ebook img

Direct Evidence for Trade-Offs between Foraging and Growth in a Juvenile Spider PDF

7 Pages·1995·7.6 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Direct Evidence for Trade-Offs between Foraging and Growth in a Juvenile Spider

1995. TheJournal ofArachnology 23:37-43 DIRECT EVIDENCE FOR TRADE-OFFS BETWEEN FORAGING AND GROWTH IN A JUVENILE SPIDER Linden E. Higgins: Dept, ofZoology, University ofTexas at Austin, Austin, Texas 78712 USA ABSTRACT. A simple modification ofclassical optimal foraging models yields explicit predictions ofthe allocation ofresources to orb-web synthesis and weight gain in a spider. Because the spiders may be trying to avoid weight loss, weight gain and web size are predicted to be be negatively correlated at higher food levels and positively correlated at lower food levels. These predictions were upheld qualitatively by experiments involvingjuvenile Nephilaclavipesand N. maculata. RESUMEN. Una sincilla modificacion a los modelos clasicos del forrajeo optimo dapredicciones explicitas sobre la repartition de recursos entre el sintesis de la tela orbiculary el aumento de peso en una arana. Dado quearanas posiblemente estan ententando evitarlaperdidade peso, la interaction entre el aumento de peso y el tamano de la tela deberia estar negativamente correlacionada bajo majores niveles de alimentacion, y posi- tivamente correlacionada bajo de condiciones de alimentacion reducida. Estas predicciones fueron appoyadas experimentalmente utilizandojuvenilesde Nephila clavipesyN. maculata. Classical foragingmodels are based on the as- al. 1991).Thus,constructionofeachorbrequires sumption that optimal allocation oflimiting re- decisions concerning the allocation ofmetabol- sourcesamongvariousexpendituresincreasesthe icallyimportantcompounds;suchdecisionscould fitness ofthe organism either directly (through directly affect growth. increased reproductive success) or indirectly In some large araneoid spiders, fitness is in- (throughincreasedgrowth)(e.g.,Schoener 1971; creasedby rapiddevelopmentand weightlossis Pykeetal. 1977; Mangel &Clark 1986). Despite predicted to reduce the fitness of the females thekeyroleofthisassumption, fewstudieshave (Higgins & Rankin, in press). The existence of described the interaction between foraging in- resources common to both foraging and growth vestment and growth at different levels offood and the fitness penalties associated with weight availability. This appears in part due to the dif- loss imply that when resources are limiting, for- ficulty of finding common units of measure agingbehaviormightbe different from that pre- (Pianka 1981; Stephens & Krebs 1986). Ifcom- dicted by foraging models calculating maximi- monresourcesareused forbothgrowthand for- zationoftherateofenergyintake.Here,Ipresent aging,thenexplicitpredictionscanbemadecon- a simple model that predicts patterns ofalloca- cerningtheirallocationatdifferentlevelsofprey tion of resources between foraging and weight availability. gainatfoodlevelsvaryingfrom verylowtovery Thestudyofforagingin orb-webbuildingspi- high. I experimentally tested this model using ders allows observation of resource allocation juvenile Nephila clavipes (L.) and N. maculata because foraging investment is measurable as (Fabr.) (Araneae: Tetragnathidae) that were fed materialinvestmentinto theweb, andthe orbis at very low, intermediate and high levels. composed of physiologically important com- THE MODEL pounds. The orb web is renewed regularly, and changes in orb-web size reflect the response of Ifit is true that the functions ofweight gain the spiderto foragingconditions (Buskirk 1975; and web building are competing for common Gillespie 1987; Higgins & Buskirk 1992). The resourcesand ifit is important to the individual viscid silk that forms the spiral ofthe orb is a to avoid weight loss, a simple modification of protein thread coated with a mixture oforganic theclassicforaginginvestmentmodels(Schoener compounds that also have physiological func- 1971) is necessary to predict behavior at very tions (Tillinghast 1984; Dadd 1985; Townley et lowlevelsofpreyavailability. The model ispre- 37 : 38 THEJOURNALOFARACHNOLOGY vestment, the spiders should reduce their for- agingeffort.Thus,therelationshipbetweenweight gain and web investment in spiders is predicted to be negativelycorrelatedat higherfoodlevels, and positively correlated lower food levels. METHODS Inordertotestthepredictionthattherelative investmentintotheorbandweightgainwilldif- fer between the shift from very low to interme- low high diate foraging success and the shift from inter- foodlevel mediate to high foraging success, I controlled prey availability in juveniles of two species of Nephila lab-reared N. davipes in Panama and N field-collected maculata in Madang, Papua . New Guinea. In both cases the investment into weightgain and orb area were recorded as func- ro tions ofprey availability, measured as the daily 0 prey weight/initial spider weight. 1 0 O The basic methods were the same in the two experiments. To control the structural environ- ment used in web construction, I placed indi- vidualson 30cmdiametersphericalframescon- mm mm sistingoftwohoopsof3 x 6 fiberglass, residualoforbsize fixedatrightanglesandsuspendedfromtheceil- Figure L—The model ofresource allocation by an ingorbracesinopen-airinsectaries. Ifedspiders orb-weavingspider. A. As food resources increase for cloocamlmlyonavapirleayblietesmtinogfletshsesbeeesspi(dTerrsig(oHniaggsipn.)s,&a agivenindividual,orbsizeincreasesthendeclinesand weightgainincreases. B.Amongspiders,theresiduals Buskirk 1992;Higgins,pers.obs.).Beeswerecol- oforbsizeandweightgainregressedagainstspidersize lected from the same nests throughout the ex- arecompared(eliminatingthe variationdueto spider periment. size). Atlow food levels(square), orb size and weight Foreachanimal,Irecordeddailyorb-webarea gain are lower than average; at medium food levels andrate ofweightgain. (Mesh size, measuredas (circle, diamond), orb size is greater and weight gain the number ofspiral strands per 2 cm (Higgins islowerthanaverage;andathighfoodlevels(triangle), &Buskirk 1992), didnotvarywith foodlevel in orbsizeislowerandweightgainishigherthanaverage. either species. N davipes: F = 3.16, ns; N . (1j34) . maculata: F(2>13) = 0.34, ns). Orbs builtby these seated graphically in Fig. 1. At high to inter- spidersare asymmetrical, and orb-web areawas mediatelevelsoffood,thespidershavesufficient estimated as a halfellipse using measurements resources to avoid weight loss and increase in- ofthe vertical and horizontal radii. While many vestment into foraging as food levels decrease. orb weavers replace the orb each day, these spi- Thisisconsistentwithclassicalforagingmodels: derssometimesonlypartiallyrenewtheorb(Lu- as average foraging success decreases, the in- bin 1973). When a web was partially new, the vestment into hunting (e. g., searching time per areawasestimatedby calculatingthe area ofthe patch) is predicted to increase (maximizing en- orbthen reducingtotheestimatedareathatwas ergygain/effort;Stephens&Krebs 1986).Ifcom- new (<V3, <V2, <%, <%). Orbs are not ofcon- mon resources are being allocated into growth stant size during the intermolt interval (Higgins and foragingthen the rate ofweightgain will be 1990), therefore, foraging investment was mea- slower, due to both decreased prey capture and sured as the mean area ofall orbs spun during increased foraging investment. However, this the intermolt period. If a spider did not molt prediction will not hold at lower food levels if duringtheexperiment, themeanareaduringthe the spiders must avoid weight loss. When re- experiment was used. sourcesare solimitingthatweightlosswouldbe Websizeandrateofweightgainarebothfunc- the price ofcontinued increases in foraging in- tions ofspidersize. Spidersize was measuredas HIGGINS-TRADE-OFFSBETWEEN FORAGINGANDGROWTH 39 web cm residual of radius, mean ± 1 s.d. Figure2.—Therelationshipbetweenmeanorb-websizeandmeandailyweightgaininNephilaclavipesunder threedifferentfood levels(% initial spiderweight), twotrials. Trial 1 (filledsymbols): diamond-20%, triangle - 35%. Trial 2 (open symbols): square- 10%, circle- 20%. the length ofthe leg I tibia + patella length, cm gravid females ofeach generation. The first two (TPL), which does not vary between molts. Spi- cohorts of spiderlings hatched in January, and der weight, which increases between molts, can the second twocohortshatchedin Mayof1986, be estimated from TPL and abdomen volume resulting in two separate trials. Each cohort was (Higgins 1992a; the same equation was used for kept in a fine-screened cage (15 cm w. x 30 cm both species). The rate ofweight gain in mgwas 1. x 60 cm h.) within an open-air, screened in- calculated for all spiders as <5wt mg (= wtFma! - sectary, and large numbers offield-caught Dro- Swtinraai/no. days observed). To eliminate the in- sophila spp. were released into the cages twice fluenceofsize,theresidualsofwebsizeandweight daily. As each spider reached the third instar, it gain regressed against spider size were the de- was removed from the common cage, marked pendentvariablesinthestatisticalanalyses.Mul- and placed onto a spherical frame. During this tivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) al- instar, each spider was offered one stingless bee lowed simultaneous comparison ofthe effects of daily (mean weight 3.5 mg, mean body length 4 prey level on the rate of weight gain and web mm). size. WhentheMANOVAwas significant, as in- The experimental treatment was initiated in ANOVA dicated by Wilks’ lambda, were used eitherthe fourth (firsttrial) orfifth (second trial) toexaminethevariationineachdependentvari- instar. Within each trial, the spiders were fed able. either two or one stingless bees daily, the mean Nephilaclavipes.—Forthisexperiment,Iraised number ofprey captured per day in the field in spidersfrombothgenerationsofabivoltinepop- the rainy and dry seasons, respectively (Higgins ulationonBarroColoradoIsland, Panama. First- & Buskirk 1992). As each spider molted, it was generationfemalesmatureintheearlyrainysea- measured(TPL,abdomenlengthandwidth,cm), son (May-June), and second generation females individuallymarkedwithTestors®enamelpaint, mature as the rains end (December-January) and randomly assigned to receive one or two (Higgins 1992a). Eggs were collected from two bees/day. Penultimate-instar males may parti- 40 THEJOURNALOFARACHNOLOGY 05 E sz. 05 05 5 ro B IB go 'w 0) -100 -50 0 50 100 web cm residual of radius, mean ± 1 s.d. Figure 3.—The relationship between mean orb-web size and mean daily weight gain in Nephila macuiata under three different relative food levels (% initial spider weight), square: <17%; triangle: <22%; diamond: <45%. tion resources differently (Higgins 1993); there- The regressionsoforb areaand rate ofweight fore, males in this instar were not used in the gainagainstspidersizeweresignificant(orbarea experiments. Treatment lasted 14 days, slightly = 166.6 + 521.6(TPL);R2=0.57,F(U6) =48.81, longer than the mean intermolt interval ofthe P < 0.001; ln(dwt) = -1.82 + 2.80 (TPL), R2 = fourthinstar.If,atanytime,anindividualmoved 0.09, F(ij34) = 3.53, ns). Insufficient spiders sur- offitsframe, the supportstrandsofthenewweb vived to allow examination ofvariation among were carefully cut and the spider with its web clutches, but the spiders ofboth clutches were was placed back onto the frame, after measure- assigned to each treatment within a trial, con- ments(afterM. Robinson, pers, comm.). Move- trolling for this factor. ment offofthe frames occurred with equivalent Nephila macuiata.—In February and March frequency in all groups (test for full indepen- of 1993, I conducted a similar experiment with dence: G= 6.61,df= 4, ns;Sokal&Rohlf1981). N. macuiata. Small groups (no more than 10 at Theexperimentaltreatmentwasstartedonav- a time) ofsecond - fifth instar spiders from the erageoneinstarlaterinthesecondtrial. Because forests ofBaitabag village, west ofMadang, Pa- ofthe differencein instarbetweenthetwo trials, pua New Guinea, were held indoors overnight the spiders differed in initial size and weight. toencouragebuildingontheframes.Afterwards, Therefore, the two treatments (1 vs. 2 prey/day) the spiders were moved to a screened, roofed yielded three levels of prey weight relative to insectary, and randomly assigned to a diet (low initial spider weight: high (35%) and medium orhigh). Thenumberofpreyitemswasadjusted (20%) in the first trial, medium (20%) and low accordingtospiderinstar. Ifedsecondandthird (10%) in the second. One individual’s relative InstarspidersDrosophilaspp.(0.67 mgeach; low food weight was 26%; preliminary analyses in- = 2/day, high = 4/day (second instar) or 6/day dicated no difference in the statistical outcome (thirdinstar)), andlargerspiders Trigonabees(5 when it was included in the medium or high mg each; low = 1/day, high = 2/day). The ex- group, so it was assigned to the former. perimentaltreatmentwascontinuedfor 10days, HIGGINS—TRADE-OFFSBETWEEN FORAGINGANDGROWTH 41 afterwhichthespiderswerereturnedtotheforest A and newindividualswere collected. The shorter interval reflects both the shorter intermolt du- ration ofthis species, and the need to maximize the number ofanimals used in the experiment. Although I had randomly assigned prey avail- ability,Icouldnotmakethespiderseatandprey consumptionwashighlyvariable. Therefore,the independentvariableusedinthestatisticalanal- yses is the relative mean weight ofprey actually eaten (presentedas % initial spiderweight). The continuous distribution of prey consumed was divided into three approximately equal groups ofincreasing relative prey weight. N. maculata were not as likely as N. clavipes to build an orb each day, perhaps due to the insectary being roofed. Therefore, the foraging investmentreported isthetotalarea oforbbuilt duringtheexperimentdividedbythenumberof dayshunting(determinedasthenumberofdays with a neworb plusthe numberofdaysthatthe spiders accepted prey although a new orb had not been built). The size range ofspiders used was larger than in the previous experiment, so the data had to be transformed to correct for heteroscedasticity prior to statistical analysis of the residuals (In (Swt) = -0.997 + 2.698 (TPL), R2=0.33,F -7.398,P < 0.025;(web/hunt)* - 8.506 + 1(01>.156)7 (TPL), R2 = 0.34, F = 8.26, {1 15) P < 0.025). spidersize,TPL1,cm RESULTS Figure4.—Therelationshipbetween mean orb-web sizeand meandailyweightgain inindividualNephila Nephila clavipes.—Both rate of weight gain maculata under three different food levels (% initial (5wt) and mean orb size varied with food level spiderweight). Square - <17%, triangle - <22%, di- (Fig. 2). From 10%to 20%relativefood(second amond- <45%. A.There is noeffectofthe sexofthe trial),dwtandorbsizeincreased,whilefrom20% individual on resource allocation. Male - filled sym- to 35% relative food (first trial), 5wt increased bols, female = open symbols. B. There is no effect of MANOVA and orb size decreased. Three com- initialspidersizeonresourceallocation.Filledsymbols pared the responses ofthe spiders to food level = residual oforb-web size, open symbols = residual within each trial, and compared between trials of5wl. atthe20%foodlevel;thesignificancelevelswere adjusted for multiple, non-independenttests. In is probably due to the larger mean initial spider the second trial, there was a significant effect of size in trial 2. food level on <5wt but not on web size (Wilks’ X Reduced rate ofweight gain had a significant = 0.356, P < 0.01; ANOVA: 6wt: F = 23.5, negative effect on the development rate of the P < 0.001; web size: F = 0.20,(1>n1s3)). There spiders, observed as an increase in the number (1 13) was a significant effect in the first trial on both of days between molts. Eighteen spiders from web size and <5wt (Wilks’ X = 0.568, P < 0.01; trial 1 were held on the assigned diet until they ANOVA: §wt: F = 8.09, P = 0.01; web size: molted and the number ofdays between molts F(h 19) = 5.41, P(1j=19)0.03). Between trials, there was correlated with the rate ofweight gain and was a significant difference in <5wt but not web initialspidersize(In(daysbetweenmolts)= 1.96 size (Wilks’ X = 0.514, P < 0.01; ANOVA 5wt: - 37.7 (5wt) + 4.81 (TPL); R2 = 0.46, F = F(1 17) = 16.05, P < 0.01; web size: F(1 17) = 0.03, 6.25, P = 0.011; only 5 spiders molted i(n2>1t5r)ial ns). The effect oftrial in the medium food level 2). 42 THEJOURNALOFARACHNOLOGY Nephila maculata —Although the pattern of 1974;Wise 1975, 1979).Inweb-buildingspiders, . variation in orb investment and rate ofweight thewebmustbebuiltbeforetheforagingquality gain among the three groups is similar to that of a site can be assessed. Perhaps due to this observed forN. clavipes, the differences are not initial investment, or due to temporal variation significant(Fig. 3;Wilks’X-0.78,P=0.5).What in prey availability, many web-building spiders is most striking is the variation among individ- arehighlysite-tenacious(Eberhard 1971;Enders uals in response to the same level offood intake 1976;Tanaka 1989;however,seeTurnbull 1964). (Fig. 4). Often, different individuals made op- Ratherthanabandoningpoorpatches,astrategy positeresponses:onespiderbuildinglarger-than- oftenpredictedbyforagingtheory(e.g.,Chamov average orbs and gaining little weight, another 1971; Pyke et al. 1977), spiders show other be- building smaller-than-average orbs and gaining havioral and physiological responses to nutri- weightrapidly. Thevariationwasnotafunction tional stress (Riechert & Luczak 1982; Sherman of the sex nor of the initial size (TPL) of the 1994). However,theseresponsescanslowgrowth individual (Fig. 4). and affect both survival and female fecundity (Higgins 1992a, 1992b, pers. obs.). The results CONCLUSIONS ofthesestudiessupportthehypothesisthatthere The two experiments uphold the predictions are potentially limiting resources necessary for ofthe model. Juvenile Nephila ofboth species both orb silk synthesisandgrowth, predictedby tendedtoincreaseorb-websizeanddecreasethe the chemical analyses oforb-silk by Tillinghast rate of weight gain with slightly reduced prey and Townley (Tillinghast 1984; Townley et al. capture; they decreased both orb-web size and 1991). The spiders must invest in the orb web rate ofweightgain in response to greater reduc- in order to capture prey, but unless critical re- tions in prey capture. This response resulted in sources are in abundance, such investment nec- the counter-intuitive pattern of decreased for- essarilydelaysgrowthanddevelopmentandpo- aginginvestmentwith lowfood availability, up- tentially compromises individual fitness. Pre- holdingthe initial assumptions that resource al- dation rate declines with increasing spider size locationisconstrainedbyaneedtoavoidweight and fecundity increases with early maturation lossandthat common resourcesare used in for- (Higgins 1992b, pers. obs.), so lengthening time aging and growth in these spiders. At very low between molts can have a detrimental effect on food levels, webs were small and few spiders fitness. To avoid reduction in weight during pe- gained weight but none lost weight. This food riods of low food availability, the interactions leveliswithintherangecapturedinPanama,but betweenforaginginvestmentandgrowthmaybe much lower than median prey capture rates ob- more complex than predicted by classical opti- served in Madang (4 prey/12 h diurnal obser- mal foraging models. & vation) (Higgins Buskirk 1992; Higgins, pers. obs.). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Nephila maculata juveniles varied greatly in their response to the experimental treatment. J. Wright’s comments and logistical support Such differences were not found in the experi- from the Smithsonian Tropical Research Insti- ment with N. clavipes, when the spiders were tute staffwere essential in the development of rearedunderhomogeneousconditions. Thisdif- this project. M. Townley and E. Tillinghastpro- ferencemightrepresentsomeintrinsicdifference videdvaluableinformationfortheinterpretation betweenspecies,butprobablyreflectsdifferences oftheexperimentandthey,R.Buskirk,W.Eber- in experience prior to the experiment. N. ma- hard, M. A. Rankin, C. Macias, G. Miller, C. culatajuveniles were collected in the field, and Pease, G. Perry, M. Singer, D. Wise and anon- undoubtedly differed in their foraging histories. ymous reviewers commented upon earlier ver- The foraginghistoryofan individual maycause sions of the manuscript. This work was sup- UT it to respond in a particular fashion to changes ported by Sigma-Xi, the Graduate Fellow- in prey capture rates. This impliesdifferencesin ship program, an NSF Dissertation Improve- the response to short vs. long term variation in mentGrant(BSR-8413831),aSTRIshort-term prey capture, which cannot be examined by the fellowship,andtheChristensenFundGrant.This short-term experiments presented here. is contribution No. 138 from the Christensen There is evidence that many spiders evolved Research Institute (P.O. Box 305, Madang, Pa- under conditions of food limitation (Anderson pua New Guinea). HIGGINS TRADE OFFS BETWEEN FORAGINGAND GROWTH 43 LITERATURECITED location among animals. Pp. 300-314, In Physio- Andtehresospni,deJr.sF.Lyco1s9a74l.entaReHsepnotnzseasntdoFsitlairsvtaattaiohnibi-n slooguirccael Eucsoelo(gTyo:wnAsnenedv,olCu.tiRo.na&ryPa.ppCraolaocwh, teodsr.)e.- ernalis(Hentz). Ecology, 55:576-585. PykBel,acGk.weHl.l,SHc.ieRn.tifPiucllPiraemss,,&BoEs.toL.n.Chamov. 1977. Busaknirdk,feeRd.inEg.in1a97t5r.opicCaolloonriba-liwteya,vianctgivsiptiydepra.tEtceorln-s Optimal foraging: A selectivereviewoftheoryand Chaogmyo,v5,6:E1.31L.4-1312987.6. Optimal foraging, the mar- Rietcehsetrs.t,QuSa.rEt..&ReJv..LuBicozla.k,.52:1193872-.154S.piderforaging: ginalvaluetheorem. Theor. Pop. Biol., 9:129-136. Behavioralresponsestoprey. Pp. 353-386, InSpi- Dadd, R. H. 1985. Nutrition: organisms. Compre- der Communication: Mechanisms and ecological hensive InsectPhysiology, Biochemistryand Phar- significance(P.N.Witt&J.S.Rovner,eds.).Prince- Ebemrahcaorldo,gyW.., VGo.l. 41,97p1p.. 31T3h-e39e0c.ology ofthe web of SchtooenneUrn,ivTe.rWsi.tyP1r9e7s1s., PrTihnecoertoyno.ffeedingstrategies. Uloborusdiversus(Araneae:Uloboridae).Oecologia Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 11:369-404. (Berlin), 6:328-342. Sherman, P. M. 1994. The orb-web: an energetic Enders, F. 1976. Effects ofprey capture, web de- and behavioural estimator of a spider’s dynamic struction and habitat physiogamy on web-site te- foragingandreproductivestrategies.Anim. Behav., 48:19-34. Gilnleascpiitey,oRf.AGr.giop1e98s7p.iderTsh.eJ.roAlreacohfnporl.e,ya3v:a7i5l-a8b2il.ity Sokal, R. R. & F. J. Rohlf. 1981. Biometry. The in aggregative behavior ofthe orb weaving spider Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Tetragnathaelongata. Anim. Behav., 35:675-681. Research,2nded.,W.H.Freeman&Co.,SanFran- Higgins, L. E. 1990. Variation in foraging invest- cisco. mentduringthe intermoiland before egglaying in Stephens,D.W.&J.R.Krebs. 1986. ForagingThe- thespiderNephilaclavipes.J.InsectBehavior,3:773- ory. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Tanaka, K. 1989. Energetic cost ofweb construc- 783. Higgins,L.E. 1992a. Developmentalplasticityand tion and its effect on web relocation in the web- fecundity in the orb-weaving spider Nephila cla- buildingspiderAgelenalimbata. Oecologia, 81:459- 464. vipes. J. Arachnol., 20:94-106. Higgins,L.E. 1992b. Developmentalchangesinthe Tillinghast,E.K. 1984. Thechemicalfractionation barrierwebunderdifferentlevelsofpredationrisk. oftheorbwebofArgiopespiders. InsectBiochem., HigJg.inIsn,secLt.BEe.hav1i9o9r3,.5:C6o3n5s-t6r5a5i.nts and plasticity in Tow1n4l:e1y1,5-M1.20A.., D. T. Bernstein,K. S. Gallagher&E. the development ofjuvenile Nephila clavipes in K. Tillinghast. 1991. Comparative study oforb Mexico. J. Arachnol., 21:107-119. webhygroscopicityandadhesivespiralcomposition Higgins, L. E. & R. Buskirk. 1992. Atrap-building inthreearaneidspiders.J.Exp.Zool.,259:154-165. predator exhibits different tactics for different as- Turnbull,A.L. 1964. Thesearchforpreybyaweb- pects offoragingbehavior. Anim. Behav., 44:485- buildingspider. Canadian Entomol., 96:568-597. 499. Wise, D. H. 1975. Food limitation of the spider Higgins, L. E. & M. A. Rankin. In press. Different Linyphia marginata: Experimental field studies. pathways in arthropod post-embryonic develop- Ecology, 56:637-646. ment. Evolution. Wise, D. H. 1979. Effects of an experimental in- Lubin,Y.D. 1973. Webstructureandfunction:the crease in prey abundance upon reproductive rates nonadhesive orb-web of Cyrtophora moluccensis oftwo orb-weaving spider species. Oecologia, 41: (Araneae:Araneidae). FormaetFunctio, 6:337-358. 289-300. Mangel, M. & C. Clark. 1986. Towards a unified foragingtheory. Ecology, 67:1127-1138. Manuscriptreceived16August1994,revised25October Pianka, E. R. 1981. Resource acquisition and al- 1994.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.