ebook img

Diurnal Time-Activity Budgets and Habitat Use of Ring-Necked Duck Ducklings in Northcentral Minnesota PDF

13 Pages·1992·6.6 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Diurnal Time-Activity Budgets and Habitat Use of Ring-Necked Duck Ducklings in Northcentral Minnesota

Wilson Bull, 104(3), 1992, pp. 472^84 DIURNAL TIME-ACTIVITY BUDGETS AND HABITAT USE OF RING-NECKED DUCK DUCKLINGS IN NORTHCENTRAL MINNESOTA Stephen J. Maxson' and Richard M. Pace, IIP Abstract.—Westudieddiurnal time-activitybudgetsand habitatuseof36 individually marked,classIIandIII Ring-neckedDuck(Aythacollaris)ducklingsduring 1983and 1984 in northcentral Minnesota. Overall, ducklings spent 40.9% oftheir time foraging, 30.8% resting, 21.0% in comfort activities, 6.0% in locomotion, and 1.3% alert. Within broods, duckling activities were synchronized. When the duckling being sampled was foraging, resting, orengaged in comfort movements, 87%, 77%, and 56%, respectively, ofthe other brood members were engaged in the same behavior. Although male ducklings were larger than theirfemale siblings, we detected no sex differences in activity budgets, dive rates, or the proportion oftime spent in subsurface vs surface feeding. Only time spent in comfort andlocomotoractivitiesvariedbytimeofday. Timespentinallactivities,exceptcomfort, differedbetween years. Five ofsixhensabandonedtheirbroodsbeforetheducklingscould fly. Thereafter, ducklings spent more time foraging and less time resting and in comfort activities. Ducklingswere neverobservedon shore.Theyspent82.6%oftheirtimeinopen water, although use ofthis habitatcomponent was less than itsavailability(96.7%). While in open water, ducklingsspent most oftheirtime foraging(50.9%), resting(20.6%), and in comfort activities (19.2%). Exposed flats, consisting ofislands ofdecayed organic debris, were used (16.0%) far in excess oftheir availability (0.3%), indicating ducklings actively selected this habitat component where they mostly rested (65.8%) and engaged in comfort activities(28.4%). Received 9May 1991, accepted 1 Feb. 1992. Time-activity budget studies are useful for quantifying how ducks ap- portion their time to cope with varying energy demands, how they cope with various environmental changes on a daily, seasonal, and year-to- yearbasis, and how these birds use habitat. Brood rearingisan important phase in the annual cycle ofducks. Several time-activity budget studies have quantified the behavior ofbrood hens and/or ducklings (e.g., Ham- mel 1973, Siegfried et al. 1976, Joyner 1977, Ringelman and Flake 1980, Afton 1983, Hickey and Titman 1983, Paulus 1984, Rushforth Guinn and Batt 1985). However, none ofthese studies has sampled individually marked ducklings ofknown sex norhaveany time-activitybudget studies been conducted on Ring-necked Duck {Aythya collaris) broods. The Ring-necked Duck is a small-bodied diving duck that breeds pri- marily in the closed boreal forest of Canada. In the United States, the ' Wetland WildlifePopulationsand ResearchGroup, MinnesotaDept, ofNaturalResources, 102 23rd St., Bemidji, Minnesota 56601. ^WildlifePopulationsand ResearchUnit, MinnesotaDept,ofNatural Resources, 500LafayetteRoad, St.Paul,Minnesota55155.(Presentaddress:U.S.FishandWildlifeService,LouisianaCooperativeFish and Wildlife Research Unit, Forestry, Wildlife and Fisheries Building, Louisiana State Univ., Baton Rouge, Louisiana70803.) 472 Maxson andPace • DUCKLING ACTIVITY BUDGETS 473 largest numbers are found in the Great Lakes states (Bellrose 1976), and it has been estimated to be the second most abundant breeding duck in the forested areas ofMinnesota (Moyle 1964). Male Ring-necked Ducks weigh more than females except during the ovulation and laying periods (Hohman et al. 1988, Hier 1989). Because standard metabolic rates are a function ofbody mass, alargerbird mustexpend more energy(and thus require more food) to conduct any given activity than a smaller bird. If thesexualsizedimorphismisalsopresentinducklingsitshouldcontribute to corresponding differences in time-activity budgets, especially with re- gard to foraging. These might be expressed as differences in total time spent foraging, the proportion oftime spent in subsurface versus surface foraging, or in diving rates. MaleandfemaleRing-neckedDucksexperiencedifferentselectionpres- sures as reflected in higher survival rates ofmales (Conroy and Eberhardt 1983). This results in adult sex ratios skewed in favor ofmales (Mendall 1958, Bellrose et al. 1961, Jahn and Hunt 1964, Anderson et al. 1969). The sexes also have differing reproductive strategies with regard to time and energy expenditure (Hohman 1984). Intersexual differences in sur- vivalandreproductive strategiesmightbeevidentasearlyastheduckling stage and could be reflected in time-activity budgets. Divingduck hens frequentlyabandon theirbroods beforethe ducklings can fly (Hochbaum 1944). Whether Ring-necked Duck ducklings alter their time-activity budgets after hens depart is unknown. How Ring- necked Duck broods modify time-activity budgets during the day or be- tween years is also unknown. Habitat use by Ring-necked Duck broods has not been quantified. We documented diurnal time-activity budgets and habitat use ofRing- necked Duck ducklings during age classes Ilb-III (i.e., ducklings 25-60 days of age) (Larson and Taber 1980) and determined whether time- activity budgets differed by sex, time ofday, oryear and whetherthe hen was present with the brood. We quantified the degree ofbehavioral syn- chrony among brood members. We also documented dive rates and the ratio ofsubsurface to surface feeding by ducklings to determine whether these differed between sexes. STUDY AREA AND METHODS Broods were observed at Refuge Pond, a 45-ha wetland 7 km northeast of Bemidji, Minnesota. This wetland was also used by broodsofMallards {Anasplatyrhynchos), Blue- wingedTeal {A. discors). Wood Ducks{Aixsponsa), and Common Goldeneyes{Bucephala clangula).Thewetlandisapermanentlyfloodedaquaticbedinalacustrinesystem(Cowardin etal. 1979).A25-hafloatingmatofsedge{Carexspp.)andcattail{Typhalatifolia)covered the northwest portion ofthe basin. The remainderofthewetland (20 ha)was largelyopen water. 474 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 104, No. 3. September 1992 We overlaid a grid on aerial photos ofthe wetland to quantify the proportion ofeach habitatcomponent.BecausewedidnotobserveRing-neckedDuckbroodsusingthefloating matportionofthewetland, weincludedonlythe openportionofthebasin in ouranalysis. Most ofthis area was classified as open water. Water depth was <1.5 m. Aquatic plants were abundant in this zone and were dominated by large leaf pondweed (Potamogeton amplifolius), floatingleafpondweed {P. natans), whitewaterlily{Nymphaeatuberosa), and watershield{Braseniaschreberi). Hardstembulrush(Scirpusacutus)wasscatteredthrough- outthe open waterzone butwastoo sparsetowarranta separateclassification. Remaining habitatcomponentsincludedstandsofcattail, standsofhardstembulrush, smallislandsof floatingsedge/cattail mat,andexposed flatsconsistingofislandsofdecayedorganicdebris. Ring-neckedDuckducklingswerecapturedbynightlighting(LindmeierandJessen 1961), sexed (Addyand MacNamara 1948), aged (Larson andTaber 1980), weighed, and banded with a U.S. Fishand WildlifeServicealuminum legband. Wealso fittedeachwith a nasal saddle (Sugden and Poston 1968) coded to allow individual identification. Observations began two daysaftercapture, allowing broods to reassembleand ducklings to habituate to nasal saddles. Ducklings were observed with binocularsand an 80x telescope from a vehicle on a hill overlookingthewetland. Datawerecollectedduring 1-hsamplingperiodsbetween4Aug.- 14 Sept. 1983 and 8 Aug.-13 Sept. 1984. We conducted 1-4 sample periods (x = 1.9) per day. Ducklingactivitiesandhabitatusewererecordedbyinstantaneoussampling(Altmann 1974) at 15-sec intervals at the tone ofa metronome (Wiens et al. 1970). Activities were classifiedas: foraging, resting, comfortmovements, locomotion,alert,andagonisticbehav- iors(Johnsgard 1965). Foragingincluded dives, dive-pauses, and surface feeding(dabbling and food-handling). Dive-pauses typically lasted only a few sec. Pauses lasting more than 15 sec were classified as alert or resting. Comfort movements included preening, bathing, stretching, wing flapping, orscratching(McKinney 1965). Locomotionincludedswimming not primarily associated with foraging, walking, and flying. During each sampling period, oneducklingwasobservedfor30minandasecondduckling,usuallywithinthesamebrood, was observed for the remaining 30 min. When possible, both sexes were observed during a sampling period. We alternated which sex was observed first and varied the individuals sampled. The day was divided into four time (CDT) blocks (1 = dawn-10:00, 2 = 10:01- 14:00, 3 = 14:01-18:00, 4 = 18:00-dusk). Within each time block, the start ofsampling was varied from day to day so that all daylight hours were sampled overthe course ofthe study. We also recorded whetherthe sample took place priorto (hen present) orafter(hen absent)ahenpermanentlyabandonedherbrood.Althoughduckhenssometimesleavetheir broodsforshortperiods(e.g.,Afton 1983,Paulus 1984),noneofoursampleswererecorded duringtemporary absences. During samplingperiods, we documented behavioral synchronyamong brood members by recordingthe activityofthe focal bird at 2.5-min intervalsand then scanningtheother brood membersandrecordingtheiractivity. Thesedatawereonlycollectedfromducklings thatwerestill accompanied bythe brood hen, becauseafterhensdeparted, broods became less clumped spatially, making instantaneous scansdifficult. Thesamplingunitforactivitybudgetanalyseswasone30-minperiod.Foreachsampling unit, the number ofinstances ofeach activity was summed and converted to percent of time. To insure homogeneity ofvariance, thearcsin square roottransformation(Anderson and McLean 1974)wasappliedtodatabeforeanalysis. Weestimatedcomponentsaffecting thevarianceofeachactivityusingmaximumlikelihoodmethodswiththeBMDP3Vprogram forgeneral mixed ANOVAs(Jennrich and Sampson 1976). Ourmodelconsistedofa fixed component for the overall mean, another fixed component for the sex by time ofday, by year, by hen presence category that described each datum, and three random components Maxson andPace • DUCKLING ACTIVITY BUDGETS 475 representingthreelevelsofsampling(i.e.,broods, individualswithinabrood,andrepeated observationson an individualduckling). Wetestedthehypothesesthatthevarianceattrib- utable to broods and individuals was zero. These variance components did not appear to contributesignificantly(noP < 0.25)tototalvariance. Therefore,wecollapsedouroriginal model to one that included only a single random component (replicates within a sex by timeofday, byyear, and by hen presencecell)andanalyzedobservedactivitieswith more traditional MANOVAandANOVAleast squares modelsusingtheGLM procedureofthe StatisticalAnalysisSystem(HelwigandCouncil 1979).Leastsquareestimatesofthevarious means were determined from the appropriate models and are reported as retransformed values. To compare the relative amounts oftime spent in surface foraging versus subsurface foraging(dive plus dive-pause), we subjectivelyomitted sampling units having fewerthan 15 datapointsin the foragecategorytoeliminatesamplescoveringonlya small portion of a foraging bout (foraging bouts typically lasted 15-25 min). For the remaining sampling units,weanalyzedtheproportionofforagingtimespentinsubsurfaceforagingusingaleast squaresanalysisofmeansmodel(Searle 1987).Wetested 10simplecontraststhataccounted foras much ofthe fourmain effects(sex, year, timeofday, hen presence)andall two-way interactionsthatmissingcellmeans(6of32cells)wouldpermit. Priortoanalysisthearcsin square root transformation was applied to thesedata. When feasible, we also recorded the total number ofdives by the focal bird during the 30minitwassampled.Diverates(thenumberofdivesper 15-secintervalduringsubsurface foragingbouts)werecalculatedbydividingthetotalnumberofdivesbythecombineddata pointsinthediveanddive-pausecategories.Wechosetoomitfromanalysissamplingunits havingfewerthan 10datapointsinthecombineddiveanddive-pausecategories. Weused a paired/-test(SnedecorandCochran 1980)tocomparediveratesbetweensexes forthose samplingperiods when dive rate data were obtained from both sexes. RESULTS Activity and habitat use data were obtained from 36 ducklings (18 male, 18 female) from six broods. We obtained 135 30-min samples from males and 104 samples from females. Individual ducklings were sampled from 1-33 times (median = 5). Activitybudgets.—OvQVdAi, Ring-necked Duckducklings averaged 40.9% oftheir time foraging, 30.8% resting, 21.0% in comfort activities, 6.0% in locomotion, and 1.3% alert (Table 1). Agonistic activities occurred rarely (<0.1% ofthe time) and were not analyzed statistically. Broods often synchronized their activities (Table 2). Synchrony was most pronounced during foraging and resting as most ofthe other brood members foraged or rested at the same time as the focal bird. Comfort activities were less synchronized indicating that comfort activities also occurred when other brood members were foraging or resting. Within broods, mean female weights at capture ranged from 77-96% of mean male weights. No female weighed more than any of her male siblings. However, tests for differences among activity budgets indicated no significant effects for sex or any interacting set ofeffects that included sex. Consequently, we omitted sex as an explanatory variable and reran 476 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 104, No. 3, September 1992 MANOVAs on the remaining three variables. The Wilk’s Lambda test criterion indicated significant overall effects for time ofday (T’= 2.31, df = 15, 605, P = 0.0033), year {F = 6.73, df = 5, 219, P = 0.0001), hen presence {F= 3.99, df= 5, 219, P= 0.0018), and the interaction between time ofday and year {F = 2.06, df = 15, 605, P = 0.0103). No overall effects were indicated for the yearby hen presence {F= 1.84, df= 5, 219, P = 0.1059), time ofday by hen presence {F = 0.82, df= 15, 605, P = 0.6518), or the year by time ofday by hen presence {F = 1.59, df= 15, 605, P = 0.0705) interactions. We used three-way ANOVAs to identify which activities were influenced by these overall effects. Onlytimespentincomfortactivities{F= 4.49, df= 3, 223,P= 0.0044) and locomotion {F = 7.04, df= 3, 223, P = 0.0002) differed by time of day. Comfort activities occurred most often during period 3 and least often during period 4 (Table 1). Locomotion was highest during period 3 and lowest during period 2. Timespentinallactivitiesexceptcomfortdifferedbetweenyears(Table 1). Ducklings spent more time foraging in 1983 than 1984 (F = 13.06, df = 1, 223, P = 0.0004). Conversely, they spent more time resting in 1984(P= 12.78, df= 1, 223, P = 0.0004). More locomotion (P= 14.67, df= 1, 223, P = 0.0002) and alert (F = 8.88, df= 1, 223, P = 0.0032) were observed in 1983. We found a significant interaction between time of day and year for resting (P = 3.10, df= 3, 223, P = 0.0278), locomotion (P = 5.03, df= 3, 223,P= 0.0021), andalert(P= 3.18, df= 3, 223,P= 0.0250). Whereas resting occurred least often in time periods 2 and 4 in 1983, resting occurred most often during these periods in 1984 (Table 1). In 1983, locomotion showed an increasing trend from time periods 1 to 3. How- ever, in 1984 there was a sharp decline in locomotion between time periods 1 and 2. Alert showed an increasing trend throughout the day in 1983 but in 1984 showed decreases in time period 2 and especially pe- riod 4. All six brood hens abandoned their ducklings during the study period. One left two days after herducklings achieved flight capability. The other hens departed 3, 4, 8, 10, and 19 days before their ducklings could fly. These departures did not appear related to the disturbance caused by nightlighting operations as hens departed an average of 12.2 days after their ducklings were captured. Typically, ducklings remained together as a brood for the first few days after being abandoned. Gradually, they became more independent and spread out more widely on the pond. Ducklingsbegan makingshort flightswhen two months oldanddispersed from the pond 2-5 days later. Afterhensabandoned theirbroods, ducklings spent moretime foraging ' Maxson andPace • DUCKLING ACTIVITY BUDGETS All Ced (^N o^ ^ 1 -H ^ c 0To0f^ 0d0 d (—N d rd^_ ‘ m — O O 00 I-Th)’ — (N d (N C9 O O w Mean d o^s’ 'dO 'dO SO >- c p o z o6 On (rNt o-hs‘ so sCoN d Q U0m 0O0N rd- ods —osH s0o0 00 u s H co —00 ssoo d sdo 0dm0 s rsi c<^ rs) M> Ku 0OTT0N ods d0IT0S d oTdf odTof < m m uuc 0O0N (dN d<N c(NN Od—s' (rd~N_ u H<Joa 5hzu sc<o fd(<N) 0o0d O<dON ossd C<—)N ca0s/.3 (UcEoO T0Of0N fsdNo) Oo—OdC (tdTN -—dH_ (oN QCDJ 0Om0N (doN oO—sd' <s-NHd ss—od' <oONS 'S 3 u H CcQ rdf or'd (dN rdsi (dOs b s ct rn 0 u 0< 2 T0Of0N -H (sNd (dmOs 00 222 £ Tt (N c<^ fc u OuKUhJ 0O0N dmTt (IdTN) oTf<td3 oidn o(-rOld- z u 00 o <Os It OS ON (N <N <N z m — O0N0 <mN -It <(Ons <n % c •r 2i -H (N <n It (U e H 478 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 104, No. 3, September 1992 Table 2 ComparisonofActivities Between Sample BirdsandOther Brood Members Activity(%)ofotherbroodmembers samplebird Samplebird members Foraging Comfort Rest Locomotion Foraging 100 643 87.3 6.8 5.1 0.8 Resting 100 638 3.3 17.1 77.7 1.9 Comfort 58 366 22.9 56.0 18.6 2.5 ’Numberofdatapointsobtainedat2.5-minintervalsduringsamples. {F= 14.31, df= 1, 223, P= 0.0002) and less time in comfort {F= 10.26, df= 1, 223, P = 0.0016) and resting {F = 7.49, df= 1, 223, P = 0.0067) than they did while the hen was present (Table 3). Time spent in loco- motion and alert remained unchanged. We had 104 30-min samples for contrast analyses ofsubsurface versus surface foraging. No sex or time ofday differences were found. The only explanatory variables making significant contributions toward overall variance in the proportion offoraging time spent in subsurface foraging were year {F = 25.49, df= 1, 78, P = 0.0001) and year by hen presence interactions {F = 8.46, df = 1, 78, P = 0.0047). Therefore, we ran an ANOVAmodeltoproduceestimatesofmeansfortheyearbyhenpresence interaction. These indicated that the effect of the brood hen’s presence was greater in 1984 than in 1983. After brood hens departed in 1983, ducklings spent a somewhat greater proportion oftheir foraging time in subsurface foraging(77.1%—present vs 95.8%—absent). However, in 1984 theproportionofsubsurfaceforagingdecreasedmarkedlyafterbroodhens departed (93.6%—present vs 49.1%—absent). Table 3 Mean PercentageofTime DucklingsSpent in Each Activitywhenthe Brood Hen Was Presentor Absent^ Activity Henpresent Henabsent Foraging 32.2 49.6 Comfort 23.6 18.3 Rest 37.2 24.4 Locomotion 6.0 6.0 Alert 1.0 1.7 Nb 98 141 •Original analyses were conducted using arcsine-transformed proportions. Depicted values are retransformed least squaresmeansfromtheappropriatemodel. ”Numberof30-minsamples. Maxson andPace • DUCKLING ACTIVITY BUDGETS 479 Table 4 Ring-necked Duck Duckling Habitat Use Vs Availabilityand Duckling Time-activity Budgetswithin Each HabitatComponenton Refuge Pond Activitywithinhabitats(%) Avail- ability Use For- Loco- Habitatcomponent {%) (%) N“ aging Comfort Rest motion Alert Open water 96.7 82.6 23,562 50.9 19.2 20.6 7.7 1.6 Exposed flat 0.3 16.0 4570 3.5 28.4 65.8 1.1 1.3 Bulrush stand 0.9 1.1 313 92.0 5.8 0.6 0.6 1.0 Floatingmat island 0.7 0.3 82 0.0 50.0 48.8 0.2 0.0 Cattail stand 1.4 0.0 0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 0.0 ’Numberofinstancesfocalducklingswererecordedineachhabitatcomponent. We recorded 5043 foraging dives during 98 30-min sampling units. The overall mean dive rate was 0.874 ± 0.181(SD) dives per 15-sec interval. For 15 sampling periods in which data were obtained from both sexes, dive rates did not differ by sex {t = 2.01, df= 14, P = 0.0637). Habitat Mineral soil made up 77% ofthe shoreline ofthe pond whilefloatingsedge/cattailmatcomprisedtheremainder. However, duck- lings were never observed on shore norwere theyever observed in cattail standsalongthepondedge. Bulrushstandsandsmallfloatingsedge/cattail mat islands appeared to be used in proportion to their availability (Table 4). Ducklingsspentthegreatmajorityoftheirtimeinopenwater,although use of this habitat component was somewhat less than its availability. Exposed flats were used far in excess oftheir availability, indicating that broods actively sought out this habitat component. Ducklings used open water primarily for foraging, but substantial amounts of comfort and resting occurred there as well (Table 4). The small amountoftime spentin bulrush standswasalmostexclusively used for foraging. On the other hand, exposed flats and floating sedge/cattail mat islands were used primarily for resting and comfort activities. DISCUSSION Because ofthe sexual size dimorphism and differing intersex survival and reproductive strategies exhibited by adult Ring-necked Ducks, it can bearguedthatsexdifferencesshouldbeevidentfromthetimeofhatching. This appears to be the case with regard to size dimorphism but not in other aspects. Compared to other stages in their life cycle, ducklings of both sexes experience relatively similar energy demands associated with growth and development. At this stage, intersex energy budget compar- isons are not complicated by differential costs ofcourtship, gamete pro- 480 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 104, No. 3, September 1992 duction, molt, etc. Major activities among brood members were syn- chronized, and both sexes foraged together in the same habitats, further indicatingthat intersex survival strategies are not markedly different dur- ing this period. Nevertheless, based on the size difference alone, we ex- pected to find sex differences in time-activity budgets particularly asso- ciated with foraging. However, we detected no significant differences in activity budgets based on sex, nor did we find sex differences in the proportion offoraging time spent in subsurface versus surface feeding or in diving rates. Several factors may have accounted for this. The range of duckling age (and thus different mass) over which we sampled may have obscured subtle sex differences in foraging time or technique. Ad- ditionally, our most direct comparison (dive rates) had a small sample size and thus low power. Further, males may have selected a slightly different diet than females or there could have been sex differences in nocturnal activity. We were unable to measure either ofthese factors. Most time-activity budget studies ofducklings have noted behavioral changes during the day, especially with regard to foraging. Early morning and late afternoon peaks in foraging have been reported for Blue-winged Teal, Mallard, and Ruddy Duck {Oxyurajamaicensis) ducklings (Joyner 1977, Ringelman and Flake 1980). Mottled Duck {Anasfulvigula) duck- lingsforagedmostinthemorningandlessduringmiddaybeforeincreasing foraging at night (Paulus 1984). Mendall (1958), though lacking quanti- tative data, reported that Ring-necked Duck broods tended to be more active in morning and evening but that feeding and resting periods oc- curred frequentlythroughoutthedaywith littleevidenceoffixedroutines. Incontrast, wedidnotdetectdifferencesintimespentforagingthroughout the daylight hours. Furthermore, time of day differences in resting, lo- comotion, and alert were inconsistent between years. The extent Ring- necked Duck duckling time-activity budgets change at night is unknown. During nightlighting operations, Minnesota Dept, ofNatural Resources duck banding crews frequently observe broods of Ring-necked Ducks foraging (R. T. Eberhardt, pers. comm.). Differences between years for all activities except comfort were likely related to changes in food abundance or availability on the pond. During age classes II and III, Ring-necked Duck ducklings eat a variety ofplants andanimals(Mendall 1958, Hohman 1985, McAuleyandLongcore 1988). Althoughjuvenile Ring-necked Ducksexhibitpreferencesforcertainfoods, availability plays a prominent role in food selection (Hohman 1985). Hohman (1984, 1985) noted year-to-year changes in foods available to Ring-necked Ducks and found that contributions ofindividual food spe- cies in class II Ring-necked Duck duckling diets differed between years Maxson andPace • DUCKLING ACTIVITY BUDGETS 481 in northwestern Minnesota. Ifone assumes that nutritional needs ofclass II and III ducklings remain the same from yearto yearand that ducklings attempt to forage efficiently, then the amount of time spent foraging at any particular stage of their development should be a function of food abundance or availability. As sufficient food becomes easier to obtain, less foraging time is required. Relationships between food levels and the amountoftime spent foraginghavebeen notedin otherwaterfowl (Paulus 1988) as well as in non-waterfowl species (e.g., Maxson and Oring 1980). The onlynotableenvironmental differencebetweenyearswasthatAugust pond levels were 16.5 cm lower in 1984. This was the lowest August water level recorded on the pond during the 1979-1984 period (R. T. Eberhardt, unpubl. data) and it may have influenced abundance or avail- ability ofduckling foods. Further evidence ofchanges in foods was the fact that duckling foraging tactics differed between years. In 1984, par- ticularly during the period after brood hens departed, ducklings spent considerably more time surface feeding (primarily dabbling) than during other phases ofour study. Given that time is limited, any change in time spent in one activity must be reflected in a corresponding change in time spent in other activ- We ities. believe that the driving factors determining duckling time-ac- tivity budgets were requirements for foraging and feather maintenance, and that, in general, other activities (especially resting) filled in any time gaps remaining after these needs had been met. Time spent in comfort activities did not differbetween years, likely because a certain proportion of time is required for feather maintenance regardless of other factors. When foraging time decreased in 1984, this time gap was compensated for primarily by increased time spent resting. An inverse relationship between time spent foraging and resting has also been noted in other waterfowl (Paulus 1988). Thegreateramount oftime spentin locomotion in 1983 was associated with the higher foraging and alert time that year. Locomotion was recorded primarily as broods travelled between foraging sites, between foraging and loafing sites, or as they moved away from potential predators. Afterhens abandoned theirbroods, ducklings spent more time foraging while spending less time resting and in comfort activities. Several expla- nations are possible. As ducklings approach adult size they require in- creased amounts offood based on the increase in their body mass alone. Without the hen to help guide them to good feeding locations, ducklings may initially be less efficient in finding sufficient food, although this may entail only a minor behavioral adjustment as ducklings had already been foraging on the pond 40-60 days. Also, there may have been some late

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.