ebook img

DTIC ADA519631: The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Flight Performance, Instrument Scanning, and Physiological Arousal in Pilots PDF

0.68 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview DTIC ADA519631: The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Flight Performance, Instrument Scanning, and Physiological Arousal in Pilots

Form Approved REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE OMB No. 0704-0188 Public reporting burden for this collection of information is estimated to average 1 hour per response, including the time for reviewing instructions, searching existing data sources, gathering and maintaining the data needed, and completing and reviewing this collection of information. Send comments regarding this burden estimate or any other aspect of this collection of information, including suggestions for reducing this burden to Department of Defense, Washington Headquarters Services, Directorate for Information Operations and Reports (0704-0188), 1215 Jefferson Davis Highway, Suite 1204, Arlington, VA 22202- 4302. Respondents should be aware that notwithstanding any other provision of law, no person shall be subject to any penalty for failing to comply with a collection of information if it does not display a currently valid OMB control number. PLEASE DO NOT RETURN YOUR FORM TO THE ABOVE ADDRESS. 1. REPORT DATE (DD-MM-YYYY) 2. REPORT TYPE 3. DATES COVERED (From - To) 2009 Published Journal Article Jun 2002 - Jul 2006 4. TITLE AND SUBTITLE 5a. CONTRACT NUMBER The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Flight Performance, Instrument Scanning, and N/A Physiological Arousal in Pilots 5b. GRANT NUMBER N/A 5c. PROGRAM ELEMENT NUMBER 62202F 6. AUTHOR(S) 5d. PROJECT NUMBER Fred H. Previc,1 Nadia Lopez,2 William R. Ercoline,3 Christina M. Daluz,2 7757 Andrew J. Workman,2 Richard H. Evans,4 and Nathan A. Dillon4 5e. TASK NUMBER P9 5f. WORK UNIT NUMBER 04 7. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 8. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER 1Northrop Grumman 3Wyle Laboratories, Inc. 1000 Wilson Boulevard Life Sciences Group Arlington, Virginia 22209-2278 1290 Hercules Drive 2Air Force Research Laboratory Houston, TX 77058 Human Effectiveness Directorate 4General Dynamics Biosciences and Performance Division Advanced Information Services Biobehavior, Bioassessment & Biosurveillance 5200 Springfield Pike Brooks City-Base, TX 78235 Dayton, OH 45431 9. SPONSORING / MONITORING AGENCY NAME(S) AND ADDRESS(ES) 10. SPONSOR/MONITOR’S ACRONYM(S) Air Force Materiel Command Biobehavior, Bioassessment & Biosurveillance Branch 711 HPW/RHP 711 Human Performance Wing Brooks City-Base, TX 78235 Air Force Research Laboratory 11. SPONSOR/MONITOR’S REPORT Human Effectiveness Directorate NUMBER(S) Biosciences and Performance Division AFRL-HE-BR-JA-2006-0021 12. DISTRIBUTION / AVAILABILITY STATEMENT Approved for public release; distribution unlimited. 13. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTES Published in the International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 19(4), 326-346, 2009. Public Affair approval No. 06-279; 9 Aug 2006. 14. ABSTRACT The effects of 34 hr of continuous wakefulness on flight performance, instrument scanning, subjective fatigue, and EEG activity were measured. Ten fixed-wing military pilots flew a series of 10 simulator profiles, and root mean squared error was calculated for various flight parameters. Ocular scan patterns were obtained by magnetic head tracking and infrared eye tracking. Flying errors peaked after about 24 to 28 hr of continuous wakefulness in line with peaks in subjective fatigue and EEG theta activity, and they were not directly attributable to degradation of instrument scanning, which was very consistent across pilots and largely unaffected by the sleep deprivation. 15. SUBJECT TERMS Fatigue, sleep deprivation, cognitive performance, EEG theta activity 16. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF: Unclassified 17. LIMITATION 18. NUMBER 19a. NAME OF RESPONSIBLE PERSON U OF ABSTRACT OF PAGES Fred H. Previc a. REPORT b. ABSTRACT c. THIS PAGE U 21 19b. TELEPHONE NUMBER (include area U U U code) Standard Form 298 (Rev. 8-98) Prescribed by ANSI Std. Z39.18 THE INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF AVIATION PSYCHOLOGY,19(4),326–346 Copyright ©2009Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1050-8414 print / 1532-7108 online DOI: 10.1080/10508410903187562 The Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Flight Performance, Instrument Scanning, and Physiological Arousal in Pilots 0 1 0 2 h c r Ma Fred H. Previc,1Nadia Lopez,2William R. Ercoline,3 15 Christina M. Daluz,2Andrew J. Workman,2 6 :3 Richard H. Evans,4and Nathan A. Dillon4 8 1 : 1Northrop Grumman, San Antonio, Texas t A 2Brooks City-Base, San Antonio, Texas ] y r 3Wyle Laboratories, San Antonio, Texas a r ib 4General Dynamics Advanced Information Engineering Systems, L l San Antonio, Texas a c i d e m o r e A [ Theeffectsof34hrofcontinuouswakefulnessonflightperformance,instrument : By scanning,subjectivefatigue,andEEGactivityweremeasured.Tenfixed-wingmili- ed tarypilotsflewaseriesof10simulatorprofiles,androotmeansquarederrorwascal- d oa culatedforvariousflightparameters.Ocularscanpatternswereobtainedbymag- l wn neticheadtrackingandinfraredeyetracking.Flyingerrorspeakedafterabout24to o D 28hrofcontinuouswakefulnessinlinewithpeaksinsubjectivefatigueandEEG thetaactivity,andtheywerenotdirectlyattributabletodegradationofinstrument scanning,whichwasveryconsistentacrosspilotsandlargelyunaffectedbythesleep deprivation. Fatigue due to sleep deprivation is considered a major risk to flight safety (Borowsky&Wall,1983;Ramsey&McGlohn,1997;Tormes&Guedry,1975), withsurveyssuggestingthatuptohalfofallpilotshaveactually“dozedoff”while flying(Caldwell&Gilreath,2002).Fatiguedegradesnotonlybasiccognitiveper- Correspondence should be sent to Fred H. Previc, 10906 Whispering Wind, San Antonio, TX 78230. E-mail: [email protected] SLEEP, FLIGHT, AND SCANNING 327 formance (Caldwell, Caldwell, Brown, & Smith, 2004; Dinges et al., 1997; Matthews,Davies,Westerman,&Stammers,2000)butalsoflightperformance, including the ability to maintain designated flight parameters (Caldwell et al., 2004; Caldwell, Caldwell, & Darlington, 2003; LeDuc et al., 1999; Morris & Miller,1996).Whethertherisktoflightsafetyisdueprimarilytoareducedgen- eral cognitive capacity or to flying-specific factors (e.g., stick control or instru- ment scanning) has yet to be determined. Sleepdeprivationexperimentshavegenerallyshownthatperformancereaches anadirintheearlymorninghours,typicallyafter24hrofsustainedwakefulness andatthetroughofthecircadiancycle,beforeimprovingslightlyasthesecond dayprogresses(Caldwelletal.,2004;Caldwelletal.,2003).Thereboundonthe seconddayispartlyduetotheeffectofthecircadiancyclethatrisesduringtheday 0 1 0 (Eddy&Hursh,2001)andalsototheimpendingcompletionoftheexperiment.In 2 ch previous simulator studies, helicopter pilots showed an earlier nadir than fixed- r Ma wing pilots, possibly because of their different daily schedules (Caldwell et al., 5 1 2004; Caldwell et al., 2003). 6 :3 Onepossiblecorrelateofthedecrementsinflightperformanceisimpaired 8 1 ocularscanningoftheflightinstruments.Fivebasiceye-movementparameters : t A thathavebeenrepeatedlystudiedinconjunctionwithfatigueare(a)blinkrate, ] ry which generally increases with sleep deprivation and fatigue (Lal & Craig, a br 2001;Morris&Miller,1996;Stern,Boyer,&Schroeder,1994);(b)pupildi- i L ameter, which typically decreases with sleep deprivation (Morad, Lemberg, l a ic Yofe, & Dagan, 2000; Ranzijn & Lack, 1997; Wilhelm, Wilhelm, Ludtke, d me Streicher&Adler,1998;Yoss,Moyer&Hollenhorst,1979);(c)saccadicve- o r e locity,whichhasbeenshowntodecreasewithsleepdeprivation(Caldwellet A [ : al.,2004;DeGennaro,Ferrara,Urbani,&Bertini,2000;Rowlandetal.,2005; y B Russo et al., 2003; but see Morris & Miller, 1996); (d) mean saccade length d e d (fixation distance), which increases with time-on-task (Lavine, Sibert, Gok- a o nl turk, & Dickens, 2002); and (e) dwell time, which in at least one study was w Do shown to decrease with time-on-task (Lavine et al., 2002). In contrast to the preceding basic eye-movement studies, no previous study has investigated changes in pilot instrument scanning with extended wakefulness, although therehavebeenseveralstudiesofpilotscanningbehaviorundernormalwake- fulness(Bellenkes,Wickens,&Kramer,1997;Itoh,Hayashi,Tsukui,&Saito, 1990;Jones,Milton,&Fitts,1949). Thechiefpurposeofthisstudywastoinvestigatechangesinflightperformance duringextendedwakefulnessofover30hrandtodeterminetherelationshipbe- tween fatigue-related flight performance decrements and both general changes (e.g.,increasedblinkrate)andspecificchanges(e.g.,reducedscanningofspecific instruments)inoculomotorbehavior.Anotherobjectivewastorelatechangesin flightperformanceandinstrumentscanningtochangesinsubjectiveandobjective fatigue and arousal, the latter being measured by the scalp-recorded electroen- 328 PREVIC ET AL. cephalogram(EEG).Previousresearchhasdemonstratedthatincreasesinlow-fre- quency(deltaandtheta)EEGactivitygenerallyparallelchangesinperformance during sleep deprivation, fatigue, or both (Caldwell et al., 2004; Lal & Craig, 2001; Morris & Miller, 1996). METHOD Participants TenpilotsfromtheUnitedStatesAirForce(USAF)participatedinthisstudyinan off-dutycapacity.Eightofthe10pilots(allmale)wereactive-dutypilotsandthe 10 remaining 2 were reserve officers. The average age of the pilots was 34.2 years 0 2 (range=23–46years),withhalfofthepilotsover35yearsandhalfat30yearsor h c r under.Theiraverageflightexperiencewas2,806hr(range=207hr–5,800hr),1 a M 5 andthecorrelationbetweenflightexperienceandagewasalmostperfect(r=.96). 1 6 All pilots signed an informed consent document approved by the Brooks City- 3 : 8 BaseInstitutionalReviewBoardandwerecompensatedfortheirparticipationbe- 1 t: cause they served in an off-duty capacity. A ] All pilots possessed at least 20/25 vision binocularly (they were allowed to y r a wearcontactlensesbutnotglassesforcorrection),hadnormalvestibularfunction r b Li asassessedbytheSharpenedRombergTest,andhadnopreviousevidenceofves- al tibularsymptomssuchasdizziness,vertigo,anddisorientation.Nopilotsuffered c i d fromsleepproblemsorseizures,andnonewascurrentlytakinganypsychoactive e m ro medication(e.g.,antihistamines,antidepressants,sleepaids,etc.)orwasahabitual e [A smoker(i.e.,consumedmorethanonecigaretteperday)orcaffeinedrinker(i.e., : y consumedmorethan100mgofcaffeineperday).Allpilotsrefrainedfromcaf- B ed feine,alcohol,andothermildstimulantsorsedativeswhilemonitoredathomeon d oa thenightbeforethesleep-deprivationperiodaswellasduringthe34hrofcontinu- l n w ouswakefulnessinthelaboratory.Nineofthe10pilotscompletedasleeplogfor o D the7dayspriortothestartoftheexperimentandhadtheirfinalnightofsleep(af- terthefirstnightoftraining)monitoredbyawrist-activitymonitor.2Thesleepdu- rationandqualityforthe3dayspriortothebeginningoftheexperimentwereana- lyzed using the Fatigue Avoidance Scheduling Tool (FAST; Eddy & Hursh, 2001),andabaseline“wakingefficiency”scorewasderivedforeachpilottopre- dicthislevelofalertnesspriortotheperiodofcontinuouswakefulness.Theaver- age amount of sleep per night was 7.46 hr and was associated with an average “wakingefficiency”scoreof90.59,withonlythreepilotsscoringbelow90(76.8, 85.2, 85.9). 1Three of our pilots had just completed undergraduate pilot training. 2One pilot misplaced his sleep log and failed to return it. SLEEP, FLIGHT, AND SCANNING 329 Apparatus: Gyroflight Sustained Operations Simulator This study was conducted in the Gyroflight Sustained Operations Simulator (GSOS; Environmental Tectonics Corporation, Southampton, PA), a four-axis flightsimulatorwithadditionalspatialdisorientation-producingcapabilities.The GSOSwascolocatedwiththeAviationSustainedOperationsLaboratoryinBuild- ing170atBrooksCity-Base,Texas.TheGSOSpossessesmotioncapabilitiesin pitch(upto±25°),roll(upto±25°),andyaw(upto360°ofsustainedyaw).The GSOSalsofeaturessubthresholdwashoutinpitchandrollaswellaslimitedheave (upto±12cm).Ithasathree-channelhigh-resolution,noncollimated,out-the-win- dowvisualdisplay,withatotalfield-of-viewof28°verticalby~120°horizontal. The GSOS aeromodel replicates the T-6 aircraft, with which most of the pilots 10 werefamiliar,anditsreconfigurableinstrumentpanelwasalsodesignedtodepict 0 2 ascloselyaspossiblethepanelontheT-6aircraft.TheGSOSwasoperatedand h c r monitoredfromacontrolstationinanadjacentroom,anditsphysiologicalrecord- a M 5 ing capability was configured to record eye movements and EEG. 1 6 3 : 8 Eye-Movement Recordings 1 : t A EyemovementswererecordedbymeansofEye-Trac6000(AppliedScienceLab- ] ry oratories, Cambridge, MA), a head-mounted system that consists of a magnetic a br headtrackerandaninfraredeyetracker.The“FlockofBirds”headtracker(As- i L cension,Burlington,VT)providessixdegree-of-freedomtrackingbymeansofa l a ic 21-Hzmagneticpulsesignaldirectedtowardasensorattachedtothehead.Thepo- d me sitionoftheeyeintheorbitwassampledat60Hzusinganinfraredbeamandcam- o r e eratomeasuretherelativeanglesofthepupilandcornealreflectance.Together, A [ : theheadandeyesignalsdeterminedgazewithanerrorof<0.5°duringcalibration. y B TheeyeandheadsignalswerethensenttoacomputerontheGSOSandrelayed d e d throughtheGSOSslipringstoamonitorlocatedinthecontrolstation.Datawere a o nl stored on a PC and analyzed later. w o D EEG Recordings EEGswererecordedusingtheGRASS-TelefactorInstrumentsAurorarecording system (West Warwick, RI) running TWin™ collection and analysis software. EEGswererecordedfromgold-cupelectrodesattwosites(C andP )andwere z z referencedtolinked-mastoidelectrodes,whileanadditionalgroundleadwasat- tachedtothescalp.EEGswererecordedwithcutofffilterssetat1Hzand70Hz and were digitized at 200 Hz. Data were then stored on a PC and analyzed later. Procedures Flight profile. The GSOS flight profile, shown in Figure 1, consisted of sevenmajorsegments:(a)takeoffat360°andclimbto8,000ft;(b)arightclimb- 330 PREVIC ET AL. 0 1 0 2 h c r a M 5 1 6 3 : 8 1 : t A ] y r a FIGURE1 TheGyroflightSustainedOperationsSimulator(GSOS)profilewiththeseven r ib segmentsinwhichvariousmeasuresofflightperformancewererecorded:(a)takeoffat360° L l andwings-levelclimbto8,000ft;(b)rightclimbingturnto10,000ft;(c)wings-levelclimbto a ic 12,000ft;(d)rightlevelturnto180°;(e)wings-leveldescentto7,500ft;(f)leftdescendingturn d e to 4,000 ft; and (g) visual descent and landing at 360°. m o r e A [ : y B ingturnto10,000ftand235°;(c)awings-levelclimbto12,000ft;(d)arightlevel d e d turn to 180°; (e) a wings-level descent to 7,500 ft; (f) a left descending turn to a o nl 4,000ftand90°;and(g)visualdescentandlanding.3Theflight,whichrequired w Do about19mintocomplete,simulatedatransitionfromadusktakeofftoanighttime landing and was performed mostly in instrument meteorological conditions (IMC).TheexceptionstoIMCwereduringabriefperiodaftertakeoff,duringa smallsectionofthewings-levelclimbwhilepilotssearchedfortraffic,andduring theturntofinalapproachfollowedbythevisualapproachandlanding.Oneach segment,thepilotwascommandedtomaintainasetofpreviouslyspecifiedcon- trolorperformanceparameters,includingairspeed(allsegments),heading(Seg- ments1,3,and5),verticalvelocity(Segments2,3,5,and6),bank(Segments2,4, and6),andlongitudinalbearingandglideslope(Segment7).Onodd-numbered flightsthepilotflewasalreadydescribed,andoneven-numberedflightsthepilot 3Inanadditionalsegment,whichwastoovariabletoallowcommandedparameters,thepilotwas required to achieve and maintain a heading at 45° to intersect final approach. SLEEP, FLIGHT, AND SCANNING 331 flewamirrorprofile,beginningwithaclimbtotheleftfollowedbyawings-level climbat125°ratherthanaclimbtotherightfollowedbyawings-levelclimbat 235°.TheGSOSprofilewasdesignedtobesemiautomatedandrequiredtheoper- atortodirectlyinstructthepilotonlyduringgrossflighterrorssuchasthewrong turning direction, wrong course, or wrong heading. On four of the flights (1, 4, 7, and 10), seven spatial disorientation conflicts were inserted in during various segments of the flight. These conflicts were de- signedtotesttheeffectsofsleepdeprivationonspatialdisorientation(Previcetal., 2007). They involved either motion illusions (an excess pitch sensation during takeoff in Segment 1, a Coriolis illusion during head tilt in Segment 4, and postrotatorysensationsfollowingrolloutfromtheturnsattheendofSegments2, 4, and 6) or visual illusions (a sloping cloud deck in Segment 3 and a narrow, 0 1 0 up-slopingrunwayinSegment4).Pilotswereinstructedtoreportanydiscrepan- 2 ch cies or conflicts with their instruments but were not informed in advance of the r Ma specificillusions.Onlytwooftheconflictswereshowntoinfluenceflightperfor- 5 1 manceduringmeasuredepochs:theslopingclouddeckonbankduringthelong 6 :3 wings-levelascentinSegment3andtheillusoryrunwayonglideslopeduringthe 8 1 landinginSegment7.However,theclouddeckeffectwasveryslight(<1°)and : t A waspresentforonlyasmallportionofSegment3,andglideslopeinSegment7 ] ry waslaterremovedfromthecompositeerrormeasure.Anyremainingeffectofcon- a br flictversusnonconflictflightswasdeterminedtobenonsignificantinaprelimi- i L naryanalysis,sothatthedatawerethencollapsedacrossconflictandnonconflict l a ic flights.Becausetransitionsbetweenflightmaneuvershadtobeeliminatedwhile d me pilots were in the process of attaining their commanded flight parameters, only o r e about 50% of the total flight was used for data analysis. A [ : Root mean squared error (RMSE) was used as the measure of flight perfor- y B mance. The RMSE values were calculated using Equation 1: d e load RMSE=∑n (i−c)2/n, (1) n w Do i=1 whereiis the observed value andcis the commanded flight parameter. EachsegmenthadthreemeasuresexceptSegment1,whichonlyhadairspeed andheading,andSegment7,becauseslopewasremovedduetotheeffectsofthe spatialdisorientationconflictsonfouroftheflights.TocomputecompositeRMSE valuesfordifferentflightsegmentsandparametersaswellasfortheentireflight, allRMSEvaluesweredividedbythebaselinevalue(theRMSEinFlight1)and then converted to log units before averaging. The grand composite average was based on a total of 19 individual values. Eye-movement calibrations and recordings. Eye movements were re- cordedduringallflights.Priortothebeginningoftheexperiment,theheadtracker wascalibratedbyplacingthemagneticsensorinapointerrodthatwasaimedat 332 PREVIC ET AL. differentportionsoftheGSOSinstrumentpanel.Justpriortoeachflight,andafter thepreflightrestingEEGrecordingssession,pilotssatdownintheGSOSandthe infraredcamerawasadjustedtoobtainagoodimageoftheeye.Then,theGSOS doorwasclosedandtherestoftheeyecalibrationwasmonitoredfromtheGSOS controlroom.Duringthenine-pointeyecalibration,thepilotscannedmostofthe GSOSinstrumentpanelwhilehisheadremainedstill.Acalibrationwasconsid- eredsuccessfulifthecalculatedgazewasnomorethan1°offatanysinglepoint (andlessthanthatonaverage).Typically,thecalibrationhadtoberepeatedoneor moretimeswhiletheoperatoroptimallyadjustedtheilluminationandsensitivity oftheinfraredcamera.Initialcalibrationsweresuccessfulon98%offlights;how- ever,manyeye-movementrecordswerelaterdiscardedbecauseeitherthepupilor cornealimageswerelostformorethan15%ofthesamplesduringtheflightoron 0 1 0 visual inspection there was too much variable drift in the eye-movement record 2 ch from the beginning to the end of the flight (see below). r Ma The eye-movement data collected during each of the seven segments corre- 5 1 spondedmostlywiththeperiodsinwhichflightperformancedataweregathered. 6 :3 Unlike the flight performance data, however, the eye-movement data were re- 8 1 cordedevenwhilethepilotrolleddowntherunwayinSegment1andtheywerere- : t A cordeduntiltouchdowninSegment7.Therewereatotalof22measuresobtained ] ry from the eye-movement recordings. Five of these were basic measures: average a br pupildiameter,averageblinkrate,meanfixationduration(definedaseyeposition i L remainingwithin1SD[0.5°]foratleastsixconsecutivesamples,or83msec),av- l a ic eragesaccadelength,andpercentageofdwelltimesgreaterthan2sec.4Unfortu- d me nately,saccadicvelocitycouldnotbemeasuredbytheEye-Trac6000systembe- o r e cause of the relatively slow head sampling rate. There were also 17 measures A [ : relatedtothepilot’sinstrumentscan.Theseincludedthepercentageofdwellson y B eachoffiveflightinstruments—theelectronicattitudedirectorindicator(EADI), d e d airspeedindicator,altimeter,horizontalsituationindicator(HSI,alsoknownasthe a o nl headingindicator),andtheverticalvelocityindicator(VVI)—aswellastheper- w Do centageofdwellsofftheinstrumentpanelaltogether.Therewerealso10measures of transitioning to and from the five flight displays as well as a measure of transitioningtoandfromtheinstrumentpanelasawhole.Indeterminingthedwell andtransitionpatternsforthefivemajorflightdisplays,theiroutlinesonthein- strumentpanelspaceweremappedtothecalibrationspacefortheeyetrackerand superimposed on the scan pattern from each flight, as shown in Figure 2. Forreasonsthatareunclear,therewasaslightdriftofgazepositionrelativeto thecalibrationinmostoftheinflighteye-trackingrecords.However,thedriftwas almostalwaysconstantwithinagivenflight,whichallowedustoperformasingle 4Dwelltimereferstotheamountoftimespentcontinuouslyinoneareaofinterest,whetherinmul- tiplefixationsornot.The2seccriterionforlongdwelltimeswasbasedonthefactthat,inpreliminary data, less than 10% of all dwell times were greater than that value. SLEEP, FLIGHT, AND SCANNING 333 0 1 0 2 h c r a M 5 1 6 3 : 8 1 : t A ] y r a r b Li FIGURE2 Arepresentativescanpatternoveranentireflight,withtheoutlinesofthefive al designated flight instruments shown in white boxes. c i d e m ro recenteringoftheentirescanpatternforeachflightpriortoanalysisandretainall e A [ butfourrecordswithexcessivevariabledrift.Therecenteringwasconductedby : By twooftheexperimenters(NLandWRE)beforeanydataanalysiswasperformed. ed Somerecordswithvariabledriftbutnormalblinkrateswereretainedforthebasic d a o oculomotor analyses, while other records showing blink rates greater than 1/sec l n ow (i.e.,greaterthan3SDaboveourmeanof0.35/sec)werediscardedfromthebasic D oculomotoranalysesbutretainedforthescan-patternanalysis.Intheend,71of 100recordswereretainedforthescan-patternanalysisand64of100recordswere usedinthebasicoculomotoranalyses,includingatleasttwoeachfromtheearly and late flights of 9 of the 10 pilots.5 EEG recordings and analysis. Electrodeswereattachedtoeachpilotafter the final training session. Each placement site was cleaned with acetone, after which electrodes were attached to the scalp with collodion and then filled with 5Onlyoneeye-movementrecordwassalvageablefromthetenthpilot,butitwasnotstatisticallyan- alyzablebecausetherewerenootherrecordsfromthatpilot.Hence,allofthatpilot’sgazedatawere discarded. 334 PREVIC ET AL. electrolytegel.Afterimpedanceswerecheckedanddeterminedtobeatacceptable levels(<5KΩ),theEEGelectrodesremainedonthroughouttheentire34hrof continuous wakefulness. Pre-andpostflightrestingEEGswererecordedfor4mineach,withtheeyes open (2 min) and the eyes closed (2 min). Preflight EEGs were recorded in the GSOScontrolareajustbeforethepilot’sgazewascalibrated,whereaspostflight EEGs were recorded immediately following each flight in the GSOS and moni- toredfromthecontrolstation.In-flightEEGswerealsorecorded,butthesewere contaminatedbytheinteractionoftheeyetracker’smagneticpulsegeneratorand themovementoftheGSOS,aswellasbypilots’eyeandlimbmovements.Unfor- tunately,postflightdatawerenotrecordedproperlyfor19of100flights,sothat onlythepreflightrestingEEGsweresubjectedtoanalysis,althoughonepilotwith 0 1 0 three missing records was removed even from that analysis. 2 ch Atotalofthree3-secepochs,eachselectedbecausetheywerefreeofanyobvi- r Ma ous muscle or blink artifacts, were selected from each EEG record. EEGs from 5 1 eachelectrodesiteandconditionwereanalyzedseparatelyfortheirFourierampli- 6 :3 tude in each of three bands: deltas (1.5–3.0 Hz), theta (3.0–8.0 Hz), and alpha 8 1 (8.0–13.0 Hz). : t A ] ry Overall schedule. Pilotsarrivedthenightbeforethebeginningofthecon- a br tinuouswakefulnessperiodforinitialtrainingontwoversionsoftheflightprofile i L (neitherofwhichwasembeddedwithspatialdisorientationconflicts)andonthree l a ic cognitivetests:thePsychomotorVigilanceTest(PVT),theMulti-AttributeTask d me Battery(MATB),andtheOperationSpanTask(OSPAN).Thesecognitivetests o r e andtheircorrelationswithchangesinflightperformanceduringcontinuouswake- A [ : fulness are described elsewhere (Lopez, Previc, Fischer, DaLuz, & Workman, y B 2009).Duringalltrainingflights,pilotswereoutfittedwiththehead-mountedop- d e d tical device and their gaze measurement was tested for its adequacy. a o nl Aftertheirmonitoredsleep,pilotsarrivedbackinthelaboratoryateither0730or w Do 0830andflewtheGSOSprofileforafinalpracticeflightandreceivedadditional trainingonthecognitivetests.AtmidmorningonDay1ofcontinuouswakefulness, eachpilothadEEGelectrodesattachedand,afteraperiodofrestandlunch,began the experiment. During an experimental session, two pilots were run in tandem, withPilot1’sfirstflightbeginningat1200andPilot2’sfirstflightbeginningat 1300.Successiveflightswererunat3-hrintervals(i.e.,1500,1800,2100,etc.,for Pilot1;1600,1900,2200,etc.,forPilot2).6Pilotsarrived5minpriortoeachflight so their resting EEG could be recorded and their eye-tracking calibration com- pleted, and they remained 5 min following each flight for the postflight resting EEG measurement. The complete sequence of flights is shown in Table 1. 6Becausetheflighttimeswerestaggeredby1hrforthetwopilots,thesessiontimesinthedatafig- ures (next section) are listed as 1230, 1530, 1830, and so on.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.