordsuchas‘‘cows”inthesentence ‘‘Thefarmermilkedthecows”thanwhenhearing‘‘cows”inalesspredictablesentencecontextoras anacousticallyisolatedtoken(seevanHeugten&Johnson,inpress,forevidencethat28-month-olds recognizeaccentedwordsbetterwhenprecededbyanaccentedsentencecontext).Thus,thecurrent studyteststhecontextualsupporthypothesisthatcontext—here,semanticsentencecontextandvisual scene context—helps children to comprehend accented speech, potentially explaining discrepant resultsinthedevelopmentalliterature. Asecondissueconcernsthetypesoferrorschildrenmakewhentheymisperceiveaccentedspeech. DotheytendtothinktheintendedreferentisanovelobjectlikechildreninCreel’s(2012)mispronun- ciationstudy?Iftheydonotmakenoveltyerrors,whyisthisthecase?Althoughlittleempiricalwork existstosuggestanswerstothisquestion,onepossibilityisthatunfamiliar-accentedpronunciations might not be sufficiently different from the familiar-accented pronunciations to trigger a novelty response,suchthatwordsareslightlyhardertorecognize(leadingtoslightlymoreerrors)butalso donotregisterasdistinctlydifferentfromanyknownwords.Thatis,theymayregisterthataccented ‘‘pig” is similar to /pɪg/, /pig/, and /pik/, but it is not a clear instance of the novel word form /pig/. Another possibility is that children may be more likely to dismiss unfamiliar-accented speakers’ knowledgeofnovelobjectnames(e.g.,Corriveau,Kinzler,&Harris,2013),dissuadingthemfrommak- ingnovelobjectselectionsforunrecognizedaccentedwords. 160 S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 Thecurrentstudy These questions were addressed using a recently developed set of sentences, the Sentences for Studying Accent Understanding in Child Experiments (SAUCE). Sentences (described in more detail in Experiment 1) were recorded by 10 talkers with accents familiar to monolingual English- speakingchildreninourarea(CaliforniaaccentsinthesouthwesternUnitedStates)andby10talkers withlessfamiliaraccents(MexicanSpanishaccents). ThestimuliwererecordedandstudieswereruninSanDiegoCounty,California.SanDiegoCounty hasaSpanish-speakingpopulationof25%(Ryan,2013),althoughonly11%(i.e.,(cid:1)40%ofthe25%)of residents are estimated to speak English less than ‘‘very well” (County of San Diego, 2013); this implies,amongotherthings,thepresenceofaforeignaccent.Presumably,someofthosewhospeak English‘‘verywell”arefluentenoughtohavenoidentifiableaccent,buteventhe11%figuresuggests thatmonolingualchildreninourareaarelikelytohavehadincidentalexposuretoMexicanSpanish accents(seeFloccia,DelleLuche,Durrant,Butler,&Goslin,2012,andHoward,Carrazza,&Woodward, 2014,foreffectsofcommunityaccentorlanguageexposure),soifanythingaccentedspeechcompre- hensiondifficultyshouldbeameliorated.Suchlimitedcommunityexposureiscommoninanumber ofmajorcitiesorothercommunitieswithimmigrantpopulationsand,thus,representsanimportant situationfor scientificinvestigation. Furthermore,Spanishiscurrentlythemostprevalentlanguage besidesEnglishintheUnitedStates(Ryan,2013)and,thus,islikelythemostprevalentforeignaccent (or,moreaccurately,groupofforeignaccents). Fourexperimentstestedpreschool-agedchildren’scomprehensionofaccentedspeechwithvary- ingdegreesofcontextualsupport.Thefirsttwoexperimentsusedpicturepointingaccuracyandvisual worldeyetracking(Tanenhaus,Spivey-Knowlton,Eberhard,&Sedivy,1995;Trueswell,Sekerina,Hill, &Logrip,1999)toevaluatechildren’scomprehensionofwordsinsentences(Experiment1)andwords inisolation(Experiment2)whenthevisualscenelimitspossibilitiestoasmallsetofpicturedrefer- ents. The next two experiments used children’s word repetition responses to gauge recognition of words in sentences (Experiment 3) and words in isolation (Experiment 4) when there is no visual scenepresenttoconstrainchildren’sinterpretations. Experiment1 Thefirstexperimentassessedunfamiliar-accentedversusfamiliar-accentedtargetwordrecogni- tioninthepresenceofbothsententialandvisualcontexts;wordsoccurredinfullsentences,andchil- dren selected the named picture from a display containing only four pictures. The target words childrenheard were selected from a larger set of recorded sentences, where the selection criterion washightargetwordtranscriptionaccuracybyadultlisteners(see‘‘Stimuli”sectionbelow).Ifchil- dren’s accent comprehension is aided by visual context, performance should be equally good (high accuracyandrapidvisualfixations)forbothaccents.Iftheyareaidedbysentencesensicality,sensical sentence performance should be good in both familiar and unfamiliar accents, with lower perfor- mance(loweredaccuracyandslowervisualfixations)fornonsensicalsentencesinunfamiliarversus familiaraccents. Method Participants Monolingual English-speaking preschool-aged children (N=32, 16 female and 16 male, M =4.61years,SD=0.69,range=3.09–5.88)fromlocalpreschoolandday-carefacilitiestookpart age in the study. In this and the experiments that follow, parents reported that their children received noexposuretoanylanguagebesidesEnglishathome. Stimuli Stimuliwerefullsentencesendinginatargetword.Sentenceswereineitheranaccentfamiliarto monolingual English-speaking children in the region (Californian English) or a less familiar accent S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 161 (MexicanSpanish-accentedEnglish).ThefullsetofSAUCEsentencescontains100sensical,100non- sensical, and 100 nonword-containing sentences recorded by 20 speakers (half U.S.-accented, half Spanish-accented;halffemale,halfmale).TargetwordsforSAUCEsentencesconsistedofnamesfor colorpicturesthatpreschool-agedchildrenlabeledconsistently.Adultandchildclozenormingdeter- mined that target words were highly probable completions of sensical sentences (e.g., ‘‘The farmer milked the cows”). To create nonsensical sentences, target words were scrambled across sentence stems(e.g.,‘‘Thefarmermilkedthenose”).Nonword-containingsentencesmatchednonsensicalsen- tencesexceptthatacontentwordinthesentencehaditsonsetconsonantreplacedwithadifferent onsetconsonant,makingthesentenceframeitselflesssensical.Thesesentenceswerenotusedhere. Ofthe10talkersperaccentthatweoriginallyrecorded,6talkersperaccent(3maleand3femalein eachaccent;totalof12)wereusedinthecurrentexperiment.Thisallowedgoodcounterbalancingof talkers across participants and conditions. Mexican-accented speakers were older on average (M=32.0 years, SD=13.6, range=20–55) than Californian-accented speakers (M=22.8 years, SD=6.6,range=18–36),inpartbecausewehadtorecruitfromthegeneralcommunityinorderto findspeakerswejudgedtobesufficientlyaccentedinEnglish.Mexican-accentedspeakershadbegun learning English at age 11 years or older (M=16.2years, SD=6.9, range=11–30), whereas Californian-accentedspeakershadalllearnedEnglishfrombirth.Multiplerecordingsweremadeof each sentence for each talker so that we could screen out gross errors and disfluencies and select the most artifact-free recording. The 6 Spanish-accented speakers had slower average speech rates (measured by sentence duration) than the 6 California-accented speakers (2632±313 vs. 2234±204ms),t(10)=3.78,p=.004. Adult speakers of American English (N=49)1 transcribed sentences and words excised from sen- tences.Targetwordrecognitionaccuracywascalculatedforeachutterance,andforthecurrentexperi- mentrecordingswerechosentohavehighadulttranscriptionaccuracy.Thelogicwasthatthiswould provideastrongertestofchildren’ssusceptibilitytoaccentunfamiliarity;showingthatchildrenhave difficulty in comprehending words that adults also have difficulty in comprehending would be less striking. Overall,adultlistenersweremoreaccurateintranscribingfamiliar-accentedtargetwords(94.6%, SD=22.5)thanunfamiliar-accentedtargetwords(90.7%,SD=29.0),v2=27.64,p<.0001,consistent with previous reports of adult difficulty in comprehending accented speech (e.g., Bradlow & Bent, 2008). Each accented word token used in Experiment 1 was transcribed correctly both in isolation andinitsfull-sentencecontextbyatleast3of4or5adults(Table1).Toguardagainstthepossibility thatadultshadlearnedoradaptedfromprevioushearingsoftheword(eachwordorsentencewas heardfivetimesperparticipantfromdifferentspeakers),accuraciesforselectedsoundfileswerealso calculatedontheirfirsthearing,showingaccuracythatwasstillhigh(Table1,lower).Althoughallof theSpanish-accentedrecordingswereincludedintheadulttranscriptionstudy,weincludedonlya subset((cid:1)20%)ofthefamiliar-accentedrecordingsasafamiliaraccentbaselinemeasureinorderto maximizeparticipants’timewithaccentedrecordings.Therefore,transcriptionaccuracyisnotavail- ableforfamiliar-accentedmaterials. Sentences for the child study were 48 matched pairs with identical sentence stems (e.g., ‘‘The farmermilkedthecows/nose”).Atotalof25wordsoccurredasbothasensicaltargetandanonsen- sicaltarget,butneverforthesamechild.Childrenheardonlyoneversionofeachsentence(sensicalor nonsensical),andheardagivenwordastargetonlyonce.Oftheoriginal10speakersofeachaccent,6 wereselectedforinclusionintheexperiment(3femaleand3maleineachgroup)foreaseofcoun- terbalancingwith48trials.Importantly,thepossiblesensicaltargetdidnotappearasanoptionon nonsensicalsentencetrials(e.g.,thecowspictureon‘‘milkedthecows”trialswasreplacedwithapic- tureofanoseon‘‘milkedthenose”trials)andviceversa.Thismeansthattherewerenohighlyplau- sibleluresamongthepicturesonnonsensicaltrials—justonenovelpictureandtwofamiliarpictures withlabelsphonologicallyunrelatedtothetargetandsemanticallyunrelatedtothesentence. 1 Forexpediency,somelistenersatourlinguisticallydiverseuniversitywereearlyacquirersofAmericanEnglishratherthan nativespeakers.Preliminaryanalysesindicatednogroupdifferences,soresultsarecollapsedacrossgroups. 162 S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 Table1 Means (and standard deviations) of proportion correct transcriptions of experimental sentences by adult English-speaking listeners. Adulttranscription Sensicality Insentence Isolatedword Allinstances Sensical 0.990(0.053) 0.910(0.138) Nonsensical 0.956(0.089) 0.931(0.108) Firstinstance Sensical 0.989(0.104) 0.882(0.325) Nonsensical 0.906(0.293) 0.885(0.320) Procedure Childrenweretestedinaquietroomorareaintheirday-carefacilityorpreschool.Sentenceswere playedoverchild-sizedKidzGearheadphones(http://www.gearforkidz.com).Picturesandsoundfiles werepresentedonaMacMinirunningMatlabR2008ausingPsychtoolbox-3(Brainard,1997;Pelli, 1997) and the Eyelink Toolbox (Cornelissen, Peters, & Palmer, 2002). On each trial, four pictures appeared:thetargetpicture,onenovelobjectpicture,andtwofamiliarobjectpicturesthatdidnot overlapsubstantiallyinsoundormeaningwiththetargetword.After500ms,thesoundfilebegan playing. Children were asked to point to the picture that the person was talking about. An experi- menterrecordedchildren’spointingresponsesbyclickingthemouseontheindicatedpicture.When necessary,theexperimenterpromptedchildrentopointortoclarifytheirpoints.Visualfixationswere recorded by an Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada) operating in remotemode. Eachchildheard48sentencesintotal,equallydistributedacrossfourconditions:sensical/familiar accent, sensical/unfamiliar accent, nonsensical/familiar accent, and nonsensical/unfamiliar accent. Acrosschildren,eachsentencewasheardequallyoftenineachofthefourconditions.Therewereeight differentlistssothateachwordineachcondition(sensicalorsenseless)washeardfrom4different talkers,2peraccent.Eachlistwaspresentedinafixedorder,whichwasrandomwiththeconstraint thatnopicturethatwasatargetontrialncouldrecur(asanontarget)anyearlierthantrialn+5.This was intended to prevent children from paying undue attention to distractor pictures as a result of recentoccurrenceasatarget. Children also completed the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth Edition (PPVT-IV; Dunn & Dunn,2007).Thisallowedustoassesswhetherlanguageknowledgefacilitatedaccentedspeechcom- prehension. In most cases, both PPVT scores and chronological age correlated with overall perfor- mance.Forconciseness,wepresentvocabularysizefindingsfromallfourexperimentsinaseparate sectionpriortotheGeneralDiscussion. Results Thereweresubstantialdifferencesinbothaccuracy(Fig.1)andvisualfixationmeasures(Fig.2)as afunctionofsentencesensicality.However,thedatasuggestednoaccent-relateddifferencesinaccu- racyorvisualfixationstotargets. Pointingresponses Data were analyzed in R (R Core Team, 2014) using the package lme4 Version 1.1-7 (Bates, Maechler, Bolker, & Walker, 2015) with logistic mixed-effects models. Mixed-effects models have the advantage of accounting for participant and item variability in a single statistic. Jaeger (2008) arguedthatlogisticregressionismoreappropriatethantransformedoruntransformedaccuracydata becauseaccuracyisfundamentallybinomiallydistributed. Alogisticregressionmodelwascomputedwithtargetselectionasthebinarydependentvariable andaccent(familiarorunfamiliar;withinparticipantsanditems)andsensicality(sensicalsentence ornonsensicalsentence;withinparticipantsanditems)assum-codedpredictors.Thestartingmodel always contained maximal random effects structure (i.e., participant and item [target word] inter- cepts)andparticipantanditemslopesforboththemaineffectsandtheinteraction,asrecommended S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 163 Fig.1. Experiment1:Children’saccuracyinpointingtothetargetpicture.Throughoutallfigures,errorbarsarestandarderrors. byBarr,Levy,Scheepers,andTily(2013).Inthecurrentexperiment,themodelwiththemaximalran- domeffectsstructuredidnotconverge,socorrelationsbetweenrandomeffectsweredroppedfrom themodelassuggestedbyBarrandcolleagues.Themodelwithoutrandomeffectscorrelationscon- vergedsuccessfully.Throughoutthearticle,effectsarereportedviamodelcomparisonusingtheanova functioninR.Thatis,foreachreportedsignificancestatistic,twomodelswerecompared;bothhadthe samerandomeffectsstructure,butonemodelomittedthefixedfactorofinterest(e.g.,accent).Asig- nificantdifferenceinvarianceaccountedforindicatesthatthisfactor(here,accent)improvesthefitof themodel. Overall,childrenweremoreaccurateforwordsheardinsensicalsentences(93.2%)thannonsensi- cal sentences (86.5%), v2(1)=9.72, p=.002. However, there was no effect of accent (unfamiliar accented,88.9%vs.familiaraccented,90.8%),v2=0.87,p=.35,norwasthereanAccent(cid:3)Sensicality interaction(v2=0.00,p=.95). Hereandinotherexperiments,weranmodelsincludingtrialhalf(firsthalfoftrialsorsecondhalf oftrials)asanadditionalpredictortoassesswhetherchildrenadaptedtotheaccentacrossthecourse of the experiment. Because no significant effects or interactions with trial half were found in any experiments,wedonotdiscussthemfurther. Noveltyerrors Our second question was whether children would misrecognize familiar but accented words as novelwordforms.Ifso,morethanonethirdof‘‘errors”(nontargetresponses)shouldbenovelpicture selections.Ifachilddidnotchoosethetarget,therewerethreeremainingpossibilities,oneofwhich wasthenovelobject.Thus,theproportionofnoveltyerrorspredictedbychanceis1/3((cid:1).33).Propor- tionsoferrorsthatwerenovelerrorsshowedlittlevisibleevidenceofnoveltyresponding(Table2).To verify this statistically, we computed a logistic mixed-effects regression, including only nontarget responses,andweassessedtheprobabilitythatanerrorwasaselectionofthenovelpicture(1;other- wise0).Toensurethattheintercepttermassesseddeviationfromchanceratherthandeviationfrom .50,anoffset—thelogoddsof1/3—wasincludedinthemodelspecification.Inaddition,becauseofthe relativelysparsenumberoferrors,whichprecludesinclusionoflargenumbersofpredictors,onlythe randomslopesforaccentwereincludedtotesteffectsofaccentonnoveltyresponding(followingBarr etal.,2013).Neithertheinterceptterm(v2=0.89,p=.34)northeeffectofaccent(v2=0.67,p=.41) approachedsignificance.Thissuggeststhatnoveltyerrorswerenomorethanwouldbepredictedby chanceeitheroverall(proportionnovel=.295)oronaccentedtrials(proportionnovel=.294). Visualfixations Lookswereanalyzedina1-s(1000-ms)timewindowrangingfrom200to1200ms.Thismorethan encompasses the full duration of target words (accented: 498±120ms; familiar-accented: 164 S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 Table2 Experiments1and2:Proportionsofnoveltyresponding(andstandarddeviations)andproportionsoferrortrialsthatwerenovelty responses(chance=.333). Experiment Unfamiliaraccent Familiaraccent Sensical Senseless Sensical Senseless 1 Novel .021(.042) .044(.056) .016(.033) .039(.063) Familiar .052(.112) .104(.120) .047(.070) .083(.112) Proportion .286 .298 .250 .319 2 Novel .047(.056) .034(.047) .044(.073) .018(.041) Familiar .078(.099) .083(.116) .060(.090) .034(.066) Proportion .375 .289 .425 .350 476±103ms;asmallbutsignificantdifference,p<.05)andincludesa200-mspositiveshifttoallow fortheamountoftime((cid:1)200ms)neededtoexecuteaspeech-signal-basedsaccade(Matin,Shao,& Boff,1993;seealsoSalverda,Kleinschmidt,&Tanenhaus,2014).FollowingBarr(2008),linearregres- sionmodelswerecomputedonempirical-logit-transformedtargetlooksbyparticipantsandbyitems, with accent and sensicality as within-participant and within-item factors. Factors were mean- centeredandsum-coded.Duetothesmallnumberofdatapointsforbothparticipantanditemanal- yses,randomeffectscorrelationswereremoved,aswastherandomeffectoftheinteraction.Totest interactionsignificance,thebasemodelandthemodelwithouttheinteractionwerecomputedwith onlytheinterceptandinteractionrandomeffectterm(again,withoutrandomeffectscorrelations),as recommendedbyBarretal.(2013). Aneffectofsensicality(v2=27.40,p<.0001andv2=20.87,p<.0001)revealedmoretargetlooks overallwhen targets wereembeddedin sensical sentences. However, therewas noeffect of accent (v2=2.23,p=.14andv2=2.14,p=.14)andnointeraction(v2=0.12,p=.73andv2=0.05,p=.82). Thus, mirroring the accuracy data, there does not appear to be a deficit in recognition speed for accentedspeech. Discussion Children showed equivalently high comprehension accuracy for familiar-accented (Californian) and less familiar-accented (Mexican Spanish) spoken words in sentence contexts. Our findings are somewhatconsistentwiththehypothesisthataverysmallsetofresponsepossibilities(fourpictures) facilitates comprehension so that the effects of accented speech on comprehension are eliminated. However, the role of sentence context in accented speech comprehension was not supported; althoughresponsesweremoreaccurateforsensicalsentencesoverall,thiseffectdidnotinteractwith accent. Thus, at least for visual context, our findings are consistent with the contextual support hypothesis. A second account of this finding, however, is that because the sentences were chosen basedonhighadulttranscriptionaccuracy,theywerealreadyeasytocomprehendeitherbecausethey wereamongtheleastaccentedinthesetofrecordingsorbecausetheiraccentcharacteristicswere lessdeleterioustorecognition. Todissociatethesetwoexplanations,thenextexperimenttestedcomprehensionoftargetwords alonewiththepre-target-wordportionofeachsentenceremoved.Theseexplanationsproducediffer- entpredictions:ifwordsareweaklyaccentedanddonotelicitcomprehensiondifficulty,childrenwill againshownoeffectsofaccentfamiliarity.However,ifcomprehensionisbolsteredbysemanticcon- text(orhindered,inthecaseofnonsensicalsentences),anaccentfamiliarityeffectwillappear. We also found no tendency for children to think that unrecognized accented words were novel words.Thisisperhapsnotsurprisinggiventhataccentedspeechcomprehensionwasnotdetectably weakerthanunaccentedspeechcomprehension.Thus,inExperiment2,weagainassessedwhether childrenshowednoveltyrespondingfortheunfamiliaraccent. S.C.Creeletal./JournalofExperimentalChildPsychology146(2016)156–180 165 Fig.2. Experiment1:Lookstopictures.Top:Sensicalsentences.Bottom:Nonsensicalsentences.Fam.,familiaraccent;Unfam., unfamiliaraccent;Distr.,distractor. Experiment2 Method Participants MonolingualEnglish-speakingchildren(N=32,16femaleand16male,M =4.72years,SD=0.63, age range=3.24–5.58)fromthesamepopulationasinExperiment1tookpart. Stimuliandprocedure ThesewereidenticaltoExperiment1exceptthatchildrenheardonlythetargetwordsinsteadof fullsentences. Results Accuracy AsinExperiment1,alogisticregressionmodelwascomputedwithtargetselectionasthebinary dependent variable and with accent and sensicality as contrast-coded predictors. The maximal
Description: