ebook img

Early Experience and Human Development PDF

303 Pages·1982·5.245 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Early Experience and Human Development

Early EXRerience ana Human De1 elopment 1 Early EXRerience ana Human Development Theodore D. wachs and Gerald E. Gruen Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana Plenum Press - New lvrk and London Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Wachs, Theodore D., 1941- Early experience and human development. Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Cognition. 2. Experience. 3. Developmental psychology. I. Gruen, Gerald E., 1937- . II. Title. BF713.W32 155.4'22 82-5273 ISBN-13: 978-1-4615-9217-4 e-ISBN-13: 978-1-4615-9215-0 DOl: 10.1 007/978-1-4615-9215-0 ©1982 Plenum Press, New York Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1982 A Division of Plenum Publishing Corporation 233 Spring Street, New York, N.Y. 10013 All rights reserved No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the Publisher To our parents, who provided us with our unique early experiences Preface Our goal in writing this book was to fill a perceived gap in the early experi ence literature. Most existing volumes on early experience and development can be dichotomized on a basic versus an applied dimension. Volumes falling on the basic side are designed for researchers and theoreticians in the biomed ical and behavioral sciences. Most existing basic volumes are either primarily based on infrahuman data or are based on single major human studies. In going over these volumes, we are not convinced of the generality of infrahu man data to the human level; in addition, we were concerned about the replicability of findings from single studies, however well designed these studies were. As a result, the relevance of data from these volumes to applied human problems is quite limited. In contrast, volumes falling on the applied side are designed primarily for those involved in intervention work with infants and young children. These applied books generally tend to be vague and nonempirical compilations of the views of experts and the collective "wisdom of the ages." Rarely in applied volumes do we find conclusions based on solid, consistent, empirical findings. Nowhere could we find a volume in which an attempt was made to summarize systematically all avail able research on early experience as it relates to critical human developmental parameters and to use these findings not only as a means of understanding the nature of development and the nature of early environmental action, but also as a means of transmitting empirically validated conclusions to those in terested in the applied uses of early experience. Our goal became the produc tion of a volume on early experience based on empirical studies but having relevance for the practitioner as well as for the scientist. We envisioned a volume which would be useful not only for the basic researcher interested in the nature of early environmental action, but also for those workers in the field who are doing intervention with high-risk or developmentally disabled babies and who are constantly faced with the question, "What do I do next?" Our interest in producing such a volume came from our own "early experi ence," in that one of us (Wachs) was trained as a child clinical psychologist Vll V1l1 PREFACE but with a strong background in developmental psychology while the other (Gruen) was trained as a developmental psychologist but with postdoctoral training in child clinical psychology. The fact that one of us (Wachs) was interested primarily in cognitive-intellectual development while the other (Gruen) was interested primal ·Iy in social development led to the major focus of the book on these two specific developmental parameters. In reading this book, the reader may become aware of differences not only in style but in orientation. This results from the fact that each of us took primary responsibility for certain sections of the book (with the exception of Chapter 10). Specifically, Wachs was primarily responsible for Chapters 1 through 5 and Chapter 9 and Gruen was responsible for Chapters 6 through 8. Chapter 10 was jointly written. However, each of us went over the other's chaprers and made suggestions both in orientation and content. Thus, at least to that extent, all chapters are a joint product. Chapter divisions were not arbitrary, but rather reflected our differing interests. Despite our monitoring of each other's chapters, the reader may be aware of major differences in orientation. This is a function not only of individual differences in style but also of the nature of the field. The fact that one of us (Wachs) is by nature (or nurture) basically empirical and atheoretical while the other (Gruen) is basi cally theoretical in orientation obviously meant that we would approach our respective chapters in different ways. These initial differences were rein forced (or reinstated) by the fact that there is simply much more available evidence on the relationship of early experience to cognitive development, and therefore these chapters tend to be empirically oriented. In contrast, the social area, while rich in theory, has comparatively less data. Chapter 8 represents Gruen's current interests while Chapter 9 was a painful exercise for Wachs in terms of attempting to develop some kind of theoretical model from empirical data. Overall, for those individuals interested in scientific aspects of early experience Chapters 2 through 9 will be most relevant; for those interested in applied aspects Chapter 10 will obviously be most rele vant, but we hope that the applied reader will also look at Chapters 3 through 7 to get an idea of how the conclusions developed in Chapter 10 have arisen. In the writing of this book we would like to acknowledge the help of Professor Alan Clarke (University of Hull) for his exchange of letters on the topic of early versus later experience. Neither one of us convinced the other, but it was a highly stimulating interchange. We are also grateful for the comments of Professor Pat Gallagher in the Department of Special Education at Purdue University for his reading and comments on our applications chapter. The comments of Professor Robert Bradley (University of Arkansas at Little Rock) were of great help in terms of scope, organization, and the translation of empirical data into intervention applications. Our secretary, Debbie Sweeney, not only typed flawlessly and corrected our grammar but most importantly, cheerfully put up with cut-and-paste rewrites and badly PREFACE ix garbled inserts; for this, special thanks. Finally, we would like to acknowl edge the pervasive influence of our intellectual father (in the case of Gruen) J. and intellectual grandfather (in the case of Wachs), Professor McV. Hunt, whose theorizing and research in the area of early experience not only helped usher in the modern age of nrly experience research but has also been an inspiration to those of us who continue to be interested in the topic. T. D. WACHS E. G. GRUEN Contents Chapter 1 Introduction Chapter 2 The Study of Early Experience 17 Chapter 3 The Physical Environment and Its Relationship to Cognitive-Intellectual Development 41 Chapter 4 The Social Environment and Its Relationship to Cognitive-Intellectual Development 67 Chapter 5 Early Experience and Cognitive-Intellectual Development: The Emotional-Attitudinal Environment 103 Chapter 6 The Earliest Social Experiences and Their Effect on Social Development 121 Chapter 7 The Socialization of Young Children 145 Chapter 8 The Relationship between Social and Cognitive Development 167 Chapter 9 The Nature of Early Environmental Action 185 Chapter 10 Early Experience and Development: Implications and Applications 209 References 251 Author Index 285 Subject Index 293 xi CHAPTER 1 Introduction The potential influence of the early years upon later behavior has always fascinated observers of human development. And the fIrst step, as you know, is always what matters most, particularly when we are dealing with those who are young and tender. That is the time when they are taking shape and when any impression we choose to make leaves a permanent mark. (Plato, The Republic) While there have been continual references to the role of early experience in development at least from the time of the Greek philosophers, as Hunt (1979) has pointed out, these scattered references never became "socially or politi cally dominant" until after 1945. A number of factors appeared to coalesce at that time to form a zeitgeist centering around the importance of early experi ence for later behavior. Thompson and Grusec (1970) suggest that three influences were of particular importance in this process. First was the influ ence of psychoanalytic theory with its emphasis on the primacy of the first six years of life for later personality development. From Freudian theory came infra human research on the effects of infantile stress on physiological and emotional development and human research on the effects of maternal deprivation in orphanage and crisis situations. A second influence came from the field of neuropsychology. Sparked by Hebb's classic work Organization of Behavior (1949), a large number of studies were developed relating the early experience of the organism to both morphological and biochemical central nervous system development. A third influence was the ethological tradition which became quite prominent after 1945. The idea that a relatively brief early exposure could insure lifelong bonding had a major influence on so cialization studies at both the human and infrahuman level. Beyond these three factors, we should also point to a fourth influence, namely the educational tradition. Starting with Rousseau and carrying on through Itard, Seguin, and Montessori, there has been a long tradition of intervention with the young child for the purpose of promoting cognitive gains. This tradition was tied to the area of early experience, at least in part, 2 CHAPTER I by publication of Hunt's seminal volume Intelligence and Experience (1961). In this volume Hunt brought together current infrahuman and human research on early experience effects and related this research to the theorizing of both Hebb and Piaget; as a result of this integration, the idea of early intervention as a means of promoting cogn ·:ive growth was firmly established. Also influ ential was the volume Stability and Change in Human Characteristics by Bloom (1964). Hypothesizing that environmental variation was most salient for a trait during the trait's most rapid growth period, Bloom presented a good deal of evidence which suggested that both intelligence and personality had their most rapid growth periods during the early years of life. The obvious conclusion was that environmental influences should be directed at develop ment primarily during this age period. As a corollary to Bloom, Ausubel (1964) suggested that once development is channeled into a specific area early in life the individual becomes less responsive to stimulation later in life. As a result of these disparate influences, there was a tremendous up surge of research on early experience during the 1950s and thereafter. Hunt (1979) has reported that from 1954 to the present the number of reported studies on early experience went from fewer than 300 to over 1,500. Reviews of this research on both the infrahuman (Ambrose, 1969; Hunt, 1979; Rei sen, 1975; Scott, 1968) and human (Freeberg & Payne, 1967; Haywood & Tapp, 1966; Hunt, 1979; Streisguth & Bee, 1972) levels clearly showed the relevance of early experience to a variety of physiological, cognitive, social, and motivational parameters. Much of this research was translated directly into practice, as witnessed by the tremendous proliferation of infant and preschool intervention programs. THE COUNTERREVOLUTION: EARLY VERSUS LATER EXPERIENCE As is well known, when a pendulum swings, at some point it reaches its apex and thereafter starts to return to a middle point. The same phenomenon appears to hold as well for ideas as for clocks. Following the almost unbridled enthusiasm for early experience during the 1960s, doubts began to set in during the 1970s as to how relevant early experience actually was. This was in part the expected reaction following the reported failure of intervention projects such as Head Start. However, part of the chorus of doubt was founded on basic scientific and methodological grounds. The increasingly powerful body of evidence running counter to the predictions of Freudian analytic theory attacked one of the foundation stones of the early experience movement. Methodological articles (King, 1958) pointed out that many of the infrahuman studies supporting the importance of early experience used enrichment or deprivation experiences only during early life periods; few systematic comparisons are available on the magnitude or duration of effects occurring when early and later experiences are com-

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.